Next Article in Journal
Pluractionality of Events in Macuxi: A Morpho-Syntactic and Semantic Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
A Special (Question) View on Wh-Doubling in Lombard Varieties
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Acoustic Variability of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ in Spanish: A Pilot Study

Department of World Languages & Literatures, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN 38152, USA
Languages 2023, 8(3), 224; https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030224
Submission received: 28 July 2023 / Revised: 10 September 2023 / Accepted: 13 September 2023 / Published: 21 September 2023

Abstract

:
Propelled by existing research on stop consonant variability in Spanish, this pilot study provides a preliminary acoustic analysis of stop consonant lenition exhibited by speakers of six different varieties of Spanish in Latin America and Spain to explore the gradient acoustic variability in the production of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ among speakers of different Spanish varieties. Using the acoustic correlate of relative intensity, this analysis considers the effect of various linguistic factors (phoneme, lexical stress, point of articulation, sonority) as well as the extralinguistic factor of Spanish variety on the production of stop consonants in initial, intervocalic position. Results display a higher degree of weakening among speakers of Peninsular and Insular varieties of Spanish compared with those of Latin-American varieties such as Colombian, Mexican, and Peruvian. These exploratory data support the gradient nature of consonant lenition and provide a baseline for future research on stop consonant variability across the Spanish-speaking world.

1. Introduction

Recent synchronic evidence reveals that stop consonant lenition exists today in different Spanish varieties (Carrasco et al. 2012; Colantoni and Marinescu 2010; Hualde et al. 2011; Lewis 2001; Oftedal 1985; O’Neill 2010), allowing for a series of observations about synchronic lenition processes and the degree of variability in stop consonant production across the Spanish-speaking world. Previous research on consonant lenition in Spanish identifies variability in consonant production for both voiced and voiceless stop consonants /ptk/ and /bdɡ/, which is affected by linguistic factors such as phoneme, sonority, point of articulation, lexical stress, and flanking vowels (Cho et al. 2006; Cole et al. 1999; Gili Fivela et al. 2008; Kirchner 2004; Lavoie 2001; Lewis 2001; Lisker and Abramson 1964; Ortega-Llebaria 2004; Wireback 1997). In addition to these linguistic factors, the extralinguistic variable of region can also affect the degree of consonant occlusion (Bayley and Tarone 2011; Labov 2001; Lewis 2001). Voicing of voiceless occlusives /ptk/ has been found in many varieties of Spanish as well as the extensive weakening of voiced occlusives /bdɡ/ (Hualde et al. 2011; Oftedal 1985; O’Neill 2010; Trujillo 1980). Although these more recent studies are acoustically based, few analyze consonant weakening variation across multiple varieties of Spanish while controlling for region.
The current study provides an exploratory acoustic analysis and comparison of the production of occlusive consonants /ptk/ with /bdɡ/ from speakers of different varieties of Spanish with the goal of demonstrating the gradient phonetic realizations of stop consonant series /ptk/ to /bdɡ/ and arranging their allophonic variants on a continuum of consonant lenition, or weakening, ranging from weak to strong. Previous work on lenition in Romance languages discusses consonant lenition both diachronically and synchronically to determine the driving factors behind language change (Cravens 2002; Jungemann 1955; Martinet 1952; Tovar 1948); however, many of these studies do not provide information regarding synchronic acoustic variability of consonant production and weakening from speakers of different Spanish varieties, while considering various linguistic and extralinguistic factors that may affect their production.

2. Background

Lenition has been described as movement towards deletion, or increased sonority; that is, the degree of openness of a particular segment, either vocalic or consonantal, decrease in articulatory effort as well as modifications to articulatory gestural patterns (Lavoie 2001). Vowels and consonants are ranked in order of their openness on a sonority scale from weakest consonantality (i.e., least obstruction of airflow), to strongest consonantality (i.e., most obstruction of airflow); that is, most open or vowel-like to most closed or consonant-like (see Table 1 from Hualde (2005, p. 72)).
The consonants within this sonority scale are also ranked according to their openness, or degree of constriction. Lass (1984) offers two hierarchical consonant scales (see 1), where movement toward the right on both hierarchies indicates lenition, or consonant weakening, and movement toward the left indicates fortition, or consonant strengthening. The hierarchy in (1a) represents a scale of openness, where rightward movement along the scale equals less resistance to airflow within the vocal tract. Hualde (2005, p. 300) defines a stop as a “consonant produced with total occlusion in the oral cavity,” not allowing the passage of air such as /ptkbdɡ/. Fricatives are “consonants produced with a narrowing of the articulatory channel, resulting in turbulence or noise,” such as /fθsʃxh/, while an approximant is a production where the articulatory organs in the oral cavity approach each other, but do not touch and do not have enough constriction to produce friction, thus differing from a fricative production (Hualde 2005, pp. 295, 297). Examples of approximants in Spanish include the allophones of /bdɡ/, [βðɣ], as well as [j] and [w] in lexical items with <ie> and <ue> combinations, where the articulatory organs approach each other, allowing some constriction but no contact. Zero represents total deletion of the consonant. The hierarchy in (1b) represents a sonority scale, where rightward movement along the scale equals higher output of periodic acoustic energy, which serves as a cue to decreased obstruction of airflow (Lass 1984). Voiceless productions do not include vibration of the glottal folds, whereas voiced segments are produced with glottal fold vibration.
(1)a.Stop → Fricative → Approximant → Zero
b.Voiceless → Voiced
While the scales in (1) may not be appropriate for other consonant segments, such as affricates, they inform the primary purpose of the current study by describing occlusive consonants with respect to their level of sonority as well as degree of constriction. This study explores the extent to which occlusive consonants assimilate in vocalic quality to their adjacent vowels (i.e., the degree of consonant lenition) and other linguistic variables that evidence weakening phenomena. These variables are discussed in the following subsections.

2.1. Lenition and Lexical Stress

Lexical stress is an important factor in consonant lenition, concretely expressed through the phonetic correlates of fundamental frequency (f0; the perception of f0 is pitch), duration and/or intensity (Hualde 2005). While a higher f0 does not necessarily indicate a stressed syllable, an f0 rise through a stressed syllable is often essential in identifying possible cues to stress rather than other types of f0 movement or f0 peaks (Hualde 2005). Duration is also a relevant phonetic correlate to the discussion on lexical stress, given that stressed syllables generally have a longer duration than unstressed syllables. The third expression of lexical stress is intensity, which is one of the phonetic correlates of particular interest to the current study. With respect to consonant lenition, measuring relative intensity (RI) or the difference in decibels (dB) between the intensity of a consonant and the intensity of the following vowel informs the notion of relative prominence effects by providing the degree of occlusion or openness of a particular segment. Stressed syllables, therefore, use increased articulatory energy and gestures, which are manifested through enhanced f0, duration and/or intensity. These three correlates enhance the prominence of lexical stress, ultimately working against the weakening processes of lenition because more prominent syllables experience less consonant weakening than unstressed syllables (Browman and Goldstein 1992; Cho et al. 2006; Gili Fivela et al. 2008; Keating et al. 2003).

2.2. Lenition and Prosodic Position

At the beginning of domains, such as syllable onset or the beginning of a prosodic word, there is strengthening, but not at the end of a domain. In a study about prosodic boundaries in two varieties of Italian, Gili Fivela et al. (2008) found domain initial strengthening in the production of consonants in an initial position, that is, at the beginning of an intonational phrase as well as in smaller domains, such as the intermediate phrase. Cho et al. (2006) investigated word-initial strengthening of native speakers of American English, concluding that there are acoustic differences between consonants in word-initial position and those in word-internal position, indicating strong evidence for word-initial consonant strengthening. In a cross-linguistic investigation between English, French, Korean, and Taiwanese to compare and determine the effects of word-initial strengthening, Keating et al. (2003) identify clear distinctions between the initial position and internal position in each of the four languages.
Intervocalic position, or VCV, is among those at the top of the list of favorable environments for lenition processes to occur (Lass 1984), given the sonorous quality of adjacent vowels and the assimilation processes that promote increased sonority of occlusive consonants when found in this position. While word-final is also a likely position in which to find consonant weakening, the current study will only focus on word-initial consonants preceded by a vowel. Given the research on domain initial strengthening, consonants in word-initial intervocalic position are expected to be more highly occluded than those in word-internal intervocalic position. These studies demonstrate the need to investigate not only phonological correlates like lexical stress and flanking vowels, but also word position. In prosodically strong positions such as word-initial as well as in stressed syllables, the articulatory gestures are reinforced in many languages, indicating a cross-linguistic phenomenon not limited to Spanish.

2.3. Lenition of Stop Consonants in Spanish

2.3.1. Lenition of /ptk/

Previous studies show that sonorization of voiceless occlusives is attested in the spontaneous speech of many varieties of Spanish, including Canary Islands Spanish, Andalusian Spanish, and the Spanish of Mallorca in the Balearic Islands (Hualde et al. 2011; Oftedal 1985; O’Neill 2010; Trujillo 1980). This phenomenon appears gradient in nature, rather than binary, with higher and lesser degrees of weakening occurring along a lenition continuum, varying according to context and sociolinguistic factors (Colantoni and Marinescu 2010; Lewis 2001; Oftedal 1985; Trujillo 1980). According to these studies, in addition to the traditional voiceless realization, underlying /ptk/ can be realized phonetically as completely or partially voiced as well as approximant (see 2).
(2)Languages 08 00224 i001
Comparative studies on voiceless stop consonant lenition conclude that some Spanish varieties exhibit higher degrees of consonant weakening than others. For example, in Lewis (2001), a Northern Spanish dialect appeared to be at a more advanced state of change with respect to consonant lenition than a Central Colombian dialect, further suggesting a lenition continuum from weak to strong. O’Neill (2010) concluded that in Andalusian Spanish, most voiceless occlusive segments in intervocalic position are realized as voiced segments, and in turn, the underlying voiced segments can result in loss. In the Spanish of Mallorca, Hualde et al. (2011) found that one-third of underlying voiceless occlusives in intervocalic position experience complete, or at least partial, voicing of /ptk/ in semi-spontaneous speech. Relative intensity differences are higher for voiceless segments than for voiced segments, an expected finding; however, the authors also conclude that the degree of lenition varies according to point of articulation. Velar segments are more sonorous than coronals, which in turn are more sonorous than labials. Important to note in the work of Hualde et al. (2011) is that despite the voiced realizations of underlying /ptk/, this does not indicate merger of phonemes /ptk/ with /bdɡ/, given that the realizations of these phoneme series differ with respect to their degree of constriction.

2.3.2. Lenition of /bdɡ/

Scholarly work on voiced stop consonant production in Spanish shows great variability in the degree of constriction according to factors such as prosodic position and flanking vowels as well as dialectal variation. In a review of Castilian Spanish, Cole et al. (1999) found significant variation in lenition of /ɡ/ according to prosodic position and flanking vowels. Their study concludes that /ɡ/ exhibits the greatest degree of weakening following a stressed vowel or with flanking vowels /o/ and /u/. Weakening is not as prominent, however, when /ɡ/ precedes a stressed vowel or flanking vowels /i/, /e/, and /a/. Results indicate that /ɡ/ has a more complete closure when flanked by /a/ instead of more closed vowels /u/ or /i/. Work completed by Ortega-Llebaria (2004) on Caribbean Spanish draws similar conclusions; /ɡ/ is more lenited with flanking vowels /i/ and /u/, but less lenited with flanking vowel /a/. Other studies such as Lewis (2001) and Kirchner (2004), however, determined that more open vowels favor weakening. Possible explanations of the differences across studies could include speaker demographic as well as task type. Participants from Lewis (2001) from Northern Spain and Central Colombia performed three tasks: semi-spontaneous conversation, textual reading, and reading of a word list; participants from Cole et al. (1999) were all from Spain and produced words set within a carrier phrase; and participants from Ortega-Llebaria (2004) were speakers of Caribbean Spanish and produced nonce words in the form of a game with cartoon characters. All these studies have very different participants as well as speech tasks; therefore, the diverse methodologies employed in these studies could be a contributing factor to the mixed results with respect to the effect of flanking vowels on consonant lenition.
Concerning prosodic position, Eddington (2011) concluded that /β/ and /ð/ favor a more occluded production following stressed syllables, in contrast with /ɣ/,1 which does not conform to the same pattern, exhibiting lower degrees of weakening between two stressed syllables. Also, word frequency affects /ð/ with higher degrees of weakening in high frequency words, but not /β/ or /ɣ/. The segment /ð/ also lenites more frequently in the past participle suffix -ado. Based on Eddington (2011), it is clear that /βðɣ/ alternations do not operate uniformly with respect to phonetic context, stress, word frequency, or suffix appearance. The current study investigates precisely this claim made by Eddington (2011), by examining the production of occlusive stop consonants through a gradient lens, with the understanding that segments may yield varied productions across different varieties of Spanish with respect to the different acoustic and extralinguistic variables studied.
Carrasco et al. (2012) compared allophonic patters of /bdɡ/ in Costa Rican Spanish and Madrid Spanish. The results determined that the Spanish of Costa Rica has a different allophonic distribution than that of Madrid, with more closed or open variants of /bdɡ/ according to position, whereas the allophones in the Madrid variety do clearly separate allophones in different positions. The allophones in Costa Rican Spanish are weaker in internal position as compared to initial position, and RI measures present higher values in stressed syllables. The authors connected these two varieties by saying that Costa Rican Spanish represents an earlier stage of weakening than the more extensive weakening found in the Madrid variety, much like the comparison made by Lewis (2001) between Central Colombian and Northern Spanish dialects.

2.3.3. Lenition of /ptk/ vs. /bdɡ/

Due to the inherent voiced and voiceless nature of /bdɡ/ and /ptk/, respectively, few studies have compared these two series with each other to investigate the gradient nature of lenition and the interplay of linguistic and extralinguistic factors such as point of articulation, lexical stress, flanking vowels or dialect. Martínez Celdrán (2009) investigated potential confusion of minimal pairs between intervocalic voiceless and voiced segments produced by a Murcian speaker. According to the author, variant realizations of the intervocalic occlusive segment can lead to possible confusion of the underlying phoneme (see 3).
(3)Languages 08 00224 i002
In a perceptual test used to determine to what extent speakers have difficulty identifying the underlying segment, the participants from Barcelona in Martínez Celdrán (2009) interpreted most of the proposed voicings––both underlyingly voiced and voiceless––as voiced in 92.5% of cases. That is, participants taking the perceptual test identified ‘bozo’ instead of the correct ‘pozo’. Participants completing the same perceptual test from Murcia, however, did not reach the same level of voicing identification, identifying the segments as voiced in only 69.48% of cases. Results of the perceptual test determined that participants favor the voiced variant when selecting among isolated words, but that confusion disappears when the words appear in context. Therefore, Martínez Celdrán (2009) concluded that due to the neutralization of voicing, tension, and duration of the segments, context is what allows the correct interpretation of consonant segments in intervocalic position.
In Argentine Spanish, Colantoni and Marinescu (2010) determined that there is not a correlation between weakening of intervocalic voiceless segments and intervocalic voiced segments. The results do not indicate clear evidence in favor of any voiceless occlusive weakening in intervocalic position in Argentine Spanish. However, the authors noted that when surface lenition of voiceless occlusives /ptk/ is observed, labial segments appear to demonstrate more lenition than coronal and velar segments, contrary to other studies that find the highest degree of lenition among velar segments (e.g., Hualde et al. 2011). When the authors observed lenition of voiced segments /bdɡ/, however, coronals are produced with more weakening than labials and velars, which is inconsistent with Hualde et al. (2011), but consistent with the results from Lavoie (2001). Colantoni and Marinescu (2010) also consider the effect of flanking vowels on the degree of weakening in Argentine Spanish, but their hypothesis that weakening would increase when the consonant segment is surrounded by more open vowels is only partially confirmed.
Previous work reviewed in this section determines that allophones of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ are gradient in nature and vary according to language variety and other linguistic factors, with some exhibiting more weakening, such as the higher degrees of stop consonant lenition found in peninsular varieties of Spanish as compared to Latin American varieties of Spanish. As shown in the previous work in this section underlyingly voiceless /ptk/ often surface as completely or partially voiced allophones [bdɡ]. Differing degrees of weakening are present with respect to point of articulation, in which velar segments are most lenited as compared with coronals and labials. These studies suggest that stop consonant lenition is not a binary phenomenon (i.e., voiced vs. voiceless), but instead more gradient in nature, where different Spanish varieties experience more or less consonant weakening according to various linguistic and extralinguistic factors. These studies inform the existing knowledge on the production of voiced stop consonants and motivate the current study by offering conclusions with respect to the variables that affect consonant lenition and demonstrate the variability that appears in stop consonant production in Spanish.

2.4. Lenition and Relative Intensity

As reviewed in the previous section, phonetic realizations of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ vary with respect to their degree of intensity across varieties of the same language as well as cross-linguistically, with realizations ranging from weak approximant to fully occluded stop, including voicing and even spirantization of the voiceless occlusive series. The acoustic variable of RI may provide evidence of consonant lenition, representing the difference in dB between the segment in question and the following vowel. This is measured by identifying the minimum intensity of the target segment using the intensity curve in Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2013), as well as the maximum intensity of the following vowel and taking the difference. The minimum pitch value set for data analysis in Praat was 75 Hz (see Figure 1, Figure 2 and Figure 3 for the Praat spectrogram and intensity curve of Spanish utterances that include voiceless and voiced consonant productions). Figure 1 represents Yo digo podo para ti (‘I say I prune for you’). Within the boundaries of the syllable po- in Figure 1, the minimum intensity value of [p] is 40.9 dB, and the maximum intensity value of the following vowel [o] is 71.4 dB. The RI difference, then, between the valley and peak is 30.5 dB.
Figure 2 includes the spectrogram and intensity curve for the utterance Yo digo busco para ti (‘I say I look for for you’). Within the boundaries of the syllabus bus- in Figure 2, the minimum intensity value of [b] is 57.1 dB, and the maximum intensity value of the following vowel [u] is 64.6 dB, yielding a RI difference of 7.5 dB. The lower RI value of this consonant [b] indicates a more open production than the [p] analyzed in Figure 1.
Figure 3 represents Yo digo buscó para ti (‘I say he looked for for you)’ with an even lower RI measurement; thus, a more open, vowel-like production of the consonant [b]. Within the boundaries of the syllabus bus- in Figure 3, the minimum intensity value of [b] is 63.0 dB, and the maximum intensity value of the following vowel [u] is 67.2 dB, with a RI difference between the valley and peak of 4.2 dB. A lower RI measurement indicates a high degree of weakening in intervocalic position. Comparing the production of [b] in Figure 2 and Figure 3, there is a difference in the consonant valleys and the vowel peaks. One contributing factor to this difference is lexical stress; that is, busco ‘I look for’ in Figure 2 is a paroxytone word, where the first syllable bus- carries lexical stress and is therefore more prominent, with less consonant weakening, while buscó ‘he looked for’ in Figure 3 is an oxytone word and carries lexical stress on -có, leaving the first syllable bus- less prominent, and therefore with more consonant weakening.
Given the inherent nature of both occlusive series, RI can be used to compare voiceless segments to their voiced counterparts, a task that is difficult to achieve with other acoustic variables such as voice onset time or percent voicing. In intervocalic position in Spanish, higher RI measurements of phonemes /ptk/ and lower RI measurements of phonemes /bdɡ/ are expected. RI can be used to provide evidence of consonant lenition and weakening by acoustically quantifying the degree of occlusion and the manner of articulation of consonant production. The current study examines the gradient nature of these lenition processes in Spanish in order to situate the occlusive consonants /bdɡ/ and /ptk/ on a lenition continuum from weak to strong and to determine the acoustic variability that exists among these segments in different Spanish varieties.

2.5. Motivations and Research Questions

From the above review of recent work on stop consonant weakening, many studies focus on either voiced stops /bdɡ/ or voiceless stops /ptk/ and only analyze one or two Spanish varieties at a time for side-by-side comparison, rendering difficult a complete synchronic analysis of stop consonant lenition across the Spanish-speaking world. These studies, therefore, motivate the current study to explore the degree of consonant weakening for both /bdɡ/ and /ptk/ and the potential degree of overlap between the phonetic realizations of these phonemes by determining the extent of allophonic variability in the speech of multiple Spanish varieties when controlling for variables such as point of articulation, lexical stress, flanking vowels, and speech variety.
For this study, data were collected from a sample set of native Spanish speakers from six different regions to create a varied native speaker pool to compare stop consonant production and variation between different varieties, including Peninsular varieties of Spanish as well as Latin American varieties (Colombian, Chilean, and Mexican Spanish). Peninsular varieties tend to exhibit more open consonant realizations, that is, higher degrees of weakening, while Latin American varieties have previously yielded more occluded consonant productions, that is, lesser degrees of weakening (Carrasco et al. 2012; Lewis 2001). The varieties represented are grouped primarily according to country, except for Spain, which is divided into two groups based on like phonetic characterizations (Hualde 2005): North-Central Peninsular varieties and Southern and Insular Peninsular varieties. The former includes participants from cities such as Ponferrada, Lugo, Valladolid, Salamanca, and Madrid, while the latter includes two southern Peninsular cities, Granada and Malaga, as well as two insular varieties from Grand Canary Island and Mallorca. The southern Peninsular cities are grouped together with the insular varieties since previous research shows that speakers from these regions exhibit higher degrees of weakening than those from North-Central Spain (c.f. Lewis 2001 for a Northern Peninsular variety, Hualde et al. 2011 and Oftedal 1985 for Insular varieties—Balearic and Canary Islands, respectively—and O’Neill 2010 for Andalusian Spanish). Although the geographic distribution of the Spanish varieties included is by no means exhaustive, the cities included offer a geographic range of different varieties across the Spanish-speaking world.
The research summarized here motivates the current study by highlighting main findings on the topic of stop consonant weakening in Spanish and identifying areas for continued exploration of acoustic variability. The research questions are the following: (1) how does consonant lenition of voiced /ptk/ and voiced /bdɡ/ vary according to linguistic factors such as point of articulation, lexical stress, and following vowel in different Spanish varieties? and (2) does consonant lenition depend on Spanish variety, and if this is the case, which varieties exhibit higher and lower degrees of stop consonant weakening? This research will explore variability in the degree of weakening across and among different Spanish varieties using RI measurements for voiced /bdɡ/ and voiceless /ptk/. In analyzing data from these geographical areas and controlling for the linguistic variables, the goal of this study is to initiate a review of synchronic consonant lenition of both voiceless occlusives as well as voiced occlusives of multiple Spanish varieties to explore existing research gaps in stop consonant variability across the Spanish-speaking world.

3. Methodology

3.1. Participants

Due to the exploratory nature of the pilot study, every effort was made to select a small, but representative, sample of native speakers of Spanish originating from a wide variety of regions where Spanish is the primary language. Therefore, eighteen participants, native speakers of a Spanish variety representing students and faculty at an American university were recruited via personal connections, and interviewed for this study (see Table 2). Ranging in age from 22 to 59 years of age and including 14 females and 4 males, participants were originally from Colombia (3), Mexico (1), Chile (3), Peru (3), Northern Peninsular Spain (7), and Southern Peninsular/Insular Spain (4). The participants spent varying amounts of time in the United States and other countries, with time away from their home country ranging from 4 months to 28 years with an average of 10.5 years spent in the United States.2 In a study of Korean–English bilinguals, Yeni-Komshian et al. (2000) show that pronunciation proficiency in the first language (L1) changes depending on age of arrival in the USA, i.e., the beginning of L2 learning. It is important to note, therefore, that time away from their home country could influence L1 pronunciation for speakers in the current study. While all study participants reported their first language as Spanish, other languages spoken included primarily English, and some speakers also had some knowledge of other languages such as French, Italian, Portuguese, Galician, and German. While recruiting participants, the intention was to find a wide variety of speakers from all over the Spanish-speaking world, while trying to have multiple speakers from each general variety; however, there was only one speaker representative of a Mexican Spanish variety. In the case of Mexican Spanish, therefore, individual speaker differences weigh heavily in the interpretation of the results and findings.

3.2. Materials and Tasks

The participants completed two tasks: a brief demographic questionnaire and a phrase-reading task. The demographic questionnaire asked for the participants’ age, gender, level of education, city of origin, language background including Spanish as well as other languages, and time spent in the United States. Following the completion of the demographic questionnaire, the participants recorded a set of phrases in a quiet room using a Blue Snowflake USB Recorder. The phrases were read via PowerPoint presentation, with one phrase per slide. The task included words replicated from Casteñada Vicente (1986), with the interested segments /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ set within the carrier phrase Yo digo ____ para ti ‘I say ____ for you’. See examples in (4) below and Appendix A Table A1 for a complete list of all words analyzed. The segments investigated here are limited to initial position only, leaving the comparison of stop consonant production between initial and internal position for future research.
(4)a.Yo digo paso para ti. ‘I say I pass for you’
b.Yo digo pasó para ti. ‘I say he passed for you’
c.Yo digo bajo para ti. ‘I say I remove for you’
d.Yo digo bajó para ti. ‘I say he removed for you’
In order to represent all of the variables in question, the task included 10 words for each phoneme /ptk/ and /bdɡ/, 5 in stressed syllable position followed by each of the 5 Spanish vowels /aeiou/, and 5 in unstressed syllable position followed by each vowel as well. The 60 phrases were included randomly in a PowerPoint presentation, along with 10 distractor phrases, totaling 70 with 2 additional phrases at the beginning to practice the format of the task. The participants read each phrase twice. This task included the production of 120 total phrases multiplied by 18 participants (120 × 18 = 2160 tokens).

3.3. Data Analysis

This study was designed to control for consonant sonority, place of articulation, lexical stress, and following vowel. Both sets of stop consonants were included in the data set, voiceless /ptk/ as well as their voiced counterparts /bdɡ/, with their members representing three distinct points of articulation: labial, dental, and velar, respectively. In Example (5), there is a consonant sonority contrast between voiceless stop consonant /t/ and voiced stop consonant /d/, while in Example (6) there is a place of articulation contrast between labial /p/, dental /t/ and velar /k/.
(5)Yo digo talo para ti. ‘I say I cut down for you’
Yo digo dejo para ti. ‘I say flavor for you’
(6)Yo digo pillo para ti. ‘I say thief for you’
Yo digo toco para ti. ‘I say I play for you’
Yo digo casa para ti. ‘I say home for you’
For each consonant segment, the task included the segment both in stressed as well as unstressed syllable position, to determine the effects of lexical stress on the degree of consonant weakening. All the consonant segments included were in word-initial position. As discussed in Section 2, word-initial position exhibits strengthening as compared to word-internal position (Cho et al. 2006; Gili Fivela et al. 2008; Keating et al. 2003). This study, therefore, limits the scope to include only stop consonants in word-initial position, assuming that more extensive degrees of weakening would be present in word-internal position. See Example (7) for a contrast between /ɡ/ in a stressed syllable gano ‘I win’ and an unstressed syllable ganó ‘she won’. Prosodic position is a controlled variable since all of the consonants analyzed were in word-initial, phrase internal, intervocalic position.
(7)Yo digo gano para ti. ‘I say I win for you’
Yo digo ganó para ti. ‘I say she won for you’
Finally, the words in the task also control for flanking vowels, evenly distributing all five vowels after each stop consonant, both in stressed and unstressed position. See Example (8), where the voiced velar segment /ɡ/ is followed by each of the five Spanish vowels, /aeiou/. The preceding vowel is kept constant in the carrier phrase: /o/. It is important to note that the ‘u’ of guerra ‘war’ and guiso ‘stew’ in (8) is not pronounced.
(8)Yo digo gano para ti. ‘I say I win for you’
Yo digo guerra para ti. ‘I say war for you’
Yo digo guiso para ti. ‘I say stew for you’
Yo digo gozo para ti. ‘I say joy for you’
Yo digo gusto para ti. ‘I say taste for you’
Following participant recruitment and participation in both tasks, each of the tokens were coded for RI of the consonant segment using Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2013) as indicated in Section 2.4. A total of 2160 tokens were coded in this way by recording the RI of each token, including occlusive consonants in both stressed and unstressed position and taking into account the following vowel. A RI measurement closer to 0 dB is indicative of a more vocalic production, that is, a weakened, pure approximant, whereas a higher RI measurement is indicative of a strengthened, more consonant-like production. The data were then analyzed using generalized mixed effects models in which the outcome variable RI in dBs was predicted by region (6 regions total), point of articulation (velar, bilabial or dental), sonority (voiced or voiceless), stress (stressed or unstressed), horizontal position (anterior, central or posterior), vertical position (high, mid or low). Several interactions were also assessed. Three total two-way interactions included region by point of articulation, region by sonority, and a stress by region. Four total three-way interactions were assessed: region by place of articulation and sonority, stress by region and sonority, region by sonority by horizontal vowel, and finally region by sonority by vertical position. Random intercepts by participant were included to take into account the nested structure of the data. The normality of residuals were determined by visual inspections of a Q-Q and residual versus fitted plots. Main effects and interactions were determined by carrying out nested model comparisons using the ANOVA function in R, in which alpha was set to 0.05. The size of the effects can be derived from the beta values in the table found in Appendix A, Table A2. The nested model comparisons were performed from a null model, i.e., intercept-only model, and each fixed effect predictor and interaction was added one at a time. In this case, significance simply tests against 0 and the model output (parameter estimates or beta values) shows the effect of each predictor while holding the others constant. Finally, model fit was determined by marginal and conditional R squared. The following section presents the results of the analysis and modeling.

4. Results

Results determine a main effect for region (χ(5) = 16.31; p < 0.005) meaning that overall there were significant differences in RI between all regions investigated. There was no main effect for point of articulation, suggesting that the model could not detect a difference in RI between bilabial, dental, or velar segments overall. Sonority (χ(1) = 1800, p < 0.005) and stress (χ(1) = 395.17; p < 0.005) both produced a main effect meaning that voiced and voiceless as well as stressed and unstressed consonants were realized with distinct RI. The horizontal (χ(2) = 29.7; p < 0.005) and vertical (χ(2) = 78.15; p < 0.005) position of vowels also produced a main effect, that is, there were differences in RI between anterior, central, and posterior vowels as well as between high, central, and low vowels. The following sections will comment on each of the variables as well as two- and three-way interactions between each of these with region.

4.1. Relative Intensity and Point of Articulation

Table 3 outlines descriptive mean RI measurements in dBs according to Spanish variety and phoneme for all six of the Spanish varieties analyzed. Modeling did detect a three-way interaction between region, point of articulation, and sonority (χ(6) = 27.38; p < 0.05), meaning that the difference in RI between bilabial, dental, and velar segments varies by whether the consonant is voiced or voiceless and by region (see Figure 4). While descriptive results depict only minor changes according to point of articulation for voiceless segments /ptk/, the results of the statistical analysis in Figure 4 determine that voiceless velar /k/ is most lenited for Colombian, Peruvian, Peninsular, and Mexican speakers. Voiceless dental /t/ is the most lenited segment among Chilean speakers. For voiced segments /bdɡ/, voiced velar /ɡ/ is least lenited for Colombian, Peruvian, Mexican, and North-Central Peninsular speakers whereas it is the most lenited for Southern/Insular Peninsular speakers. Voiced dental /d/ is the most lenited for Chilean speakers. Overall, results determined that speakers exhibit greater/lesser degrees of weakening according to point of articulation and sonority.
The RI contrast between more occluded consonant segments and more lenited consonant segments is illustrated below by two Praat images of the same utterance, Yo digo peso para ti ‘I say weight for you’, first in Figure 5 from a Colombian speaker and then in Figure 6 from a speaker from Southern Spain. Within the boundaries of pe- in Figure 5, the minimum intensity value of [p] is 34.5 dB, and the maximum intensity value of the following vowel [e] is 71.6 dB. The RI difference between the valley and peak for [p] by the Colombian speaker is 37.1 dB. Within the boundaries of pe- in Figure 6, the minimum intensity value of [p] is 39.6 dB, and the maximum intensity value of the following vowel [e] is 58.3 dB. The RI difference between the valley and peak for [p] by the Southern Peninsular speaker is 18.7 dB. As is evidenced in Figure 5 and Figure 6, the production of [p] by the Southern Peninsular speaker is weaker than that of the Colombian speaker.

4.2. Relative Intensity and Sonority

Combining the results from Table 3 yields the average RI of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ in each of the six Spanish varieties analyzed. These mean results are presented in Table 4 along with the standard deviation (SD) for each. Column 2 of Table 4 indicates that the average RI of /ptk/ ranges from 20.2 to 27.1 dB, while column 3 of Table 4 determines the average RI of /bdɡ/ to range from 7.4 to 16.5 dB. While the average production of the voiced and voiceless stop consonants in the six varieties of Spanish do not overlap, all varieties do maintain a distinction between the voiced and voiceless series since there is at least a 6.3 dB RI difference (=IntDiff) separating the two, found in the last column of Table 4. The results here are consistent with the previous literature highlighted in Section 2 in that Peninsular varieties (both North-Central Peninsular and Southern/Insular Peninsular in the case of this study) exhibit lower RI values than those among speakers of Latin American varieties; however, of particular interest are the RI values for speakers of Chilean varieties since they show the lowest RI values overall. Other scholars (Figueroa and Evans 2014; Pérez 2007; Rogers and Mirisis 2018) report extensive weakening of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ in Chilean Spanish.
Figure 7 illustrates the mixed effect model results of a two-way interaction between region and sonority (χ(5) = 82.64; p < 0.005). The effect of sonority (the difference in RI between voiced and voiceless consonants) differs by region. RI values on the y-axis ranging from 0 to 30 dB where 0 dB represents less occlusion and more vowel-like sounds whereas a RI value closer to >20 dB is indicative of a higher degree of occlusion and more consonant-like sound. In Figure 7, there is a distinct separation between the production of the voiced and voiceless segments within each variety, that is—/bdɡ/ are consistently less occluded than their voiceless counterparts, /ptk/, which is to be expected. Within each Spanish variety, the speakers are not conflating phoneme groups, ultimately maintaining a phonemic distinction between voiced /bdɡ/ and voiceless /ptk/, regardless of where their values are on the continuum. The consonant productions of the speakers do not overlap; however, Mexican /bdɡ/ average RI values approach the average RI value for /ptk/ in Chilean varieties (see Table 4). The proximity in RI values indicates that the voiceless /ptk/ in Chilean Spanish has a similar production to Mexican /bdɡ/, which are two independent sets of phonemes. It is noteworthy that most varieties maintain at least 10 dB Intensity Difference between /ptk/ and /bdɡ/, except for speakers of Peruvian and Mexican varieties where the Intensity Difference between /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ is 8.8 dB and 6.3 dB, respectively (see Table 4).

4.3. Relative Intensity According to Lexical Stress

As discussed previously, studies on consonant lenition in Spanish have shown that consonant segments in stressed syllables are produced with less consonant weakening than those in unstressed syllables. This was illustrated in Figure 2 and Figure 3 in Section 2.4 on variables. The data presented in Table 5 present descriptive RI measurements according to Spanish variety and lexical stress. Overall, occlusives in stressed position have higher RI values than those in unstressed position; this is confirmed by a main effect of stress (χ(1) = 395.17; p < 0.005). Overall, voiced and voiceless consonants are realized with distinct RI. There is not, however, a two-way interaction between stress and region, meaning that the effect of stress on RI does not depend on region (χ(5) = 6.94; p < 0.225).
This is further explored by distinguishing voiceless /ptk/ and voiced /bdɡ/ in Table 6 and Table 7, respectively. Descriptive data in both tables reveal that an intervocalic stop consonant initiating a stressed syllable is more occluded than an intervocalic stop consonant initiating an unstressed syllable. While the RI ranges vary for each Spanish variety, it is important to note that the stressed context, no matter the variety, is more occluded than the unstressed context, which is to be expected. The difference between the stressed and unstressed context is listed in column 4 of Table 6 and Table 7. In Table 6, the average RI in stressed and unstressed position for voiceless /ptk/ range from 3.5 dB in Chilean Spanish to 6.4 dB in Southern Peninsular and Insular Spanish. In Table 7, the average RI between stressed and unstressed position for voiced /bdɡ/ ranges from 2.7 dB in Colombian Spanish to 5.4 dB in Southern Peninsular and Insular Spanish. Despite these differences, modeling did not detect a three-way interaction between stress, sonority, and region, meaning that the difference in RI between stressed and unstressed position does not vary by whether the consonant is voiced or voiceless, or by region (χ(6) = 6.04; p < 0.418) (see Figure 8).

4.4. Relative Intensity According to Following Vowel

Previous research yields mixed results regarding the effects of flanking vowels on the degree of lenition of voiced and voiceless occlusives (Cole et al. 1999; Kirchner 2004; Lewis 2001; Ortega-Llebaria 2004); therefore, the current study investigated the effect of following vowel on RI where the task type involved a high degree of control. Because all tokens were produced within a carrier phrase, the left flanking vowel was controlled, and in all cases is /o/; the right flanking vowel was tested following each of the six occlusive consonants, in both stressed and unstressed positions.
Descriptive data provide some trends with respect to the effect of following vowel on RI for most varieties investigated. Although both horizontal (anterior, central, posterior) and vertical (high, mid, low) position both exhibited statistically significant differences in RI, there are no two-way interactions with either of these variables with region. That is, RI differences between anterior, central, and posterior vowels do not depend on region, but horizontal and vertical position on their own are significant. The same is the case for RI differences between high, mid, and low vowels. Table 8 presents descriptive RI values according to vowel position. For most varieties investigated, high and anterior vowels have lower RI values, and therefore more weakening, whereas central, low, and mid vowels have higher mean RI values. In other words, occlusives followed by the high, anterior /i/ are most lenited while occlusives followed by the low, mid vowel /a/ are least lenited in almost all the varieties investigated.

5. Discussion

The results presented in the previous section detail gradient variation in the degree of consonant occlusion according to the geographical location of the Spanish varieties analyzed depending on various linguistic factors. This section discusses the results within the context of the research questions, repeated here: (1) how does consonant lenition of voiced /ptk/ and voiced /bdɡ/ vary according to linguistic factors such as point of articulation, sonority, lexical stress, and following vowel in different Spanish varieties? and (2) does consonant lenition depend on Spanish variety, and if this is the case, which varieties exhibit higher and lower degrees of stop consonant weakening? Key points are summarized in Table 9.
Previous research suggests that velar segments would produce higher degrees of weakening when compared with labial and dental segments; this is only partially confirmed by the results of this study. Lewis (2001, p. 160) states that “[t]he influence of place of articulation on RI differences was not statistically significant” which is consistent with the statistical modeling from the current data set. When considering the interaction of both point of articulation and region on RI, however, the results are indeed significant, meaning that speakers from different regions exhibit differing degrees of weakening according to point of articulation. Given that Lewis (2001) includes data from one Peninsular variety and one Latin American variety to measure relative intensity differences, these results show promise for future research on differences in point of articulation according to region. Colantoni and Marinescu (2010) found the highest degree of lenition for dental stops in Argentine Spanish; this is also the case for dental stops for Chilean Spanish speakers in the current study; geographical proximity here could play a role in why point of articulation differs for Chilean speakers compared with those from the other speaker groups. Carrasco et al. (2012) find that voiced velar /ɡ/ is more lenited than bilabial /b/ and dental /d/ in postconsonantal position, but not in post-vocalic position where /ɡ/ is stronger. Given that the task items for the current study include only those in post-vocalic position, the findings confirm that /ɡ/ is more occluded than /b/ and /d/ and not only in North-Central Peninsular speakers, but also for Colombian, Peruvian, and Mexican speakers. The results here show that the voiced velar segment is stronger than the voiced bilabial and voiced dental, but that this does depend on region. Findings from the current analysis of point of articulation on relative intensity shows that region is also a significant factor in the equation and should be taken into account when reporting on stop consonant lenition variation.
In agreement with previous research on consonant lenition, results confirm that stop consonant productions are more lenited when followed by a lexically unstressed syllable as opposed to a lexically stressed syllable (Browman and Goldstein 1992; Cho et al. 2006; Gili Fivela et al. 2008; Keating et al. 2003). This is to be expected, confirming that higher degrees of weakening occur in unstressed syllables when compared to their stressed counterparts; however, the effect of lexical stress on RI does not interact with region. All data points were collected from word initial position; although still intervocalic within the carrier phrase, this is a prosodically strong position, and consonant weakening still occurs, perhaps albeit to a lesser degree. Although previous work tells us that consonant lenition in Spanish occurs both within words and across word boundaries, higher degrees of lenition are expected in word internal position as compared to word initial position. While the mixed effects model does produce a main effect for vowel horizontal and vertical positions; these variables do not interact with region, meaning that speakers from different regions do not produce more/less consonant weakening than those from other regions. The results for the current study are consistent with those found in Ortega-Llebaria (2004) where more lenition is found with high vowels /i/ and /u/ and less lenition with the low and mid vowel /a/, but contrast with those found in Lewis (2001) and Kirchner (2004), where mid and low vowels favor weakening, indicating a need for more data regarding the overall effects of flanking vowels on consonant lenition. Variables that could have impacted the results of the vowel analysis are task type. For example, data from Lewis (2001) include both spontaneous speech as well as a textual reading, a word list, and a short phrase list. Participants in Ortega-Llebaria (2004)’s study were asked to remember names of made-up cartoon characters; both of these studies use very different task types from the current study. It is noteworthy, however, that this study only analyzed the results of the following vowel on consonant production, keeping constant the preceding vowel as part of the carrier phrase, Yo digo ____ para ti ‘I say ____ for you’. The preceding vowel in all cases was /o/. Further research should explore both flanking vowels for potential effects on the degree of consonant weakening according to Spanish variety.
Statistical modeling confirms that RI of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ does depend on region overall and that the difference in RI between voiceless /ptk/ and voiced /bdɡ/ also differs by region. These results expand on those found in Lewis (2001) where /ptk/ is more lenited in a Northern Peninsular Spanish variety than a Central Colombian variety, as well as those from Carrasco et al. (2012) where Madrid Spanish /bdɡ/ has more extensive weakening than in a Costa Rican variety. Both Lewis (2001) and Carrasco et al. (2012) suggest that Peninsular varieties of Spanish are at a more advanced stages of consonant weakening than varieties in Latin America. The results from this highly controlled study confirm these results except for Chilean Spanish, which displays the greatest degree of stop consonant weakening among all six varieties investigated. What is clear across all speakers is the higher degree of occlusion for voiceless segments /ptk/ when compared to voiced segments /bdɡ/, which is to be expected considering the sonorous qualities of the voiced series. More occlusion, however, is a relative description when discussing the different Spanish varieties, since a higher degree of occlusion of /bdɡ/ in Mexican varieties is comparable to the lower degree of occlusion of /ptk/ in Chilean varieties.
Although there is no overlap between varieties, these data always display a distinction between /ptk/ and /bdɡ/, even though some Spanish varieties might be considered overall higher-leniting dialects or lesser-leniting varieties. It is also important to note that within each variety, the production of /bdɡ/ never reaches/overlaps with the production of /ptk/, maintaining the phonological distinction between the voiceless and voiced phonemes This indicates that within each variety, even if the speakers of that variety produce stop consonants with high degrees of lenition, they always distinguish between the two stop consonant series. Considering the push–pull chain and Structuralist models of language change (Alarcos Llorach 1961; Martinet 1952; Veiga 1988), speakers consider these two sets of segments /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ to be different and despite language evolution and increased lenition in some varieties, the distinction is consistently maintained, and a weaker production of /ptk/ will not threaten the existence of /bdɡ/. This simply indicates that production of /bdɡ/ will also be weaker. This is not a phonological change, rather a allophonic innovation within the existing phonological system. The results from the current data set, therefore, establish that Spanish variety does play a role in the degree of consonant weakening, where some varieties have lower RI values and therefore less occluded occlusive productions while others have higher RI values and therefore more highly occluded occlusive productions. The speakers of Chilean and Southern/Insular Peninsular varieties consistently produced /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ with lower RI values and overall weaker consonant productions, whereas speakers of Colombian and Mexican varieties exhibited higher degrees of occlusion for /ptk/ and /bdɡ/, respectively. The other varieties investigated are found in between the two, highlighting the gradient nature of consonant lenition. Consonant productions of speakers of Chilean varieties as well as Southern/Insular Peninsular are realized with more vowel-like qualities, even in word initial position. Speakers of the Mexican and Colombian varieties included here, on the other hand, tend to produce more conservative, occluded realizations of stop consonants, with only slight and sporadic weakening. Concerning the varieties analyzed for this study, the data display a range of consonant productions, instead of consistent productions of like-sounding consonants. This finding is relevant in that it motivates future research on the effect of language variety on consonant lenition. Preliminary findings indicate that there indeed exists the possibility of a correlation of consonant production of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ between more leniting and less leniting Spanish varieties with respect to RI.
Given the exploratory nature and highly controlled task design and analysis of this study, various limitations such as task design, participant demographics, sociolinguistic variables, and determination of Spanish ‘variety’ will inform future work on this topic. Expansion of this work should seek to measure the gradient nature of stop consonant lenition as it occurs in spontaneous, rather than laboratory, speech. The carrier phrase used to elicit the production of stop consonants can prime certain prosodic effects of the segment in question, resulting in relative intensity measurements that are not reflective of spontaneous speech. A larger, more representative participant pool would also contribute to the robustness of the results, especially since the Mexican Spanish variety only had one representative participant, and thus, the results are highly susceptible to individual variation. Many of the participants reported having spent many years away from their home country living in the United States. This environment of English–Spanish contact could have ultimately affected the overall results since some of the participants had only spent four months in the United States while others had lived in the United States for almost thirty years. Research has shown that extended periods of time in an immigrant country can have linguistic effects on the L1; therefore, the circumstances of participant bilingualism could have had an effect due to English contact, where lenition of intervocalic occlusive segments is not as strong. Future research would incorporate data from a larger pool of monolingual, native speakers of Spanish who still reside in their hometown/home country and have minimal contact with other language in addition to strengthening the participant pool for each Spanish variety tested.
The consideration of other sociolinguistic variables such as gender, level of education, and socioeconomic status could also prove fruitful for the investigation of consonant lenition across different varieties of Spanish. Previous studies such as Acosta-Martínez (2014), Alba (1988), Cedergren and Sankoff (1974), Labov (1966, 1972), Malaver and Samper Padilla (2016), Poplack (1978), and Rojas (1981) determine that these sociolinguistic variables are key to exploring phonological variation. The regional divisions of the data (Mexico, Peru, Colombia, Chile, North-Central Peninsular, and Southern/Insular Peninsular) used here are also problematic in that identifying dialects according to seemingly arbitrary borders corresponding with specific countries could be misleading when considering specific features of a given variety. For example, considering the speech of Mexico to be one singular “variety” is troublesome since this area is so vast, encompassing multiple varieties with distinctive features within one so-called “variety”, not to mention that the current data set includes data from only one single speaker of Mexican Spanish. This is also true of smaller regions that have differing characteristics within a smaller area, such as the Canary Islands or coastal vs. inland Colombia. For the study, North-Central Peninsular Spanish and Southern Peninsular and Insular (i.e., Canary Islands) Spanish were separated into two distinct regions; however, the results indicated that the Southern Peninsular was not radically different from the Northern Peninsular varieties. Expansion of this preliminary exploration of the effects of region on stop consonant lenition should take into account these limitations.

6. Conclusions

This pilot study’s approach to analyzing voiceless and voiced stop consonants in multiple varieties of Spanish has contributed valuable insights into stop consonant variability across the Spanish-speaking world. However, the results have raised additional questions that are valuable for continued work on stop consonant lenition variability in Spanish and offer many avenues for further research on this topic: expansion and more detailed analysis of sociolinguistic variables that affect the degree of consonant lenition, expansion of the participant pool, improved distribution of the data set, and perceptual tasks both inter- and intra-dialectally to determine comprehensibility and identification of underlying segments. This research raises the following question: how does the allophonic overlap and variability between voiceless and voiced stop consonants affect their perception within each variety and between speakers of the same and different varieties? How will perception vary according to listeners of the same and different varieties? While context has been shown to be a determining factor in the correct identification of underlying segments by interlocutors (Martínez Celdrán 2009), if the context were ambiguous or removed completely, it is possible that speakers would incorrectly identify underlying segments. The incorrect identification of underlying segments is evidence of language change in progress, or at the very least, evidence of the different stages of consonant lenition of different varieties of Spanish. Therefore, an important next step in the study of consonant lenition would be to create perceptual tests where participants must identify underlying segments based on recordings that they hear. Executing these perceptual tests would be two-fold: testing speaker perception of the stop consonants of their own variety and testing speaker perception of the stop consonants of other varieties to determine speaker sensitivity to consonant weakening and comprehensibility.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Wisconsin-Madison (protocol code 2015-0127, approved on 2 March 2015).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are unavailable due to privacy restrictions.

Acknowledgments

I am very grateful to Kyle Parrish for his assistance with the statistical analysis and interpretation of the results for this study; his contributions have undoubtedly made the preliminary results of this study more robust. I would also like to express my gratitude to the anonymous reviewers for providing constructive feedback on an earlier version of this paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Pilot study task. The production of each word was produced in the carrier phrase: Yo digo _____ para ti. Words duplicated from Casteñada Vicente (1986).
Table A1. Pilot study task. The production of each word was produced in the carrier phrase: Yo digo _____ para ti. Words duplicated from Casteñada Vicente (1986).
Voiceless Occlusive
Stressed Position
Voiceless Occlusive
Unstressed Position
Voiced Occlusive
Stressed Position
Voiced Occlusive
Unstressed Position
pasopasóbajobajó
pesopesobesobesó
pillopillóbirlobirló
podopodóbotobotó
punzopunzónbuscobuscó
talotalódatodató
telatelóndejodejó
tiratiródictodictó
tocotocódonodonó
tubotumbódudodudó
casacasóganoganó
quedoquedóguerraguerrero
quitoquitóguisoguisó
cobrocobrógozogozó
curacurargustogustó
Table A2. Regression table including coefficients and standard errors for each term in the model.
Table A2. Regression table including coefficients and standard errors for each term in the model.
Fixed EffectsEst.S.E.t val.d.f.p
(Intercept)9.351.645.7223.510.00
RegionColombia2.722.311.1723.510.25
RegionMexico7.073.272.1623.510.04
RegionNorthcentral Spain4.131.962.1123.510.05
RegionPeru5.992.312.5923.480.02
RegionSouthern Insular Spain1.352.160.6223.510.54
Point.of.articulationdental−1.940.86−2.252445.000.02
Point.of.articulationvelar−0.540.86−0.632445.000.53
Sonorityvoiceless12.260.8614.262445.000.00
Stressunstressed−3.950.50−7.952445.000.00
Horizontal.positioncentral1.760.732.402445.000.02
Horizontal.positionposterior0.010.550.022445.000.99
Vertical.Positionmid1.270.552.302445.000.02
RegionColombia:Point.of.articulationdental3.621.221.982445.000.00
RegionMexico:Point.of.articulationdental0.601.720.352445.000.73
RegionNorthcentral Spain:Point.of.articulationdental2.191.032.132445.000.03
RegionPeru:Point.of.articulationdental0.701.220.582445.000.56
RegionSouthern Insular Spain: Point.of.articulationdental1.061.140.942445.000.35
RegionColombia:Point.of.articulationvelar3.831.223.152445.000.00
RegionMexico:Point.of.articulationvelar2.151.721.252445.000.21
RegionNorthcentral Spain:Point.of.articulationvelar0.891.030.872445.000.39
RegionPeru:Point.of.articulationvelar1.051.220.862445.000.39
RegionSouthern Insular Spain:Point.of.articulationvelar−1.641.14−1.442445.000.15
RegionColombia:Sonorityvoiceless3.061.222.512445.000.01
RegionMexico:Sonorityvoiceless−5.801.72−3.372445.000.00
RegionNorthcentral Spain:Sonorityvoiceless−1.451.03−1.412445.000.16
RegionPeru:Sonorityvoiceless−2.211.22−1.822445.000.07
RegionSouthern Insular Spain:Sonorityvoiceless−1.621.14−1.432445.000.15
RegionColombia:Stressunstressed0.810.701.162445.000.25
RegionMexico:Stressunstressed−1.080.99−1.092445.000.28
RegionNorthcentral Spain:Stressunstressed−0.380.59−0.642445.000.52
RegionPeru:Stressedunstressed−0.590.70−0.852445.000.40
RegionSouthern Insular Spain:Stressunstressed0.210.660.312445.000.75
RegionColombia:horizontal.positioncentral1.081.041.042445.000.30
RegionMexico:Horizontal.positioncentral1.821.471.242445.000.22
RegionNorthcentral Spain: Horizontal.positioncentral0.420.880.482445.000.63
RegionPeur:Horizontal.positioncentral1.511.041.462445.000.15
RegionSouthern Insular Spain: horizontal.positioncentral0.160.970.162445.000.87
RegionColombia:Horizontal.positionposterior1.040.781.322445.000.19
RegionMexico:Horizontal.positionposterior2.291.112.072445.000.04
RegionNorcentral Spain:Horzontal.positionposterior0.610.660.932445.000.35
RegionPeru:horizontal.positionposterior0.940.781.202445.000.23
RegionSouthern Insular Spain: Horizontal.positionposterior0.340.730.472445.000.64
RegionColombia:Vertical.positionmid0.960.781.22445.000.22
RegionMexico:Vertical.Positionmid0.821.110.732445.000.46
RegionNortncentral Spain:Vertical.Positionmid0.380.660.572445.000.57
RegionPeru:Vertical.Positionmid1.460.781.862445.000.06
RegionSouthern Insular Spain:Vertical.Positionmid0.640.730.872445.000.38
RegionChile:Point.of.articulationdental:Sonorityvoiceless1.111.220.912445.000.36
RegionColombia:Point.of.articulationdental:Sonorityvoiceless−2.311.22−1.902445.000.06
RegionMexico:Point.of.articulationdental:Sonorityvoiceless1.432.110.682445.000.50
RegionNorthcentral Spain:Point.of.articulationdental:Sonorityvoiceless−0.100.80−0.122445.000.90
RegionPeru:Point.of.articulationdental:Sonorityvoiceless−0.471.22−0.382445.000.70
RegionSouthern Insular Spain:Point.of.articulationdental:Sonorityvoiceless1.411.051.342445.000.18
RegionChile:Point.of.articulationvelar:Sonorityvoiceless0.381.220.312445.000.75
RegionColombia:Point.of.articulationvelar:Sonorityvoiceless−4.651.22−3.822445.000.00
RegionMexico:Point.of.articulationvelar:Sonorityvoiceless−1.852.11−0.882445.000.38
RegionNortcentral Spain:Point.of.articulationvelar:Sonorityvoiceless−1.510.80−1.902445.000.06
RegionPeru:Point.of.articulationvelar:Sonorityvoiceless−3.231.22−2.662445.000.01
RegionSouthern Insular Spain:Point.of.articulationvelar:Sonorityvoiceless1.401.051.332445.000.18

Notes

1
Eddington (2011) is unique in his assumption that the approximant productions /βðɣ/ are the underlying, abstract mental representations, and the stop productions [bdɡ] represent possible allophonic realizations, contrary to traditional phonological analyses (Hualde 2005).
2
The participants in the study did not complete a language dominance survey to show that they are all still Spanish dominant; however, in my personal communications with each of the participants, they expressed their continued use of Spanish in their daily lives, either at home, at work, or both despite having spent extended time away from their home country. For future work on this topic, the inclusion of a language dominance survey, such as the Bilingual Language Profile (BLP) (Birdsong et al. 2012), would be used to improve the quality of the study.

References

  1. Acosta-Martínez, Junice Altagracia. 2014. Phonological Variation in Cibaeño Spanish: Social Networks as Potential Predictors of Semi-Vocalization (Version 1). Chicago: University of Illinois at Chicago. Available online: https://hdl.handle.net/10027/19001 (accessed on 17 July 2021).
  2. Alarcos Llorach, Emilio. 1961. Fonología Española, 3rd ed. Madrid: Gredos. [Google Scholar]
  3. Alba, Orlando. 1988. Estudio sociolingüístico de la variación de las líquidas finales de palabra en el español cibaeño. In Studies in Caribbean Spanish Dialectology. Edited by Robert M. Hammond and Melvyn Resnick. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bayley, Robert, and Elane Tarone. 2011. Variationist perspectives. In The Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Edited by Susan M. Gass and Alison Mackey. New York: Routledge, pp. 41–56. Available online: https://www.taylorfrancis.com/chapters/edit/10.4324/9780203808184-5/variationist-perspectives-robert-bayley-elaine-tarone (accessed on 21 June 2021).
  5. Birdsong, David, Libby M. Gertken, and Mark Amengual. 2012. Bilingual Language Profile: An Easy-To-Use Instrument to Assess Bilingualism. Austin: COERLL, University of Texas at Austin. Available online: https://sites.la.utexas.edu/bilingual/ (accessed on 20 January 2012).
  6. Boersma, Paul, and David Weenink. 2013. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer. Available online: http://www.praat.org (accessed on 16 April 2016).
  7. Browman, Catherine, and Louis Goldstein. 1992. Articulatory phonology: An overview. Phonetica 49: 155–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Carrasco, Patricio, José Ignacio Hualde, and Miquel Simonet. 2012. Dialectal differences in Spanish obstruent allophony: Costa Rican versus Iberian Spanish. Phonetica 69: 149–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Casteñada Vicente, María Luisa. 1986. El V.O.T. de las consonantes sordas y sonoras españolas. Estudios de Fonética Experimental II: 92–110. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cedergren, Henrietta J., and David Sankoff. 1974. Variable rules: Performance as a Statistical Reflection of Competence. Language 50: 333–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Cho, Taehon, James M. McQueen, and Ethan A. Cox. 2006. Prosodically driven phonetic detail in speech processing: The case of domain-initial strengthening in English. Journal of Phonetics 35: 210–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Colantoni, Laura, and Irina Marinescu. 2010. The scope of stop weakening in Argentine Spanish. In Selected Proceedings of the 4th Conference on Laboratory Approaches to Spanish Phonology. Edited by Marta Ortega-Llebaria. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 100–14. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cole, Jennifer, José Ignacio Hualde, and Khalil Iskarous. 1999. Effect of prosodic context on /g/ lenition in Spanish. In Proceedings of the 4th International Linguistics and Phonetics Conference. Edited by Osamu Fujimura, Brian D. Joseph and Bohumil Palek. Prague: The Karolinum Press, pp. 575–89. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cravens, Thomas D. 2002. Comparative Historical Dialectology: Italo-Romance Clues to Ibero-Romance Sound Change. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
  15. Eddington, David. 2011. What are the contextual variants of [β ð ɣ] in colloquial Spanish? Probus 23: 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Figueroa, Mauricio, and Bronwen G. Evans. 2014. Cue weighting of acoustic variables in the perception of Chilean Spanish approximant consonants. Paper presented at the 2014 Meeting of the British Association of Academic Phoneticians, Oxford, UK, April 7–9. [Google Scholar]
  17. Gili Fivela, Barbara, Sonia d’ Apolito, Antonio Stella, and Francesco Sigona. 2008. Domain initial strengthening in sentences and paragraphs: Preliminary findings on the production of voiced bilabial plosives in two varieties of Italian. Paper presented at 8th International Seminar on Speech Production, Strasbourg, France, December 8–12; Edited by Rudoph Sock, Susanne Fuchs and Yves Laprie. Strasbourg: The National Institute for Research in Computer Science and Control (INRIA), pp. 205–8. [Google Scholar]
  18. Hualde, José Ignacio. 2005. The Sounds of Spanish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  19. Hualde, José Ignacio, Miquel Simonet, and Marianna Nadeu. 2011. Consonant lenition and phonological recategorization. Journal of Laboratory Phonology 2: 301–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Jungemann, Frederick H. 1955. La teoría del Sustrato y los Dialectos Hispano-Romances y Gascones. Translated by Emilio Alarcos Llorach. Madrid: Gredos. [Google Scholar]
  21. Keating, Patricia, Taehong Cho, Cécile Fougeron, and Chai-Shune Hsu. 2003. Domain-initial articulatory strengthening in four languages. In Phonetic Interpretation (Papers in Laboratory Phonology 6). Edited by John Local, Richard Ogden and Rosalind Temple. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 143–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Kirchner, Robert. 2004. Consonant lenition. In Phonetically Based Phonology. Edited by Bruce Hayes, Robert Kirchner and Donca Steriade. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 313–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Labov, William. 1966. The Social Stratification of English in New York City. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics. [Google Scholar]
  24. Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. [Google Scholar]
  25. Labov, William. 2001. Principles of Linguistic Change, Volume 2: Social Factors. Malden: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lass, Roger. 1984. Phonology: An Introduction to Basic Concepts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Lavoie, Lisa. 2001. Consonant Strength: Phonological Patterns and Phonetic Manifestations. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lewis, Anthony. 2001. Weakening of Intervocalic /p, t, k/ in Two Spanish Dialects: Toward the Quantification of Lenition Processes. Unpublished. Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, USA. [Google Scholar]
  29. Lisker, Leigh, and Arthur S. Abramson. 1964. A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word 20: 384–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Malaver, Irania, and José Antonio Samper Padilla. 2016. Estudio de la /d/ intervocálica en los corpus PRESEEA. Boletín de filología 51: 325–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Martinet, André. 1952. Celtic lenition and Western Romance consonants. Language 28: 192–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Martínez Celdrán, Eugenio. 2009. Sonorización de las oclusivas sordas en una hablante murciana: Problemas que plantea. Estudios de Fonética Experiemental 18: 253–71. [Google Scholar]
  33. O’Neill, Paul. 2010. Variación y cambio en las consonantes oclusivas del español de Andalucía. Estudios de Fonética Experimental 19: 11–41. [Google Scholar]
  34. Oftedal, Magne. 1985. Lenition in Celtic and in Insular Spanish. (=Monographs in Celtic Studies from the University of Oslo, 2). Oslo: Universitetsforlaget. [Google Scholar]
  35. Ortega-Llebaria, Marta. 2004. Interplay between phonetic and inventory constraints in the degree of spirantization of voiced stops: Comparing intervocalic /b/ and intervocalic /g/ in Spanish and English. In Laboratory Approaches to Spanish Phonology. Edited by Timothy Face. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 237–53. [Google Scholar]
  36. Pérez, Hernán Emilio. 2007. Estudio de la variación estilística de la serie /bdg/ en posición intervocálica en el habla de los noticieros de la television chilena. Estudios de fonética experimental 16: 228–59. [Google Scholar]
  37. Poplack, Shana. 1978. Dialect Acquisition among Puerto Rican Bilinguals. Language in Society 7: 89–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Rogers, Brandon M. A., and Christina A. Mirisis. 2018. Voiceless stop and reduction and linguistic and social phenomena in Concepción, Chile. Borealis: An International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics 7: 187–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Rojas, Nelson. 1981. Sobre la semivocalización de las líquidas en el español cibaeño. In El español del Caribe: Ponencias del VI Simposio de Dialectología. Edited by Orlando Alba. Santiago: Universidad Católica Madre y Maestra, pp. 271–87. [Google Scholar]
  40. Tovar, Antonio. 1948. La sonorización y caída de las intervocálicas, y los estratos indoeuropeos en Hispania. Boletín de la Real Academia Española 28: 265–80. [Google Scholar]
  41. Trujillo, Ramón. 1980. Lenguaje y cultura en Masca. Santa Cruz de Tenerife: Editorial Interinsular Canaria—Instituto Andrés Bello. [Google Scholar]
  42. Veiga, Alexandre Rodríguez. 1988. Reaproximación estructural a la lenición protoromance. Verba 15: 17–78. [Google Scholar]
  43. Wireback, Kenneth J. 1997. The Role of Phonological Structure in Sound Change from Latin to Spanish and Portuguese. New York: Peter Lang. [Google Scholar]
  44. Yeni-Komshian, Grace H., James E. Flege, and Serena Liu. 2000. Pronunciation proficiency in the first and second languages of Korean-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3: 131–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Intensity curve of the phrase ‘Yo digo podo para ti’ ‘I say ‘I prune’ for you’.
Figure 1. Intensity curve of the phrase ‘Yo digo podo para ti’ ‘I say ‘I prune’ for you’.
Languages 08 00224 g001
Figure 2. Intensity curve of the phrase ‘Yo digo busco para ti’ ‘I say ‘I look for’ for you’.
Figure 2. Intensity curve of the phrase ‘Yo digo busco para ti’ ‘I say ‘I look for’ for you’.
Languages 08 00224 g002
Figure 3. RI of /b/ in the phrase ‘Yo digo buscó para ti’ ‘I say ‘he looked for’ for you’.
Figure 3. RI of /b/ in the phrase ‘Yo digo buscó para ti’ ‘I say ‘he looked for’ for you’.
Languages 08 00224 g003
Figure 4. Predicted RI values of three-way interaction between region, point of articulation, and sonority.
Figure 4. Predicted RI values of three-way interaction between region, point of articulation, and sonority.
Languages 08 00224 g004
Figure 5. Intensity curve of the phrase Yo digo peso para ti ‘I say weight for you’ produced by a speaker from Medellin, Colombia (speaker Col1).
Figure 5. Intensity curve of the phrase Yo digo peso para ti ‘I say weight for you’ produced by a speaker from Medellin, Colombia (speaker Col1).
Languages 08 00224 g005
Figure 6. Intensity curve of the phrase Yo digo peso para ti ‘I say weight for you’ produced by a speaker from Málaga, Spain (speaker SSp4).
Figure 6. Intensity curve of the phrase Yo digo peso para ti ‘I say weight for you’ produced by a speaker from Málaga, Spain (speaker SSp4).
Languages 08 00224 g006
Figure 7. Predicted RI values of the two-way interaction between region and sonority.
Figure 7. Predicted RI values of the two-way interaction between region and sonority.
Languages 08 00224 g007
Figure 8. Predicted RI values of the three-way interaction between stress, sonority, and region.
Figure 8. Predicted RI values of the three-way interaction between stress, sonority, and region.
Languages 08 00224 g008
Table 1. Sonority scale in Spanish (Hualde 2005, p. 72).
Table 1. Sonority scale in Spanish (Hualde 2005, p. 72).
Sonority ScaleVowel/Consonant GroupsSegments
6low vowela
5mid vowelse o
4high vowelsi u
3liquidsl (ʎ) ɾ r ¯
2nasalsm n ɲ
1obstruentsf (θ) s x ʝ tʃ b d ɡ p t k
Table 2. Detailed participant demographics.
Table 2. Detailed participant demographics.
SpeakerGenderAgeOriginYears in USASpanish Variety
Col1F28Medellin3.5Colombian
Col2F46Bogota14Colombian
Col3F35Cordoba, Barranquilla7Colombian
Mex1F36Sonora8Mexican
Chi1F29Santiago7Chilean
Chi2F43Santiago15Chilean
Chi3F22Antofagasta0.33Chilean
Per2F49Lima22.5Peruvian
Per3M29Lima3Peruvian
NSp1M59Ponferrada28North-Central Peninsular
NSp2F34Lugo4North-Central Peninsular
NSp3F24Valladolid0.5North-Central Peninsular
NSp5F54Salamanca25North-Central Peninsular
NSp6F47Madrid15North-Central Peninsular
SSp1F29Grand Canary Island1.5Southern/Insular Peninsular
SSp2M34Mallorca4Southern/Insular Peninsular
SSp3M35Granada6Southern/Insular Peninsular
SSp4F52Malaga26Southern/Insular Peninsular
Table 3. Relative intensity measurements (dB) according to Spanish variety and phoneme.
Table 3. Relative intensity measurements (dB) according to Spanish variety and phoneme.
Spanish Variety/p//t//k//b//d//ɡ/
Colombian27.126.527.512.414.115.7
Peruvian24.424.522.415.214.916.0
Mexican22.922.822.612.712.913.0
North-Central Peninsular23.523.322.612.712.913.9
Southern/Insular Peninsular20.821.320.010.19.38.0
Chilean20.519.720.48.26.37.7
Table 4. Decibel distinction between /ptk/ and /bdɡ/.
Table 4. Decibel distinction between /ptk/ and /bdɡ/.
Spanish Variety/ptk//SD/bdɡ//SDIntDiff
Colombian27.1/2.5614/2.8913.1
Peruvian23.8/4.5615/0.458.8
Mexican22.8/0.0016.5/0.006.3
North-Central Peninsular23.2/2.4112.9/3.5910.3
Southern/Insular Peninsular21.5/4.689.1/1.8812.4
Chilean20.2/1.307.4/3.1412.8
Table 5. Relative intensity (dB) measurements according to Spanish variety and lexical stress.
Table 5. Relative intensity (dB) measurements according to Spanish variety and lexical stress.
Spanish VarietyStressedUnstressedIntDiff
Colombian22.1193.1
Peruvian21.617.14.5
Mexican22.117.15.0
North-Central Peninsular20.115.84.3
Southern/Insular Peninsular17.911.96.9
Chilean15.811.84.0
Table 6. Relative intensity (dB) measurements according to Spanish variety and lexical stress for /ptk/.
Table 6. Relative intensity (dB) measurements according to Spanish variety and lexical stress for /ptk/.
Spanish Variety/ptk/ Stressed/ptk/ UnstressedIntDiff
Colombian28.925.23.7
Peruvian25.721.93.8
Mexican25.719.95.8
North-Central Peninsular25.221.14.1
Southern/Insular Peninsular23.817.46.4
Chilean21.918.43.5
Table 7. Relative intensity (dB) measurements according to Spanish variety and lexical stress for /bdɡ/.
Table 7. Relative intensity (dB) measurements according to Spanish variety and lexical stress for /bdɡ/.
Spanish Variety/bdɡ/ Stressed/bdɡ/ UnstressedIntDiff
Colombian15.412.72.7
Peruvian17.612.35.3
Mexican18.914.34.6
North-Central Peninsular15.110.64.5
Southern/Insular Peninsular11.86.35.4
Chilean9.65.24.4
Table 8. Relative intensity measurements according to vowel position.
Table 8. Relative intensity measurements according to vowel position.
Spanish VarietyAnteriorPosteriorCentral/LowHighMid
Colombian19.820.821.519.221.4
Peruvian18.619.520.517.720.4
Mexican18.220.520.718.320.4
North-Central Peninsular17.518.118.817.018.6
Southern/Insular Peninsular14.614.815.513.815.6
Chilean13.613.614.712.914.2
Table 9. Summary of main findings in the study.
Table 9. Summary of main findings in the study.
Spanish VarietyMain Findings
Region × point of articulationLower RI for /t/ among Colombian and Chilean speakers
Lower RI for /k/ among Peruvian, Mexican, North-Central Peninsular, and Southern/Insular and Peninsular speakers
Higher RI for /b/ among Southern/Insular and Peninsular and Chilean speakers
Higher RI for /ɡ/ than /b/ and /d/ among Colombian, Peruvian, Mexican, and North-Central Peninsular speakers
Region × sonorityLower RI for /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ among Chilean, North-Central Peninsular, and Southern/Insular and Peninsular speakers
Higher RI for /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ among Peruvian, Colombian and Mexican speakers
Region × stressLower RI for /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ when followed by a lexically unstressed syllable as opposed to a lexically stressed syllable, RI does not depend on region
Region × vowelLower RI for high and anterior vowels, RI does not depend on region
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Butera, B. Acoustic Variability of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ in Spanish: A Pilot Study. Languages 2023, 8, 224. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030224

AMA Style

Butera B. Acoustic Variability of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ in Spanish: A Pilot Study. Languages. 2023; 8(3):224. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030224

Chicago/Turabian Style

Butera, Brianna. 2023. "Acoustic Variability of /ptk/ and /bdɡ/ in Spanish: A Pilot Study" Languages 8, no. 3: 224. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8030224

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop