Next Article in Journal
Quality of Prenatal and Childhood Diet Predicts Neurodevelopmental Outcomes among Children in Mexico City
Previous Article in Journal
Exploration of the Association between Dietary Fiber Intake and Hypertension among U.S. Adults Using 2017 American College of Cardiology/American Heart Association Blood Pressure Guidelines: NHANES 2007–2014
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Impact of a Modified Version of Baby-Led Weaning on Dietary Variety and Food Preferences in Infants

by
Brittany J. Morison
1,
Anne-Louise M. Heath
1,
Jillian J. Haszard
1,
Karen Hein
2,
Elizabeth A. Fleming
1,
Lisa Daniels
1,
Elizabeth W. Erickson
1,
Louise J. Fangupo
1,
Benjamin J. Wheeler
3,
Barry J. Taylor
4 and
Rachael W. Taylor
5,*
1
Department of Human Nutrition, University of Otago, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand
2
Department of Food Science, University of Otago, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand
3
Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, University of Otago, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand
4
Office of the Dean, Dunedin School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand
5
Department of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2018, 10(8), 1092; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10081092
Submission received: 12 July 2018 / Revised: 6 August 2018 / Accepted: 13 August 2018 / Published: 15 August 2018

Abstract

:
The aim of this study was to determine whether food variety and perceived food preferences differ in infants following baby-led instead of traditional spoon-feeding approaches to introducing solids. A total of 206 women (41.3% primiparous) were recruited in late pregnancy from a single maternity hospital (response rate 23.4%) and randomized to Control (n = 101) or BLISS (n = 105) groups. All participants received government-funded Well Child care. BLISS participants also received support to exclusively breastfeed to 6 months and three educational sessions on BLISS (Baby-Led Weaning, modified to reduce the risk of iron deficiency, growth faltering, and choking) at 5.5, 7, and 9 months. Food variety was calculated from three-day weighed diet records at 7, 12, and 24 months. Questionnaires assessed infant preference for different tastes and textures at 12 months, and for ‘vegetables’, ‘fruit’, ‘meat and fish’, or ‘desserts’ at 24 months. At 24 months, 50.5% of participants provided diet record data, and 78.2% provided food preference data. BLISS participants had greater variety in ‘core’ (difference in counts over three days, 95% CI: 1.3, 0.4 to 2.2), ‘non-core’ (0.6, 0.2 to 0.9), and ‘meat and other protein’ (1.3, 0.8 to 1.9) foods at 7 months, and in ‘fruit and vegetable’ foods at 24 months (2, 0.4 to 3.6). The only differences in perceived food preferences observed were very small (i.e., <5% difference in score, at 12 months only). Infants following the modified Baby-Led Weaning were exposed to more varied and textured foods from an early age, but only an increased variety in ‘fruit and vegetable’ intake was apparent by two years of age.

1. Introduction

Parents have traditionally introduced their infant to complementary foods by spoon-feeding them puréed foods. As the infant learns to manage solid foods, a wider variety of foods with increasingly complex textures are offered [1]. Although the progression may include some finger foods, these do not usually represent a significant component of the diet until later in complementary feeding [2,3,4,5,6]. This parent-led approach to complementary feeding differs substantially from Baby-Led Weaning (BLW), which advocates bypassing purées and instead recommends that the infant self-feeds all their foods in a solid form from the start of weaning [7,8,9,10]. Advocates of BLW suggest this approach leads to healthier food preferences due to the infant being exposed to greater food variety and role modeling through ‘joining in’ at the family meal [11,12,13].
No research appears to have investigated whether baby-led approaches influence food variety, even though different first foods are typically offered [14,15,16,17]. Learning to like ‘healthy’ foods is modifiable [18,19], and infants who are repeatedly exposed to foods varying in taste and texture are more willing to eat those foods and other novel foods later in childhood [20,21]. Although advocates claim that BLW infants prefer a wider range of foods [7], this has only been examined in one small study, where significant differences were seen for just one of eight food groups examined [22].
We recently completed a randomized clinical trial (RCT) determining whether growth differed in infants following baby-led versus parent-led approaches to complementary feeding (Baby-Led Introduction to SolidS [BLISS] study) [23]. This analysis of preplanned secondary outcomes focuses on potential differences in food variety and preferences from 7 to 24 months of age.

2. Materials and Methods

BLISS compared modified BLW with usual care (traditional spoon-feeding; Control). BLISS followed the general principles of BLW (infants feed themselves handheld foods and are involved in family meal times) but was modified in response to concerns that BLW may increase the risk of iron deficiency, choking, and growth faltering [24]. As protocol [25] and primary outcome [23] papers have been published, only relevant information is provided here. The study was approved by the Lower South Regional Ethics Committee (LRS/11/09/037), and written informed consent was obtained from all adult participants before randomization.
Women were recruited in late pregnancy from sequential bookings (December 2012–March 2014) at the only maternity hospital in Dunedin, New Zealand (>97% of all births). Exclusion criteria were applied in late pregnancy (not living locally, mother ≤16 years, booked after 34 weeks gestation) and after birth (prematurity, congenital abnormality likely to affect feeding/growth), producing a final sample size of 206 participants (Figure 1). The participants were randomly allocated to Control (n = 101) or BLISS (n = 105) groups, using random length blocks after stratification for parity (first child, subsequent child) and maternal education (non-tertiary, tertiary) by the study biostatistician.
Both control and intervention children received Well Child care, a nationally funded health program from antenatal until 5 years of age [26,27]. Families in the BLISS group received eight additional contacts. Five contacts (antenatal to 5 months of age) were delivered by an International Board Certified Lactation Consultant (IBCLC) to assist prolonging exclusive breastfeeding and delaying the introduction of complementary foods until 6 months of age. A trained BLISS researcher met individually with each family at 5.5, 7, and 9 months of age to provide advice and support about the BLISS approach to complementary feeding. The essential characteristics are: 1) offer foods that the infant can pick up and feed themselves, 2) offer one high-iron food at every meal, 3) offer one high-energy food at each meal, and 4) offer foods prepared in a way that is suitable for the infant’s developmental age to reduce the risk of choking and avoid offering foods listed as high-choking-risk foods [25]. A variety of pretested [28] resources provided ideas for adapting family meals following a baby-led philosophy, while promoting responsive feeding practices. Parents were encouraged to offer a variety of healthy foods to their infants, wherever possible offering foods from the family meal.
Demographic information was obtained at baseline (late pregnancy) from hospital records (birth weight, infant sex, parity, level of household deprivation [29]) and questionnaire (maternal education, employment status, ethnicity, self-reported pre-pregnancy height and weight), after the mothers had provided consent but before randomization to intervention group.
Three-day weighed diet records were collected on randomly assigned non-consecutive days (one weekend day, two week days) over a three-week period at 7, 12, and 24 months of age to assess food variety. When relevant, the parents were asked to record the weight of infant formula powder, added water, and total prepared weight of the formula offered. Estimated total daily volumes were used for breast milk (750 g/day at 7 months, 448 g/day at 12 months [30], 59 g/breastfeed at 24 month [31]), with the reported volume of prepared formula subtracted from these amounts as appropriate for mixed-fed infants. The participants received written and oral instructions for completing the records [25].
Food variety scores were calculated for four food groups (‘core foods’, ‘non-core foods’, ‘meat and other protein’, and ‘fruit and vegetables’) on the basis of the method of Scott et al [32]. Diet records were only included if all three days had been completed. Individual foods were assigned to food groups (Table 1). Each individual food was only counted once over the three days, regardless of how many times that particular food had been offered. Foods that were similar (e.g., different cultivars of apples) were only counted once, but different forms of the food were counted as individual items (e.g., raw apple and stewed apple would provide two counts). Mixed food dishes and commercial baby foods were broken down into their component parts (e.g., a steak and cheese pie would provide three counts: ‘meat and other protein’ (steak), ‘non-core’ foods (pastry), and ‘core’ foods (cheese)). However, the mixed dish or baby food itself only provided one point for the total variety score. For each food or beverage item, the parents also indicated whether the item was puréed, mashed, diced, naturally smooth, whole, or liquid (Table 2).
Food preferences at 12 months of age were assessed by asking the parents to indicate whether their toddler had ever been offered (never offered, 1–3 times, 4–6 times, 7–10 times, 11 or more times, don’t know) and subsequently consumed (‘yes, they always eat it’, ‘yes, they sometimes eat it’, ‘yes, but they rarely eat it’, ‘no, they reject it after tasting’, and ‘no, they refuse to taste it’), each of 21 foods. The 21 foods were chosen from those most commonly consumed by toddlers in New Zealand [33] and assigned to six taste categories (‘sweet’, ‘salty’, ‘savory-meat’, ‘savory-non-meat high-protein’, ‘savory-vegetable’, and ‘savory-French fries’) and four texture categories (‘smooth’, ‘lumpy’, ‘chewy’, ‘crunchy’). This taste and texture allocation was decided after asking a separate group of 14 parents with age-appropriate children to assign each food to what they considered was the most appropriate category, with final classification established where there was at least 50% agreement. Twenty of the 21 foods were consistently assigned to a certain taste category; the only food not assigned was peanut butter, for which 43% of parents indicated a savory taste and 36% a salty taste. Parents were in agreement regarding the texture of 16 of these foods, with five foods not consistently assigned to a specific texture category (olives, tomato, baloney, cheese, French fries). Thus, 20 foods were included in analyses for taste, and 16 foods for texture.
At 24 months of age, food preference was assessed using an existing questionnaire that included single foods (e.g., banana), groups of similar foods (e.g., savory snacks including potato chips and crackers), and mixed dishes (e.g., lasagna) [34]. Minor modifications were made to the questionnaire to make it appropriate for New Zealand families (e.g., ‘salad greens’ was replaced with ‘lettuce’).
The mothers indicated how much the child liked each of these foods using one of six response options: ‘dislikes a lot’, ‘dislikes a little’, ‘neither dislikes nor likes’, ‘likes a little’, ‘likes a lot’, and ‘hasn’t tried it’. Because of the relatively small size of our sample, we used the factors of Wardle et al. [34] to create preference scores for ‘vegetables’ (n = 9 items), ‘fruit’ (n = 9 items), ‘meat and fish’ (n = 8 items), and ‘desserts’ (n = 6 items). The scores were obtained as the mean liking on the response scale from 1 (dislikes a lot) to 5 (likes a lot) of all items within each factor. The missing items were imputed with the mean of the remaining items in the scale.

Statistics

The primary aim of our RCT was to determine whether BLISS improves the body mass index (BMI) at 12 months of age. Sample size calculations were based on detecting a difference in BMI of 0.4. Sample size calculations were not undertaken in relation to dietary variety or food preferences, as these were secondary outcomes of the main trial (i.e., part of our dietary quality measures) [25]. Differences between BLISS and Control groups are therefore presented with 95% CI for interpretation without P-values, after adjustment for maternal education and parity (stratification variables) and infant sex. Statistical analysis was carried out using Stata 14.2 (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX, USA). The demographic differences between participants who provided dietary data at 12 or 24 months and those who did not were compared using t-tests for continuous variables and chi-squared tests for categorical variables.
Linear regression was used to determine the mean difference in food variety scores (counts) between BLISS and Control groups at 7, 12, and 24 months and the mean difference in exposure and preference scores for different tastes and textures at 12 months. Residuals were plotted and visually assessed for homogeneity of variance and normality.
Due to skewed data, group medians (25th, 75th percentiles) are presented for mean daily proportions of forms in which food was consumed at 7, 12, and 24 months and for food preference scores at 24 months using the factors of Wardle et al. [34]. Differences between groups were assessed using bootstrapped median regression with 100 replications.

3. Results

3.1. Participants

Control and BLISS participants were similar at baseline (Table 3). Almost half of all mothers were university-educated (48.5%), and fewer participants came from households with higher levels of deprivation (21.4%) than would be expected nationally (30%). Participant retention was high (Figure 1): 166 participants (80.6%) remained at two years of age. While virtually all participants provided food preference data (94%–97%), fully completed three-day diet records were provided by a lower percentage of participants (76.7% at 7 months, 70.7% at 12 months, 62.7% at 24 months). Families who provided any data for analyses at 12 months (n = 174) did not differ from those who did not provide data (n = 32) in terms of maternal BMI (p = 0.338), maternal education (p = 0.211), household deprivation (p = 0.795), or group (p = 0.203), although participating mothers were older than non-participants (31.9 versus 27.8 years, p < 0.001).
As reported previously [23], BLISS infants were exclusively breastfed for longer than Control infants, and more BLISS infants met the World Health Organization guideline to delay solids to 6 months than Control infants. However, there were no significant group differences in the estimated intake of breast milk (all p ≥ 0.940) or infant formula (all p ≥ 0.170) at any time point, nor in the proportion of infants who were introduced to solids earlier than 4 months (5.2% in BLISS versus 12.8% in Controls, p = 0.057). BLISS children consumed a greater proportion of their food as ‘whole’ foods at each age, particularly at 7 months (31% of foods compared with 8% in Controls, p < 0.001, Table 2).

3.2. Food Variety

At 7 months of age, BLISS participants had greater total food variety (difference, 95% CI: 3.0, 1.1 to 4.8, Table 4), consisting of greater variety in the intake of ‘core foods’ (1.3, 0.4 to 2.2), ‘non-core foods’ (0.6, 0.2 to 0.9), and ‘meat and other protein’ (1.3, 0.8 to 1.9), with no difference in the ‘fruit and vegetable’ variety (−1.1, −2.4 to 0.2). By 24 months of age, the only significant difference between groups was a higher ‘fruit and vegetable’ variety count in BLISS compared with Control children (2.0, 0.4 to 3.6).

3.3. Food Preferences

At 12 months of age, food preferences were assessed for different tastes (Table 5) and textures (Table 6). Children in both groups had been offered each taste 4.8 (‘salty’) to 7.4 (‘savory-meat’ and ‘savory-non-meat high-protein’) times on average by 12 months (Table 3). Although BLISS participants were offered ‘savory-vegetables’ (difference in number of times, 95% CI: 0.8, 0.01 to 1.5) and ‘savory-non-meat high-protein foods’ (1.3, 0.6 to 2.1) more often than Control participants, a difference in perceived preference (i.e. foods actually consumed) was only apparent for ‘savory-non-meat high-protein’ foods and was small (0.2, 0.04 to 0.4; i.e., 4.7%, 0.9% to 9.3% of Control value). Similarly, in terms of texture, BLISS infants were more likely to be offered (1.1, 0.4 to 1.7) lumpy foods but only slightly more likely to consume them (0.2, 0.01 to 0.3; i.e., 4.8%, 0.2% to 7.1% of Control value) (Table 6). At 24 months of age (Table 7), overall preference scores were high for ‘fruit’ and moderately high for ‘meat and fish’ and ‘desserts’, with slightly lower values for ‘vegetables’. However, no significant differences in these preference scores were apparent between groups at this age.
Because group differences in the duration of exclusive breastfeeding and timing of the introduction of complementary foods could explain some of the observed differences in food exposures and preferences, post-hoc mediation analyses [35] were run for all statistically significant results, with duration of exclusive breastfeeding or age of introduction of complementary foods as mediators. Neither variable was a mediator for any of the relationships found. In fact, the adjustment for age of introduction of complementary foods strengthened the differences seen between the groups for variety of ‘core foods’ and ‘non-core foods’ and amount of liquids at 7 months of age; amount of whole food and preferences for ‘savory-non-meat high-protein’ and lumpy foods at 12 months of age; amount of whole food and liquids at 24 months, although all differences were small and did not influence the interpretation of the findings reported above.

4. Discussion

Our data demonstrate that a baby-led approach to complementary feeding leads to increased dietary variety and greater exposure to more textured foods at a young age. Infants following BLISS had greater variety in the intake of ‘core foods’, ‘non-core foods’, and ‘meat and other protein sources’ at 7 months and in the ‘fruit and vegetable’ variety at 24 months. By 12 months of age, they also had increased preference for ‘savory-non-meat high-protein’ foods and were more likely to be offered, and consume, lumpy foods. However, these differences were very small and appeared to be transitory, in that no differences in preference for ‘vegetables’, ‘fruit’, ‘meat and fish’, or ‘desserts’ (or the foods classified as ‘savory-non-meat high-protein’ and ‘lumpy’ in the analysis at 12 months) were apparent by 24 months of age.
No previous studies appear to have examined whether the dietary variety differs in infants following baby-led rather than parent-led approaches to complementary feeding, despite the expectation that opportunities to eat a more adult-type diet with the family would lead to a wider variety of foods being consumed [11,12,13]. There are several potential reasons why the dietary variety might be increased with a baby-led approach to feeding. Breastfeeding exposes infants to a wider variety of tastes than occurs with formula feeding [18,36], and BLISS infants were exclusively breastfed for several weeks longer than Control infants [23]. In addition, others [37] have shown that the early introduction of complementary foods may be detrimental to food variety; young children who had been introduced to complementary foods before 6 months of age were 2.5 times more likely (95% CI: 1.1–5.7) to consume a limited variety of foods than those who had been introduced to solids after 6 months. In our sample, BLISS infants were on average introduced to complementary foods several weeks later than Control infants, and a significantly greater proportion did not introduce solids until 6 months of age or later (64.6% of BLISS infants compared with 18.1% of Control infants) [23]. However, mediation analysis showed that the differences in food variety between the groups were not explained by either duration of exclusive breastfeeding or age when complementary feeding started. It is likely, therefore, that it was the baby-led approach to complementary feeding itself that was responsible for the greater dietary variety in the BLISS infants at 7 months. Group differences in dietary variety at 7 months of age were no longer apparent by 12 months. Interestingly, although no statistically significant differences in the ‘fruit and vegetable’ variety were observed in infancy, BLISS children consumed a wider variety of fruit and vegetables at two years of age, suggesting some benefit to dietary variety over the longer-term. It would be useful to determine whether this increased variety continues to older ages, given the health benefits of consuming a wide variety of fruit and vegetables [38].
As with food variety, limited research has examined food preferences in baby-led compared to parent-fed infants. Townsend et al. [22] reported that of eight different food groups examined, differences in preference were only apparent for one: baby-led infants were more likely to prefer ‘carbohydrate’ foods, despite lower exposure to these foods than spoon-fed infants. Repeated exposure to a food is one of the primary determinants of its acceptance [20], and the child must be allowed to experience the flavor and taste of the fruit or vegetable to learn to like it [39], perhaps as many as ten times [40]. Our data demonstrate that BLISS infants were offered vegetables more often than Control infants at 12 months of age, but that this was not reflected in greater consumption. However, our infants were only exposed to the vegetables we investigated around six times on average, which might explain this lack of effect. Texture is also important. Letting young children experience more varied textures is associated with fewer food refusals [41] and a better acceptance of weaning foods later on [42]. However, although children in the BLISS study were offered lumpy foods more often and showed a greater preference for these foods at 12 months of age, a preference for the same foods was not apparent at 24 months of age, suggesting little effect on long-term healthy food preferences. These findings are consistent with the observation that food fussiness was significantly lower in BLISS infants than Control infants at 12 months but not at 24 months [23].
The strengths of our study include the randomized trial design, the high level of adherence to the intervention, and the repeated measurement of dietary variety and food preferences using a range of tools. The use of a randomized trial design overcomes the known differences in demographics and parental feeding practices that exist in those who choose to follow BLW rather than traditional feeding practices [14,43], and that could conceivably influence the development of food preferences. Infants who had been randomized to the BLISS intervention group were significantly and substantially more likely to follow a baby-led approach to infant feeding than infants in the Control group [23], so we can be confident that the null findings are not due to poor adherence to the intervention. We also determined whether group differences in breastfeeding or the age of solids introduction might have influenced our findings, given their known effect on variety and development of food preferences in young children [36]. However, adjustment for these variables had little effect on the group differences observed.
Our study also has some limitations. This was an analysis of secondary outcomes of interest from a wider RCT. Although study retention was 81% overall at two years of age, only 78–84% of parents completed the questionnaires on food preferences, and 50–72% provided three complete days of weighed diet record data for food variety assessment. However, no differences in a range of demographic variables (except maternal age) were observed between the families who did and those who did not provide data. Also, dietary variety was estimated at each time point using only three days of diet records, which may be insufficient, given that the accurate assessment of variety in children may need as much as two weeks [44]. However, our diet records were collected on non-consecutive days, which improves the accuracy of dietary variety estimations when compared with consecutive recording days [44]. At 12 months of age, we only assessed taste and texture preferences using a relatively limited range of foods. However, this food list was developed from a larger pool of foods, and a separate validation study was undertaken to ensure consistency in parental interpretation of the tastes and textures represented by the foods. Finally, our two groups differed in contact time, which could potentially affect the outcomes.

5. Conclusions

Our findings demonstrate that a baby-led approach to complementary feeding increases dietary variety and exposure to more textured foods at a young age. By two years of age, the only difference observed was a higher variety in ‘fruit and vegetable’ intake. In contrast, any impact on perceived food preferences appeared to be only transitory.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, B.J.M., A.-L.M.H., K.H., B.J.W., B.J.T., and R.W.T.; Methodology, B.J.M., A.-L.M.H., K.H., and R.W.T.; Formal analysis, J.J.H.; Investigation, B.J.M., E.A.F., L.D., E.W.E., and L.J.F.; Resources, B.J.M., A.-L.M.HK.H., E.A.F., and R.W.T.; Data curation, R.W.T.; Writing-Original draft preparation, B.J.M. and R.W.T.; Writing-Review & Editing, B.J.M., A.-L.M.H., J.J.H., K.H., E.A.F., L.D., E.W.E., L.J.F., B.J.W., B.J.T., and R.W.T.; Validation, J.J.H. and R.W.T.; Visualization, R.W.T.; Supervision, A.-L.M.H., K.H., and R.W.T.; Project administration, E.A.F.; Funding acquisition, A.-L.M.H., B.J.W., B.J.T., and R.W.T.

Funding

The BLISS Study was funded by Lottery Health Research, Meat & Livestock Australia, Karitane Products Society, Perpetual Trustees, The New Zealand Federation of Women’s Institutes, and the University of Otago, with ‘in kind’ contributions from Heinz Watties Ltd. Taylor is supported by a Fellowship from Karitane Products Society. Daniels was in receipt of a University of Otago Doctoral Scholarship, and Erickson was in receipt of a University of Otago Masters Scholarship. The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all participants and families.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. McAndrew, F.; Thompson, J.; Fellows, L.; Large, A.; Speed, M.; Renfrew, M.J. Infant Feeding Survey 2010; Health and Social Care Information Centre: London, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  2. Infant Feeding and Nutrition: Complementary Foods. Available online: https://wicworks.fns.usda.gov/wicworks/Topics/FG/Chapter5_ComplementaryFoods.pdf (accessed on 20 March 2018).
  3. Food and Nutrition Guidelines for Healthy Eating for Infants and Toddlers (Aged 0–2): A Background Paper. Available online: https://www.health.govt.nz/system/files/documents/publications/food-and-nutrition-guidelines-healthy-infants-and-toddlers-revised-dec12.pdf (accessed on 20 March 2018).
  4. National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC). Infant Feeding Guidelines: Information for Health Workers; Department of Health and Ageing: Canberra, Australia, 2012; pp. 1–160.
  5. Guiding Principles of Complementary Feeding of the Breastfed Child. Available online: http://www.who.int/nutrition/publications/guiding_principles_compfeeding_breastfed.pdf (accessed on 20 March 2018).
  6. Guiding Principles for Feeding Non-Breastfed Children 6–24 Months of Age. Available online: http://www.who.int/maternal_child_adolescent/documents/9241593431/en/ (accessed on 20 March 2018).
  7. Rapley, G.; Murkett, T. Baby-Led Weaning; Vermilion: Londo, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  8. Gibson-Moore, H. New perspectives of first foods, complementary feeding and obesity. Nutr. Bull. 2015, 40, 349–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Brown, A.; Lee, M.D. Early influences on child satiety-responsiveness: The role of weaning style. Pediatr. Obes. 2015, 10, 57–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Cichero, J.A.Y. Introducing solid foods using baby-led weaning vs. spoon-feeding: A focus on oral development, nutrient intake and quality of research to bring balance back to the debate. Nutr. Bull. 2016, 41, 72–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Rapley, G. Baby-led weaning: Transitioning to solid foods at the baby’s own pace. Community Pract. 2011, 84, 20–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Rapley, G. Baby-led weaning: The theory and evidence behind the approach. J. Health Visit 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Cameron, S.L.; Heath, A.-L.M.; Taylor, R.W. How feasible is baby-led weaning as an approach to infant feeding? A review of the evidence. Nutrients 2012, 4, 1575–1609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Brown, A.; Lee, M. A descriptive study investigating the use and nature of baby-led weaning in a UK sample of mothers. Matern. Child Nutr. 2011, 7, 34–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Wright, C.M.; Cameron, K.; Tsiaka, M.; Parkinson, K.N. Is baby-led weaning feasible? When do babies first reach out for and eat finger food? Matern. Child Nutr. 2011, 7, 27–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Rowan, H.; Harris, C. Baby-led weaning and the family diet. A. pilot study. Appetite 2012, 58, 1046–1049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. D’Andrea, E.; Jenkins, K.; Mathews, M.; Roebothan, B. Baby-led weaning: A preliminary investigation. Can. J. Diet. Pract. Res. 2016, 77, 72–77. [Google Scholar]
  18. Mennella, J.A. Ontogeny of taste preferences: Basic biology and implications for health. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 99, 704S–711S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Barends, C.; de Vries, J.H.M.; Mojet, J.; de Graaf, C. Effects of weaning exlusively with vegetables on vegetable intake at the age of 12 and 23 months. Appetite 2014, 81, 193–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Nicklaus, S. Children’s acceptance of new foods at weaning. Role of practices of weaning and of food sensory properties. Appetite 2011, 57, 812–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Ventura, A.K.; Worobey, J. Early influences on the development of food preferences. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, R401–R408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Townsend, E.; Pitchford, N.J. Baby knows best? The impact of weaning style in food preferences and body mass index in early childhood in a case-controlled sample. BMJ Open 2012, 2, e000298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Taylor, R.W.; Williams, S.M.; Fangupo, L.J.; Wheeler, B.J.; Taylor, B.J.; Daniels, L.; Fleming, L.A.; McArthur, J.; Morison, B.; Erickson, L.W.; et al. Baby-led and conventional approaches to complementary feeding for obesity prevention: A randomised controlled trial. Jama Pediatr. 2017, 171, 838–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Cameron, S.L.; Heath, A.-L.M.; Taylor, R.W. Healthcare professionals’ and mothers’ knowledge of, attitudes to, and experiences with, baby-led weaning: A content analysis study. BMJ Open 2012, 2, e001542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Daniels, L.; Heath, A.-L.H.; Williams, S.M.; Cameron, S.L.; Fleming, E.A.; Taylor, B.J.; Wheeler, B.J.; Gibson, R.S.; Taylor, R.W. Baby-Led Introduction to SolidS (BLISS) study: A randomised controlled trial of a baby-led approach to complementary feeding. BMJ Open 2015, 15, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. About Lead Maternity Carer (LMC) Services. Available online: https://www.midwife.org.nz/women-in-new-zealand/about-lead-maternity-carer-lmc-services (accessed on 25 June 2018).
  27. Well Child Tamariki Ora Visits. Available online: http://www.health.govt.nz/your-health/services-and-support/health-care-services/well-child-tamariki-ora (accessed on 25 June 2018).
  28. Cameron, S.L.; Heath, A.-L.M.; Taylor, R.W. Development and pilot testing of baby-led introduction to solids-a version of baby-led weaning modified to address concerns about iron deficiency, growth faltering and choking. BMJ Open 2015, 15, 99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. NZDep2013 Index of Deprivation. Available online: https://www.health.govt.nz/publication/nzdep2013-index-deprivation (accessed on 25 June 2018).
  30. Dewey, K.G.; Heinig, M.J.; Nommsen, L.A.; Lonnerdal, B. Adequacy of energy intake among breast-fed infants in the DARLING study: Relationships to growth velocity, morbidity, and activity levels. Davis Area Research on Lactation, Infant Nutrition and Growth. J. Pediatr. 1991, 119, 538–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Briefel, R.R.; Kalb, L.M.; Condon, E.; Deming, D.M.; Clusen, N.A.; Fox, M.K.; Harnack, L.; Gemmill, E.; Stevens, M.; Reidy, K.C. The Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study 2008: Study design and methods. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2010, 110, S16–S26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Scott, J.A.; Chih, T.Y.; Oddy, W.H. Food variety at 2 years of age is related to duration of breastfeeding. Nutrients 2012, 4, 1464–1474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Szymlek-Gay, E.A.; Ferguson, E.L.; Heath, A.-L.M.; Fleming, E.A. Quantities of foods consumed by 12- to 24-month old New Zealand children. Nutr. Diet. 2010, 67, 244–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Wardle, J.; Sanderson, S.; Gibson, E.L.; Rapoport, L. Factor-analytic structure of food preferences in four-year-old children in the UK. Appetite 2001, 37, 217–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Baron, R.M.; Kenny, D.A. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Person Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 1173–1182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Dunn, R.L.; Lessen, R. The influence of human milk on flavor and food preferences. Curr. Nutr. Rep. 2017, 6, 134–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Shim, J.E.; Kim, J.; Mathai, R.A.; the Strong Kids Research Team. Associations of infant feeding practices and picky eating behaviors of preschool children. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2011, 111, 1363–1368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Liu, R.H. Dietary bioactive compounds and their health implications. J. Food Sci. 2013, 78, A18–A25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Mennella, J.A.; Reiter, A.R.; Daniels, L.M. Vegetable and fruit acceptance during infancy: Impact on ontogeny, genetics, and early experiences. Adv. Nutr. 2016, 7, 211S–219S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Birch, L.L.; Gunder, L.; Grimm-Thomas, K.; Laing, D.G. Infants’ consumption of a new food enhances acceptance of similar foods. Appetite 1998, 30, 283–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Werthman, J.; Jansen, A.; Havermans, R.; Nederkoorn, C.; Kremers, S.; Roefs, A. Bits and pieces. Food texture influences food acceptance in young children. Appetite 2015, 84, 181–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Northstone, K.; Emmett, P.; Nethersole, F. Avon Longitudinal Study of Pregnancy and Childhood: The effect of age of introduction to lumpy solids on foods eaten and reported feeding difficulties at 6 and 15 months. J. Hum. Nutr. Diet. 2001, 14, 43–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Brown, A.; Lee, M.D. Maternal control during the weaning period: Differences between mothers following a baby-led or standard weaning approach. Matern. Child Health 2011, 15, 1265–1271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Falciglia, G.A.; Horner, S.L.; Liang, J.; Couch, S.C.; Levin, L.S. Assessing dietary variety in children: Development and validartion of a predictive equation. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2009, 109, 641–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flow of participants through the study.
Figure 1. Flow of participants through the study.
Nutrients 10 01092 g001
Table 1. Foods included in each food variety grouping.
Table 1. Foods included in each food variety grouping.
Core foods (No Maximum Count)
Dairy products
  Milk alternative 1
  Any cheese
  Sweetened yoghurt 2
  Unsweetened yoghurt 2
  Custard
  Low-fat dairy 3
  High-fat dairy 4
  Ice cream
  Milk not as a drink 5
Grains
  Individual breakfast cereals counted separately
  Baby rice cereal
  Breads-white
  Breads-whole meal or wholegrain
  Breads-novelty 6
  Rice
  Pasta
  Crackers
  Cereal bars
  Baby rusks
  Other grain products
Milk
  Breast milk
  Infant formula
  Cow’s milk as a drink
Non-core foods (no maximum count)
Savoury
  Pies
  Burgers
  Battered fish
  Pizza
  Fried chicken
  Fried potatoes 7
  Pastries 8
  Dips 9
  Savoury muffins or scones
  Croissants
  Potato chips
  Popcorn
  Corn chips
  Other salty snacks
  Other takeaway foods
Sweet
  Cakes or slices
  Muffins or fruit loaves
  Sweet scones or pancakes
  Sweet pastries
  Biscuits 10
  Individual ‘other desserts’ counted separately
  Candy
  Chocolate
  Frozen ice block 11
Other drinks
  Soda
  Fruit-flavored drink
  Flavored milk drink
  Fruit juice
  Tea
  Milo, chocolate, and malt drink
  Coffee
Meat and other protein (maximum = 16)
Eggs
Peanut butter
Nuts or seeds
Baked beans
Hummus
Legumes
Vegetarian meat substitutes
Beef
Lamb
Pork
Venison
Chicken or turkey
Fish or shellfish
Sausages
Processed meats or cold cuts 12
Offal and other unspecified meats
Pâté
Fruit and vegetables (no maximum count)
Individual fruits 13 and vegetables 14 counted separately
1 soy, almond, rice, bran, oat milks; 2 includes soy; 3 cottage cheese, low-fat cheeses; 4 cream, sour cream, cream cheese; 5 milk or milk substitutes were counted as dairy produce if they were added to cooking or cereals; 6 fruit, nut, seed, vegetable bread; 7 French fries, hash browns, fritters; 8 sausage rolls, savories; 9 excludes hummus; 10 includes semi-sweet biscuits; 11 includes sorbet; 12 includes bacon and ham; 13 includes avocado; 14 includes mushrooms, excludes French fries.
Table 2. Form of food 1 consumed by the participants at each time point (from three-day weighed diet records).
Table 2. Form of food 1 consumed by the participants at each time point (from three-day weighed diet records).
7 Months12 Months24 Months
Control
(n = 77)
BLISS
(n = 85)
p8Control
(n = 69)
BLISS
(n = 75)
p8Control
(n = 56)
BLISS
(n = 57)
p8
Puréed 28.8 (2.6, 18.5)3.7 (0, 9.0)0.0090 (0, 3.6)0 (0, 3.1)>0.9990 (0, 0)0 (0, 0)>0.999
Mashed 36.1 (1.3, 13.5)3.1 (0, 7.5)0.0542.2 (0, 9.6)0.8 (0, 3.1)0.2350 (0, 1.7)0 (0, 1.4)>0.999
Diced 40 (0, 2.2)0 (0, 2.1)>0.9992.8 (0, 5.6)0 (0, 5.1)0.0841.1 (0, 5.9)1.1 (0, 6.3)0.927
Smooth 53.7 (0, 8.6)7.5 (3.7, 11.9)0.2095.2 (1.4, 9.7)4.7 (1.7, 8.8)0.4228.6 (3.7, 16.0)8.8 (4.2, 12.8)0.543
Whole 67.9 (2.6, 21.1)31.0 (20.5, 36.4)<0.00120.8 (13.2, 31.7)28.2 (19.2, 34.6)0.02537.7 (29.6, 49.2)49.7 (37.7, 62.5)<0.001
Liquid 754.0 (43.8, 65.9)44.9 (36.6, 55.3)0.00857.1 (47.4, 66.4)57.4 (49.6, 66.6)0.63136.6 (23.0, 51.3)32.8 (20.7, 42.4)0.008
1 Data presented as median (25th, 75th percentile) of the percentage of daily food and drink intake by weight (excluding drinking water). 2 Food blended together to make a smooth consistency, 3 mashed by hand to make a lumpy consistency, 4 chopped into small pieces and needing a spoon to eat it, 5 originally smooth in texture with no modification, 6 pieces that are large enough to easily handle and eat by hand, 7 breast milk, infant formula, cow’s milk, juice; 8 median regression bootstrapped with 100 replications, adjusting for maternal education and parity and infant sex. Number of missing responses for form of food at: 7 months, 171/6709 food items (2.5%); 12 months, 28/6673 food items (0.4%); 24 months, 221/5362 (4.1%). These food items were included in the calculation of participant’s total weight of food intake but not assigned to a form of food. BLISS: Baby-Led Introduction to SolidS.
Table 3. Characteristics of the study population at baseline.
Table 3. Characteristics of the study population at baseline.
CategoryControl (n = 101)BLISS (n = 105)
n (%)n (%)
Maternal variables
Age (years) 1Mean (SD)31.3 (6.2)31.3 (5.0)
Pre-pregnancy BMI 2Mean (SD)25.6 (5.6)25.9 (6.3)
EducationSchool only29 (28.7)34 (32.4)
Post-secondary19 (18.8)24 (22.9)
University53 (52.5)47 (44.8)
ParityFirst child42 (41.6)43 (41.0)
Subsequent child59 (59.4)62 (59.0)
EthnicityNZEO85 (84.2)83 (79.0)
Māori or Pacific10 (9.9)15 (14.1)
Asian6 (5.9)7 (6.7)
Household variables
Household deprivation 31–3 (Low)29 (28.7)31 (29.5)
4–749 (48.5)53 (50.5)
8–10 (High)23 (22.8)21 (20.0)
Infant variables
Birth weight (g) 4Mean (SD)3531 (486)3509 (451)
Sex 5Male53 (52.5)43 (41.0)
Female47 (47.5)62 (59.0)
Data expressed as n (%) except where indicated; Data missing for 1 1, 7 2, 3 4, and 1 5 participants; NZEO refers to New Zealand European and Others; BMI: body mass index. 3 Uses the New Zealand Index of Deprivation 2013 which combines nine variables from the 2013 census relating to communication (no access to the internet at home), income (receiving a means tested benefit or living below income thresholds), unemployment, qualifications, home ownership, single parent families, living space, and transport access. A deprivation score is provided for each meshblock which is a geographical unit defined by Statistics New Zealand containing about 60–10 people. The score reflects the extent of material and social deprivation and is used to construct defiles from 1 (least deprived) to 10 (most deprived).
Table 4. Food variety (counts) at 7, 12, and 24 months of age over the three days of the three-day weighed diet record.
Table 4. Food variety (counts) at 7, 12, and 24 months of age over the three days of the three-day weighed diet record.
7 months12 months24 months
Food group 1Control
(n = 72)
BLISS
(n = 76)
Difference 2
(95% CI)
Control
(n = 61)
BLISS
(n = 69)
Difference 2
(95% CI)
Control
(n = 53)
BLISS
(n = 51)
Difference 2
(95% CI)
Core foods5.8 (2.9)7.1 (2.4)1.3 (0.4, 2.2)9.4 (2.6)9.3 (2.5)0.0 (−0.9, 0.9)9.8 (2.4)9.7 (2.2)0.1 (−0.7, 0.9)
Non-core foods0.5 (1.0)1.1 (1.1)0.6 (0.2, 0.9)2.3 (1.7)2.2 (1.7)−0.1 (−0.7, 0.5)4.9 (2.7)4.0 (2.6)−0.7 (−1.8, 0.3)
Meat & other protein1.9 (1.7)3.2 (1.8)1.3 (0.8, 1.9)3.6 (1.6)4.0 (1.7)0.5 (−0.02, 1.1)4.0 (1.6)4.4 (1.8)0.4 (−0.2, 1.1)
Fruit and vegetables9.2 (4.1)8.1 (3.7)−1.1 (−2.4, 0.2)11.9 (4.4)11.1 (5.1)−0.6 (−2.2, 1.1)9.8 (4.2)11.5 (4.1)2.0 (0.4, 3.6)
Total food variety13.1 (6.1)15.9 (5.4)3.0 (1.1, 4.8)21.4 (5.8)21.6 (6.0)0.4 (−1.6 2.4)24.6 (6.4)25.3 (6.8)1.3 (−1.1, 3.7)
Data presented as mean (SD). 1 Broad categories of foods were: ‘core foods’ (dairy, grains, milk), ‘non-core foods’ (savory and sweet snacks, drinks), ‘meat and other protein’, and ‘fruit and vegetables’. See Table 1 for food and drink items within each category; 2 Difference (95% CI) in variety counts in BLISS relative to Controls analyzed using linear regression, adjusting for maternal education and parity (stratification variables) and infant sex.
Table 5. Food exposure and perceived preference scores for different tastes at 12 months of age in relation to the complementary feeding style.
Table 5. Food exposure and perceived preference scores for different tastes at 12 months of age in relation to the complementary feeding style.
Taste categoryControl
(n = 81)
BLISS
(n = 92)
Difference (95% CI)
BLISS: Control 3
Exposure score—i.e., offered to the infant 1
Sweet 46.0 (2.1)5.7 (2.3)−0.3 (−1.0, 0.4)
Savory-vegetable 55.9 (2.8)6.5 (2.2)0.8 (0.01, 1.5)
Savory-meat 67.1 (2.8)7.7 (2.5)0.4 (−0.3, 1.2)
Savory-non-meat high-protein 76.8 (2.7)8.0 (2.5)1.3 (0.6, 2.1)
Savory-French fries 86.8 (3.8)7.0 (4.0)0.2 (−1.0, 1.4)
Salty 95.0 (2.3)4.6 (2.3)−0.5 (−1.2, 0.2)
Preference score—i.e., consumed by the infant 2
Sweet 44.5 (0.5)4.5 (0.4)0.0 (−0.1, 0.1)
Savory-vegetable 54.1 (0.8)4.2 (0.6)0.1 (−0.1, 0.3)
Savory-meat 64.4 (0.6)4.5 (0.5)0.1 (−0.1, 0.2)
Savory-non-meat high-protein 74.3 (0.7)4.5 (0.5)0.2 (0.04, 0.4)
Savory-French fries 84.7 (0.4)4.6 (0.6)−0.1 (−0.3, 0.1)
Salty 94.4 (0.8)4.4 (0.8)0.0 (−0.3, 0.3)
1 mean (SD) number of times this type of food was offered. Response options were: ‘never offered’, 1–’3 times’, ‘4–6 times’, ‘7–10 times’, ‘11 or more times’, and ‘don’t know’, and were coded as 0, 2, 5, 8.5, 11, and missing, respectively. For each food-type category, the mean number of exposures for the foods in that category was calculated for each participant. Missing values (or ‘don’t know’ responses) were excluded; 2 as data on intake were only available for foods that had been offered, preference score is presented as mean (SD) of the mean score of intake of the foods in that scale on a response scale from 1 (no, refuses to taste) to 5 (always eats when offered); 3 difference (95% CI) in scores in BLISS relative to Controls analyzed using linear regression, adjusting for maternal education and parity (stratification variables) and infant sex; 4 bananas, cookies, yogurt, raisins, breakfast cereals; 5 broccoli, cabbage, spinach, cauliflower, tomato; 6 baloney, ground meat, cooked meat cuts, sausage; 7 cheese, baked beans, egg; 8 French fries/hot chips/wedges; 9 yeast extract, olives.
Table 6. Food exposure and perceived preference scores for different textures at 12 months of age in relation to the complementary feeding style.
Table 6. Food exposure and perceived preference scores for different textures at 12 months of age in relation to the complementary feeding style.
Texture categoryControl
(n = 81)
BLISS
(n = 92)
Difference (95% CI)
BLISS: Control 3
Exposure score—i.e., offered to the infant 1
Smooth 48.0 (3.6)7.6 (3.3)−0.4 (−1.5, 0.6)
Lumpy 57.5 (2.3)8.5 (1.9)1.1 (0.4, 1.7)
Chewy 65.2 (2.3)5.6 (2.1)0.4 (−0.2, 1.0)
Crunchy 73.7 (2.7)3.5 (2.6)−0.2 (−1.0, 0.5)
Preference score—i.e., consumed by the infant 2
Smooth 44.6 (0.6)4.6 (0.6)0.0 (−0.2, 0.2)
Lumpy 54.2 (0.5)4.4 (0.4)0.2 (0.01, 0.3)
Chewy 64.3 (0.5)4.3 (0.5)0.0 (−0.1, 0.2)
Crunchy 74.6 (0.5)4.5 (0.5)0.0 (−0.2, 0.1)
1 mean (SD) number of times this type of food was offered. Response options were: ‘never offered’, 1–’3 times’, ‘4–6 times’, ‘7–10 times’, ‘11 or more times’, and ‘don’t know’, and were coded as 0, 2, 5, 8.5, 11, and missing, respectively. For each texture category, the mean number of exposures for the foods in that category was calculated for each participant. Missing values (or ‘don’t know’ responses) were excluded; 2 as data on intake were only available for foods that had been offered, preference score is presented as mean (SD) of the mean score of intake of the foods in that scale on a response scale from 1 (no, refuses to taste) to 5 (always eats when offered); 3 difference (95% CI) in scores in BLISS relative to Controls analyzed using linear regression, adjusting for maternal education and parity (stratification variables) and infant sex; 4 yogurt, yeast extract; 5 ground meat, baked beans, egg, cauliflower, banana, broccoli; 6 raisins, spinach, cooked meat cuts, sausage, cabbage; 7 chunky peanut butter, breakfast cereals, cookies.
Table 7. Perceived food preference scores at 24 months of age in relation to the complementary feeding style [34].
Table 7. Perceived food preference scores at 24 months of age in relation to the complementary feeding style [34].
Food CategoryControl
(n = 75)
BLISS
(n = 86)
Difference (95% CI)
BLISS: Control 1
‘Vegetables’ 23.7 (3.1, 4.1)3.8 (3.2, 4.2)0.1 (−0.2, 0.5)
‘Fruit’ 34.8 (4.4, 4.9)4.8 (4.4, 4.9)0.1 (−0.1, 0.2)
‘Meat and fish’ 44.2 (3.7, 4.6)4.3 (4.0, 4.5)0.1 (−0.2, 0.3)
‘Desserts’ 54.4 (3.8, 4.8)4.4 (4.0, 4.8)0.1 (−0.2, 0.4)
Data presented as median (25th, 75th percentile). Scores found as the mean liking (on a response scale from 1 (dislikes a lot) to 5 (likes a lot) for all items. Missing items were imputed with the mean of the remaining items in the scale; 1 difference (95% CI) in scores in BLISS relative to Controls analyzed using median regression, adjusting for maternal education and parity (stratification variables) and infant sex; 2 broccoli, cabbage, carrots, cauliflower, green beans, mushrooms, parsnips, lettuce, and tomato; 3 apples, bananas, oranges, grapes, peaches, strawberries, fruit juice, as well as ice cream, and frozen ice pops; 4 beef, lamb, pork, chicken, bacon, fried fish, fresh fish, and canned fish; 5 cream, cakes, sweet pastries, fruit-based desserts, custard, and dairy desserts.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Morison, B.J.; Heath, A.-L.M.; Haszard, J.J.; Hein, K.; Fleming, E.A.; Daniels, L.; Erickson, E.W.; Fangupo, L.J.; Wheeler, B.J.; Taylor, B.J.; et al. Impact of a Modified Version of Baby-Led Weaning on Dietary Variety and Food Preferences in Infants. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1092. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10081092

AMA Style

Morison BJ, Heath A-LM, Haszard JJ, Hein K, Fleming EA, Daniels L, Erickson EW, Fangupo LJ, Wheeler BJ, Taylor BJ, et al. Impact of a Modified Version of Baby-Led Weaning on Dietary Variety and Food Preferences in Infants. Nutrients. 2018; 10(8):1092. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10081092

Chicago/Turabian Style

Morison, Brittany J., Anne-Louise M. Heath, Jillian J. Haszard, Karen Hein, Elizabeth A. Fleming, Lisa Daniels, Elizabeth W. Erickson, Louise J. Fangupo, Benjamin J. Wheeler, Barry J. Taylor, and et al. 2018. "Impact of a Modified Version of Baby-Led Weaning on Dietary Variety and Food Preferences in Infants" Nutrients 10, no. 8: 1092. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10081092

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop