Next Article in Journal
Interpretations of the Role of Plasma Albumin in Prognostic Indices: A Literature Review
Next Article in Special Issue
Evolving a Model for Cochlear Implant Outcome
Previous Article in Journal
Results of the Italian RESILIEN-T Pilot Study: A Mobile Health Tool to Support Older People with Mild Cognitive Impairment
Previous Article in Special Issue
Triple Semi-Circular Canal Occlusion and Cochlear Implantation: A Treatment Option for Single-Sided Menière’s Disease with Functional Deafness—A Case Series
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Influence of Age on Speech Recognition in Noise and Hearing Effort in Listeners with Age-Related Hearing Loss

Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University Medicine Halle, Ernst-Grube-Straße 40, 06120 Halle (Saale), Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12(19), 6133; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12196133
Submission received: 30 August 2023 / Revised: 20 September 2023 / Accepted: 20 September 2023 / Published: 22 September 2023

Abstract

:
The aim of this study was to measure how age affects the speech recognition threshold (SRT50) of the Oldenburg Sentence Test (OLSA) and the listening effort at the corresponding signal-to-noise ratio (SNRcut). The study also investigated the effect of the spatial configuration of sound sources and noise signals on SRT50 and SNRcut. To achieve this goal, the study used olnoise and icra5 noise presented from one or more spatial locations from the front and back. Ninety-nine participants with age-related hearing loss in the 18–80 years age range, specifically in the 18–30, 31–40, 41–50, 51–60, 61–70, and 71–80 age groups, participated in this study. Speech recognition and listening effort in noise were measured and compared between the different age groups, different spatial sound configurations and noise signals. Speech recognition in noise decreased with age and became significant from the age group of 50–51. The decrease in SRT50 with age was greater for icra5 noise than for olnoise. For all age groups, SRT50 and SNRcut were better for icra5 noise than for olnoise. The measured age-related reference data for SRT50 and SNRcut can be used in further studies in listeners with age-related hearing loss and hearing aid or implant users.

1. Introduction

Human hearing typically deteriorates with age. Regarding the correlation of the age-related decline in outer hair cells and later in inner hair cells, the pure-tone hearing thresholds are negatively affected. In addition to pure-tone hearing loss, speech recognition thresholds in noise increase with age and may be correlated with reduced outer hair cell function [1]. Even individuals with normal pure-tone hearing thresholds (i.e., normal hearing listeners) may experience reduced speech recognition in noise with age [2], which may be caused by cognitive decline. Both peripheral dysfunction and cognitive factors contribute to age-related hearing loss and communication deficits, and are difficult to disentangle.
To assess speech recognition in noisy environments in relation to age, the HÖRSTAT study examined 1903 adults using the Göttingen Sentence Test (GÖSA) and pure-tone audiometry. The study found a gradual decline in pure-tone and speech recognition thresholds (SRTs) as age increases [2]. As part of the UK Biobank, around 500,000 individuals aged 40–69 years were evaluated for speech-in-noise hearing and cognition. The data show that speech-in-noise hearing decreases exponentially with age after the age of approximately 50. This contrasts with earlier audiogram data, which indicate a more linear decline in men under 40 years and consistently less hearing loss in women [3].
The decline in the ability to understand speech in older listeners cannot be solely attributed to the effects of aging on the auditory periphery and cognition [4,5]. Furthermore, cognitive abilities play a significant role in understanding speech in noisy settings [6,7]. It is important to take into account the contributions of central auditory processing at the brainstem and cortical levels. According to the hypothesis, age-related alterations in the balance between inhibitory and excitatory neural mechanisms modify the production of gamma oscillations, thereby influencing perceptual binding [8]. Consequently, speech comprehension in noisy environments is also affected. Among older adults, central auditory processing at the level of sensation, as indicated by sensory gating, has a minimal effect on speech recognition in noise. However, it significantly impacts perceptual organization [8].
Although hearing loss and age have been identified as the main contributors to decreased speech recognition in noisy environments [9], hearing in noise deficits could also start from an early age. There are also non-speech psychoacoustic aspects to consider, like dichotic signal detection, multi-burst masking, stream segregation, and modulation detection influencing speech recognition in noise, even when hearing aids are employed [10]. In a study carried out on young and elderly people with normal hearing conditions, Füllgrabe and Moore [11] proved that there is a strong correlation between the understanding of speech in noisy environments and sensitivity to temporal fine structures.
Speech perception in noise can be assessed using matrix sentence tests [12]. The Oldenburg Sentence Test (OLSA, HoerTech, Oldenburg, Germany) is a commonly used matrix test available for several languages, and it is accessible for clinical use [12,13,14]. It enables the evaluation of speech intelligibility in noisy scenarios for people with different levels of hearing loss. It includes a variety of noise signals that can be utilised to generate complex listening conditions with multiple sources of speech and noise. Various noise signals are utilised in clinical practice and research to measure speech reception thresholds (SRTs). Noise that has the same spectrum as speech (e.g., the olnoise employed in the OLSA) can provide effective masking. The SRT measured in fluctuating noise can be higher than that measured in stationary noise [15]. Weißgerber et al. [16] conducted a study on amplitude-modulated Fastl noise and assessed the speech recognition of a group of bimodal cochlear implant users, comparing their results to age-matched hearing aid users, individuals without subjective hearing loss, and a young normal hearing control group. The researchers observed that speech recognition in modulated noise was more considerably impacted than in continuous noise with an increase in hearing loss. In this study, olnoise generated by a male speaker and icra5 noise, which simulates speech with pauses, were used. The icra5 noise was originally developed using live English speech from the EUROM database by the International Collegium of Rehabilitative Audiology (ICRA) [17].
In clinical practice, speech recognition in noisy situations is measured using various parameters. The number and spatial positioning of speech and noise sources, as well as the type of speech material and adaptive procedures used, can all vary between measurements of speech recognition in noise [18,19,20]. This makes it difficult to compare results, due to the absence of reference data for individuals with normal age-related hearing across different age groups. For the first time, Decambron et al.’s study [21] has presented objective signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) values for varied age cohorts. The study assessed the 50% speech recognition threshold (SRT50) in noise via the ‘Vocale Rapide dans le Bruit’ test among 200 patients. The exam incorporated five noise sources with frontal speech presentation. The median values for SRT ranged from −0.37 dB (SNR) for those aged between 20–30 years to 6.84 dB (SNR) for individuals above 70 years of age. Mukari et al. [22] conducted a study on speech recognition in noisy environments using the Hearing in Noise Test (HINT). The researchers compared the Speech Reception Threshold (SRT) values obtained from three different noise sources: frontal, right and left. The study found evidence of an age-related decline in speech recognition ability, with older adults demonstrating poorer performance. Additionally, the research indicates that hearing thresholds have a significant impact on speech recognition ability in quiet conditions for older adults. However, it indicates that additional factors, including central auditory processing and cognitive abilities, could be more significant determinants in speech recognition performance in noisy environments.
In addition to assessing speech perception in noisy conditions, it is also important to evaluate listening effort. Active listening requires cognitive resources such as focus and attention. Decreased speech levels in quiet environments or the Signal-to-Noise Ratio (SNR) in noisy situations increase the demand on cognitive resources [23]. It is anticipated that age-related drops in pure-tone thresholds or hearing deficits beyond will lead to heightened listening effort, which may subsequently impact numerous daily communication scenarios with greater demands [23]. Listening effort increases with age regardless of hearing sensitivity [24]. According to the study by Kwak and Han [25], older adults experience a greater amount of listening effort compared to younger people as a result of background noise, directionality and ageing. A direct correlation was found between participants’ listening effort and their working memory and processing speed performance whilst understanding speech in background noise. Thus, it can be inferred that older adults require more cognitive resources to comprehend speech in such conditions [26].
Listening effort can be measured objectively by pupillometry. However, it was observed that pupil dilatation was less affected by SNR variation than subjective measures of listening effort [27]. The Adaptive Categorical Listening Effort Scaling Test (ACALES) is a subjective clinical procedure that has recently emerged offering an excellent means of measuring listening effort in noisy environments [24]. The sentential stimuli are presented alongside varying levels of background noise, quantified by signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs). The evaluation of listening effort is based on subjective responses and utilises either a 7-point or 14-point categorical scale that spans from a ‘no effort’ response to one indicating ‘extreme effort’. The SNRs of ACALES are adjusted presentation-by-presentation, based on prior subjective ratings. This guarantees comprehensive coverage across all possible categories and ultimately determines the moderate-level listening effort SNRcut [26,28]. ACALES has been effectively employed to measure listening effort in individuals with hearing implants within a challenging acoustic environment [18,20].
This study aimed to evaluate the influence of age on the SRT50 of the Oldenburg sentence test (OLSA). For the first time, reference values for different age groups will be provided, including different spatial speech and noise configurations as well as noise signals. Noise will be presented in front (S0N0; reference and training condition) or behind (S0N180), as well as in a semicircle in front (S0N0,45,−45) or behind (S0N135,180,−135) of the participant. The study also examined for the first time how age affects listening effort when hearing in noisy environments.

2. Materials and Methods

This study consisted of exploratory cohorts and was prospective and non-interventional. It focused on adult volunteers with age- and sex-related pure-tone thresholds. The subjects were recruited in Halle (Saale), Germany, through personal contacts of the authors. The inclusion criteria required age-related hearing in both ears, fluency in German (native speakers), and to be between 18 and 80 years of age. Age-related hearing was confirmed when the bilateral pure-tone thresholds for air-conduction, averaged over 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz (4PTA), were not worse than the age- and sex-related 95th percentile of the ISO 7026 [29] and there was symmetry between both ears. The Freiburg Monosyllables Test was used to measure the Word Recognition Score (WRS) of both ears, at a sound pressure level of 65 dB via headphones. Audiological evaluations were performed using an AT100 audiometer (Auritec, Hamburg, Germany).
Participants were excluded if they were unable to visit the study site, lacked fluency in the German language, demonstrated a lack of comprehension of the study procedures (also to ensure mental health), were pregnant, or did not meet the inclusion criteria. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants for their participation in the study. The study was conducted at the Audiology Lab of University Medicine Halle, Germany, after receiving ethical approval from the Medical Faculty of the Martin Luther University Halle-Wittenberg (approval number 2021-044) and was conducted in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
The participants were selected from six age categories (18–30, 31–40, 41–50, 51–60, 61–70, 71–80 years). The sample size was calculated assuming an alpha of 0.05 and a power of 80%. The slope of the speech discrimination function at the reflection point, i.e., the SRT50, was about 17% per dB [13]. For sample size estimation in this study, a difference of at least 2 dB (SD = 2 dB) was considered necessary to account for a relevant SRT50 difference. Therefore, each age required at least 16 participants in the sample size.
For testing speech recognition in noisy environments, the German Matrix Sentence Test OLSA (Hörtech, Oldenburg, Germany) was used, which was played through four loudspeakers. Signal generation and presentation were conducted using the Oldenburger Measurement Application 2.2 R&D software from Hörtech in Oldenburg, Germany, along with a Gigaport eX audio interface from ESI Audiotechnik in Leonberg, Germany, and a PLMRA400 amplifier from Pyle in Brooklyn, NY, USA. Continuous noise signals were presented at a sound pressure level of 65 dB. The stimuli utilised were either the generic noise of the Oldenburg Logatome Speech Corpus with a male voice (olnoise) or the icra5 noise. Lists of 20 sentences were presented in front (S0N0; reference and training condition) or behind (S0N180), as well as in a semicircle in front (S0N0,45,−45) or behind (S0N135,180,−135), of the participant, all at a distance of 1 metre from the head centre (refer to Figure 1). In clinical practice, many different spatial configurations of speech and noise sources are used. Since many hearing aid or cochlear implant algorithms aim for a better understanding of speech coming from the front and to mitigate noise coming from frontal and other directions, we focus on speech coming from the front in this study. This is also relevant for the elderly who are to be included in this study. For each sentence, the sound pressure level was adjusted based on the participant’s response to the preceding sentence. This technique was performed to measure the open-set speech recognition threshold (SRT) for 50% correct recognition (SRT50), which was the primary endpoint for the S0N0 condition using olnoise, as well as a secondary endpoint for all other spatial conditions and the icra5 noise.
Listening effort was assessed utilizing ACALES v2.2 software (Hörtech, Oldenburg, Germany). The participants received a sequence of two consecutive sentences from the OLSA, with diverse signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs), presented frontally, in a background noise continuously played at 65 dB SPL (either olnoise or icra5) either from the frontal (S0N0) or rear (S0N180) loudspeaker after two training runs. Following the presentation of two OLSA sentences in noise, participants were instructed to rate their listening effort on a scale of eight response categories, ranging from ‘no effort’ to ‘only noise’. The signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) was changed adaptively for every round of two sentences, based on the previous assessment of the subjectively perceived listening effort, and was presented at a different SNR ranging between −40 dB and +20 dB. A listening effort function was computed by the software. The SNRcut, which represents the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) at which a moderate effort was measured (called SNRcut), was assessed as a secondary endpoint for all experimental conditions by identifying the cutting point at 4 ESCU.
All the participants were seated and their heads were immobilised with a Papillon head fixation system (Focal Meditech, Tilburg, The Netherlands). After completing two OLSA and two ACALES training sessions, all participants accomplished 15 testing runs in which the noise signals and spatial conditions were applied in a pseudorandom sequence.
The primary and secondary endpoints were analysed descriptively in this exploratory study and normality was assessed using the Shapiro–Wilk test. An ANOVA for repeated measures was used to compare the distributions of SRT50 and SNRcut across both the ‘noise signal’ and ‘spatial configuration’ experimental conditions, as well as the ‘age group’ factor among subjects. Mauchly’s test was used to verify the assumption of sphericity. The Bonferroni correction was employed to adjust degrees of freedom for all post hoc comparisons. Statistical analyses were conducted using version 28 of the SPSS software (IBM, Ehningen, Germany). Linear regression analysis was performed to measure the influence of the variables ‘age’ and ‘binaurally averaged PTA’ on the SRT50 and SNRcut.

3. Results

All participants successfully completed the experiment. The demographic data of participants from all age groups are presented in Table 1. Figure 2 provides a comparison of SRT50 and SNRcut across all spatial configurations of signal and noise sources for all participants. All spatial conditions demonstrated a better SRT50 and reduced listening effort for icra5 noise compared to olnoise.
The box plots in Figure 3 depict the SRT50 distributions across all age groups. Table 2 provides the specific means and standard deviations for all conditions. The ANOVA conducted on the SRT50 distribution demonstrated significant main effects of noise signal (F(1,89) = 1730.8, p < 0.001), spatial configuration (F(2.41,223.5) = 488.5, p < 0.001) and the between-subjects factor of age group (F(5,89) = 270.3, p < 0.001). Interactions between the noise signal and age group (F(5) = 18.3, p < 0.001), spatial configuration and age group (F(12.6) = 2.1, p < 0.01), noise signal and spatial configuration (F(2.43,216.6) = 738.9, p < 0.001) and noise signal and spatial configuration × age group (F(12.2) = 3.35, p < 0.001) were also significant. The post hoc comparison showed a better (lower) SRT50 for icra5 noise than for olnoise overall and in all age groups. The mean SRT50 was lowest (best) for the S0N180 presentation, significantly different between all spatial configurations with an increase to S0N180,135,−135, S0N0 and S0N0,45,−45. The post hoc comparison showed a significant increase between the age groups 18–50 and 61–80 years when olnoise was used. However, when icra5 noise was used, the difference was significant between the age groups 18–30 years, 51–80 years and between the age groups 31–50 years and 61–80 years.
Figure 4 shows the SNRcut distributions for all age distributions, used noise signals and spatial distributions. The specific means and standard deviations are summarised in Table 2. The ANOVA of the SNRcut distribution revealed significant main effects of noise signal (F(1,92) = 266.9, p < 0.001), spatial configuration (F(1,92) = 228.2, p < 0.001), but not for the between-subjects factor of age group. Only the interaction between noise signal and spatial configuration was significant (F(1,92) = 8.12, p < 0.01). The post hoc comparison showed a lower SNRcut for icra5 noise than for olnoise overall and for all spatial configurations. The mean SNRcut was better for the S0N180 presentation compared to the S0N0 condition.
Table 3 lists the linear regression results for the variables of age and the binaural PTA. For the S0N0 condition with olnoise, a significant linear regression was observed between age and SRT50, and age and SNRcut. The linear regression between age and speech perception in noise was also significant for the S0N0,45,−45 conditions, but not for all other conditions. Binaurally averaged PTA was correlated with speech perception in noise for all conditions except for the S0N0 condition. The linear regression between binaural PTA and listening effort in noise was not significant.

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to provide age-related reference signal-to-noise ratios at which 50% speech recognition in noise (SRT50) and moderate listening effort (SNRcut) are assessed by participants. A decline in speech recognition in noise was observed with increasing age. This decrease was significant from the age group of 51–60 years and above. The decrease in SRT50 with age was greater when icra5 noise was used than when olnoise was used. Overall, speech could be discriminated at lower SNRs with icra5 noise than with olnoise. Since icra5 allows for listening into gaps in the noise signal (‘gap listening’ [30]), this finding is consistent with previous results using other speech-modulated noise signals [16,31,32]. For olnoise, the decrease from 18–30 to 71–80 years was smallest for the S0N0,45,−45 condition (1.6 dB) and largest for the S0N180,135,−135 condition (2.12 dB). Using icra5, this difference was also smallest for the S0N0,45,−45 condition (4.46 dB), but largest for the S0N0 condition (6.93 dB). The spatial configuration itself also influenced the age-related SRT50 and was observed in all age groups. When speech was presented from the front and noise from the back, the best SRT50 was observed for both noise signals used. This is consistent with spatial release from masking due to the separation of speech and noise sources [33,34]. However, the measured improvements are still smaller than in a similar study by Decambron et al. [21].
For olnoise, the SRT50 was −6.64 dB (SD: 0.75) for the 18–30-year-olds, which is within the range originally reported for the OLSA matrix test [13] and later research [14]. Adding more noise sources improved speech recognition in noise, as the spatial release of masking was increased by adding more correlated noise signal. However, in icra5, the addition of two more noise sources decreased speech intelligibility and thus worsened the SRT50. This finding may be due to the uncorrelated noise signals used in this condition.
Listening effort in noise was influenced by the noise signal used. Speech recognition in icra5 was rated as less demanding, resulting in a lower (better) SNRcut compared to the olnoise conditions. Apparently, the gaps in the speech-modulated noise resulted in better speech recognition, which was rated as less demanding. The presentation of speech and noise from separate sources resulted in less listening effort. No age effect was observed for all spatial and noise conditions.
The observed standard deviations of the SNRcut were larger than those of the SRT50. This may be due to the difficulty of the task. Although the ACALES task was explained in detail, it cannot be excluded that some participants focused more on the speech recognition rating than on the actual listening effort. In such cases, a different internal scale would have been used. The resolution of the scale, with only seven levels, may also have contributed to the larger standard deviation.
Age correlates with increased pure-tone thresholds. It is therefore difficult to disentangle the influence of both factors on speech perception and listening effort in noise. In this study, the influence of the binaurally averaged PTA on both endpoints was assessed by regression analysis. As expected, a correlation between this PTA and the SRT50 was observed for most conditions. For the clinically relevant condition of S0N0 presentation of noise and speech, this correlation was not significant, but the effect of age was. We conclude that both age and PTA may influence speech perception in noise, at least partially independently. However, the study design, especially the inclusion criteria, has limitations for such an analysis. At younger ages, only participants with good PTA thresholds were included, whereas participants with poorer PTA were only included at older ages. This is reflected in the larger standard deviations and percentiles in the older age groups. This means that younger participants with increased PTA were excluded from the study, which limits a sufficient analysis of the influence of PTA. Also, a specific analysis of high frequency hearing loss, which may correlate with speech-in-noise difficulties, was not performed in this study. Both pure-tone thresholds and age are correlated, but are not the only factors in speech recognition in noise that decline with age. However, many of these factors have to be taken into account when age-related reference values are provided for different age groups.
As speech recognition in noise and listening effort are measured in clinical routines with different spatial configurations and noise signals, this study provides reference data for normal hearing listeners of all ages. The reference values will be useful for comparisons with patient groups such as those with idiopathic sudden hearing loss [35]. In addition to speech recognition in quiet conditions, improved hearing in noise conditions is one of the goals of cochlear implantation and can be used as an indication criterion [36,37]. Especially in patients with unilateral deafness, hearing in noise can be significantly improved by a cochlear implant [38,39,40,41,42] and may be cost-effective compared to no intervention or other interventions [43]. A study of patients with different indications for speech recognition in quiet and noise conditions showed that those patients undergoing cochlear implant candidacy testing who qualified only by a poor hearing in noise had improved hearing in both quiet and noise conditions [44]. As these effects may also be caused by the pre-processing technology in cochlear implants, hearing aid or active middle-ear implant users, different spatial configurations will be required to demonstrate benefits. The age-related reference data reported here may also serve as a benchmark in these cases.
The study results are limited to the German language, as only German-speaking participants were included. In addition, other spatial configurations of speech and noise as well as different noise signals are not covered by the study results. The study did not specifically analyse frequency-specific pure-tone thresholds, which may have differed between age groups.
In conclusion, this study provides reference data on speech perception and listening effort in noise for listeners with age-related hearing loss. In addition to the clinically most relevant spatial condition with frontal speech and noise presentation, this study also provides reference data for more complex spatial conditions and different noise signals.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.R.; Methodology, T.R.; Formal Analysis, T.R. and T.M.W.; Investigation, T.R., A.C.K., L.W. and T.M.W.; Resources, T.R. and S.K.P.; Data Curation, T.R.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, T.R. and T.M.W.; Writing—Review and Editing, T.R., A.C.K., L.W., T.M.W. and S.K.P.; Visualization, T.R. and T.M.W.; Supervision, T.R.; Project Administration, T.R.; Funding Acquisition, T.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Cochlear Research and Development Limited, Addlestone, UK, grant number IIR-2317.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Martin Luther University Halle-Wittenberg (project identification code 2021-044).

Informed Consent Statement

All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Supporting raw data may be obtained through special request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We thank T. Hocke (Cochlear Deutschland GmbH & Co. KG, Hannover, Germany.) for intellectual, administrative, and technical support in designing the experiment.

Conflicts of Interest

T.R. and A.C.K. received funding from Cochlear Research and Development Limited, Addlestone, UK for this work and travel reimbursements. All other authors declare no conflicts of interest. The sponsor had no role in the execution, interpretation, or writing of the study.

References

  1. Keithley, E.M. Pathology and mechanisms of cochlear aging. J. Neurosci. Res. 2020, 98, 1674–1684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Von Gablenz, P.; Holube, I. Hörverlust und Sprachverstehen im Alter. Laryngo-Rhino-Otol. 2017, 96, 759–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Moore, D.R.; Edmondson-Jones, M.; Dawes, P.; Fortnum, H.; McCormack, A.; Pierzycki, R.H.; Munro, K.J. Relation between speech-in-noise threshold, hearing loss and cognition from 40–69 years of age. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e107720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Lindenberger, U.; Ghisletta, P. Cognitive and sensory declines in old age: Gauging the evidence for a common cause. Psychol. Aging 2009, 24, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Lin, F.R.; Yaffe, K.; Xia, J.; Xue, Q.-L.; Harris, T.B.; Purchase-Helzner, E.; Satterfield, S.; Ayonayon, H.N.; Ferrucci, L.; Simonsick, E.M. Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Intern. Med. 2013, 173, 293–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Wingfield, A.; Tun, P.A. Cognitive supports and cognitive constraints on comprehension of spoken language. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 2007, 18, 548–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Pichora-Fuller, M.K.; Schneider, B.A.; Daneman, M. How young and old adults listen to and remember speech in noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1995, 97, 593–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Ross, B.; Dobri, S.; Schumann, A. Speech-in-noise understanding in older age: The role of inhibitory cortical responses. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2020, 51, 891–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Kocabay, A.P.; Aslan, F.; Yüce, D.; Turkyilmaz, D. Speech in Noise: Implications of Age, Hearing Loss, and Cognition. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2022, 74, 345–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Humes, L.E.; Kidd, G.R.; Lentz, J.J. Auditory and cognitive factors underlying individual differences in aided speech-understanding among older adults. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Füllgrabe, C.; Moore, B.C.J.; Stone, M.A. Age-group differences in speech identification despite matched audiometrically normal hearing: Contributions from auditory temporal processing and cognition. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2014, 6, 347. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  12. Kollmeier, B.; Warzybok, A.; Hochmuth, S.; Zokoll, M.A.; Uslar, V.; Brand, T.; Wagener, K.C. The multilingual matrix test: Principles, applications, and comparison across languages: A review. Int. J. Audiol. 2015, 54 (Suppl. S2), 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Wagener, K.C.; Brand, T.; Kollmeier, B. Entwicklung und Evaluation eines Satztests für die deutsche Sprache Teil III: Evaluation des Oldenburger Satztests. Z. Audiol. 1999, 38, 86–95. [Google Scholar]
  14. Abdel-Latif, K.H.A.; Meister, H. Speech Recognition and Listening Effort in Cochlear Implant Recipients and Normal-Hearing Listeners. Front. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 725412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Hochmuth, S.; Kollmeier, B.; Brand, T.; Jürgens, T. Influence of noise type on speech reception thresholds across four languages measured with matrix sentence tests. Int. J. Audiol. 2015, 54 (Suppl. S2), 62–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Weißgerber, T.; Stöver, T.; Baumann, U. Speech perception in modulated noise assessed in bimodal CI users. HNO 2023, 71, 487–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Chan, D. EUROM—A spoken language resource for the EU. In Proceedings of the 4th European Conference on Speech Communication and Speech Technology Eurospeech, Madrid, Spain, 18–21 September 1995; pp. 867–870. [Google Scholar]
  18. Wagner, F.; Todt, I.; Wagner, J.; Ernst, A. Indications and candidacy for active middle ear implants. Adv. Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. 2010, 69, 20–26. [Google Scholar]
  19. Puder, H.; Fisher, E.; Hain, J. Optimized Directional Processing in Hearing Aids with Integrated Spatial Noise Reduction. In Proceedings of the IWAENC 2012, International Workshop on Acoustic Signal Enhancement, Aachen, Germany, 4–6 September 2012; VDE Verlag: Offenbach, Germany, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  20. Rahne, T.; Fröhlich, L.; Wagner, L.; Kropp, M.H.; Müller, A. Speech perception and hearing effort using a new active middle ear implant audio processor. Eur. Arch. Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. Head Neck 2022, 279, 4667–4675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Decambron, M.; Leclercq, F.; Renard, C.; Vincent, C. Speech audiometry in noise: SNR Loss per age-group in normal hearing subjects. Eur. Ann. Otorhinolaryngol. Head Neck Dis. 2022, 139, 61–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Mukari, S.Z.-M.S.; Wahat, N.H.A.; Mazlan, R. Effects of ageing and hearing thresholds on speech perception in quiet and in noise perceived in different locations. Korean J. Audiol. 2014, 18, 112–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Peelle, J.E. Listening Effort: How the Cognitive Consequences of Acoustic Challenge Are Reflected in Brain and Behavior. Ear Hear. 2018, 39, 204–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Krueger, M.; Schulte, M.; Brand, T.; Holube, I. Development of an adaptive scaling method for subjective listening effort. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2017, 141, 4680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Kwak, C.; Han, W. Age-Related Difficulty of Listening Effort in Elderly. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 8845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Luts, H.; Eneman, K.; Wouters, J.; Schulte, M.; Vormann, M.; Buechler, M.; Dillier, N.; Houben, R.; Dreschler, W.A.; Froehlich, M.; et al. Multicenter evaluation of signal enhancement algorithms for hearing aids. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2010, 127, 1491–1505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Magudilu Srishyla Kumar, L. Measuring Listening Effort Using Physiological, Behavioral and Subjective Methods in Normal Hearing Subjects: Effect of Signal to Noise Ratio and Presentation Level. Ph.D. Thesis, James Madison University, Harrisonburg, VA, USA, 2020. Volume 7. Available online: https://commons.lib.jmu.edu/diss202029/7 (accessed on 1 September 2023).
  28. Degeest, S.; Keppler, H.; Corthals, P. The Effect of Age on Listening Effort. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2015, 58, 1592–1600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. DIN EN ISO 7029:2017-06; Akustik- Statistische Verteilung von Hörschwellen in Bezug auf das Alter und das Geschlecht. Beuth Verlag GmbH: Berlin, Germany, 2017.
  30. Cooke, M. A glimpsing model of speech perception in noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2006, 119, 1562–1573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Rader, T. Speech Perception of Cochlear Implanted Patients with Combined Electric-Acoustic Stimulation. Ph.D. Thesis, Technische Universität München, Munich, Germany, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  32. Zirn, S.; Polterauer, D.; Keller, S.; Hemmert, W. The effect of fluctuating maskers on speech understanding of high-performing cochlear implant users. Int. J. Audiol. 2016, 55, 295–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Litovsky, R. Spatial release from masking. Acoust. Today 2012, 8, 18–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Wagner, L.; Geiling, L.; Hauth, C.; Hocke, T.; Plontke, S.; Rahne, T. Improved binaural speech reception thresholds through small symmetrical separation of speech and noise. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0236469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kitoh, R.; Nishio, S.-Y.; Usami, S.-I. Speech perception in noise in patients with idiopathic sudden hearing loss. Acta Oto-Laryngol. 2022, 142, 302–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hamzavi, J.; Franz, P.; Baumgartner, W.D.; Gstöettner, W. Hearing performance in noise of cochlear implant patients versus severely-profoundly hearing-impaired patients with hearing aids. Audiol. Off. Organ Int. Soc. Audiol. 2001, 40, 26–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Lundberg, E.M.H.; Strong, D.; Anderson, M.; Kaizer, A.M.; Gubbels, S. Do Patients Benefit from a Cochlear Implant when They Qualify Only in the Presence of Background Noise? Otol. Neurotol. Off. Publ. Am. Otol. Soc. Am. Neurotol. Soc. Eur. Acad. Otol. Neurotol. 2021, 42, 251–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Arndt, S.; Laszig, R.; Aschendorff, A.; Hassepass, F.; Beck, R.; Wesarg, T. Cochlea-Implantat-Versorgung von Patienten mit einseitiger Taubheit oder asymmetrischem Hörverlust. HNO 2017, 65, 98–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Arndt, S.; Wesarg, T.; Stelzig, Y.; Jacob, R.; Illg, A.; Lesinski-Schiedat, A.; Ketterer, M.C.; Aschendorff, A.; Speck, I. Einfluss einseitiger Taubheit auf das Hörvermögen des besseren Ohrs. HNO 2020, 68, 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Kitterick, P.T.; Smith, S.N.; Lucas, L. Hearing Instruments for Unilateral Severe-to-Profound Sensorineural Hearing Loss in Adults: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Ear Hear. 2016, 37, 495–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Rahne, T.; Plontke, S.K. Functional Result After Cochlear Implantation in Children and Adults with Single-sided Deafness. Otol. Neurotol. Off. Publ. Am. Otol. Soc. Am. Neurotol. Soc. Eur. Acad. Otol. Neurotol. 2016, 37, e332–e340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Speck, I.; Challier, P.; Wesarg, T.; Jakob, T.F.; Aschendorff, A.; Hassepass, F.; Arndt, S. Is the cochlear implant a successful long-term solution for single-sided deaf and asymmetric hearing-impaired patients. Eur. Arch. Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. 2021, 278, 3257–3265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Lee, C.; Yeung, M.W.; Falk, L.; Ali, A.; Walter, M. Implantable Devices for Single-Sided Deafness and Conductive or Mixed Hearing Loss: A Health Technology Assessment. Ont. Health Technol. Assess. Ser. 2020, 20, 1–165. [Google Scholar]
  44. Mudery, J.A.; Francis, R.; McCrary, H.; Jacob, A. Older Individuals Meeting Medicare Cochlear Implant Candidacy Criteria in Noise but Not in Quiet: Are These Patients Improved by Surgery? Otol. Neurotol. Off. Publ. Am. Otol. Soc. Am. Neurotol. Soc. Eur. Acad. Otol. Neurotol. 2017, 38, 187–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Experimental setup for speech and noise presentation. Speech signals were presented frontally, while noise signals were presented in a frontal (A) or backward (B) configuration.
Figure 1. Experimental setup for speech and noise presentation. Speech signals were presented frontally, while noise signals were presented in a frontal (A) or backward (B) configuration.
Jcm 12 06133 g001
Figure 2. Speech recognition (A) and listening effort (B) in noise for all spatial conditions with olnoise (white) or icra5 noise (grey), presented as box plots (median, min, max), for all included participants.
Figure 2. Speech recognition (A) and listening effort (B) in noise for all spatial conditions with olnoise (white) or icra5 noise (grey), presented as box plots (median, min, max), for all included participants.
Jcm 12 06133 g002
Figure 3. Speech recognition in noise as box plots (median, min, max) for all age groups for the spatial conditions of (A) S0N0, (B) S0N180, (C) S0N0,45,−45, and (D) S0N180,135,−135. The SRT50 is shown as box plots for the use of olnoise (white) or icra5 noise (grey).
Figure 3. Speech recognition in noise as box plots (median, min, max) for all age groups for the spatial conditions of (A) S0N0, (B) S0N180, (C) S0N0,45,−45, and (D) S0N180,135,−135. The SRT50 is shown as box plots for the use of olnoise (white) or icra5 noise (grey).
Jcm 12 06133 g003
Figure 4. Listening effort in noise as box plots (median, min, max) for all age groups for the spatial conditions of (A) S0N0 and (B) S0N180. The SNR5cut is shown as box plots for the use of olnoise (white) or icra5 noise (grey).
Figure 4. Listening effort in noise as box plots (median, min, max) for all age groups for the spatial conditions of (A) S0N0 and (B) S0N180. The SNR5cut is shown as box plots for the use of olnoise (white) or icra5 noise (grey).
Jcm 12 06133 g004
Table 1. Demographic data.
Table 1. Demographic data.
Age Group, Years
Characteristics 18–3031–4041–5051–6061–7071–80
Number 181615181616
Age, mean (SD), years 24.6 (3.9)34.5 (2.7)46.8 (2.7)53.0 (1.5)66.2 (2.9)75.8 (2.9)
Men/women, N 14/413/38/712/612/613/3
Hearing, mean (SD), dB PTA
Right ear 8.0 (4.7)8.3 (3.6)13.3 (5.2)17.1 (7.4)22.3 (9.5)25.9 (8.8)
Median [25th, 75th percentiles]7.5 [5.0,10.0]6.9 [6.3,11.3]13.8 [10.0,17.5]15.6 [11.3,20.0]19.4 [15.0,29.7]25.3 [19.4,34.1]
Left ear 6.8 (5.1)8.4 (4.5)14.1 (5.1)17.1 (6.4)22.6 (8.2)27.3 (11.2)
Median [25th, 75th percentiles]6.3 [3.8,7.5]7.5 [5.0,12.2]11.3 [10.0,18.8]15.6 [12.2,22.8]22.5 [13.1,28.8]30.0 [17.8,36.9]
Average over both ears7.5 (5.3)8.5 (3.8)14.4 (4.5)17.5 (6.8)23.1 (8.4)26.6 (9.7)
Median [25th, 75th percentiles]6.8 [4.4,9.4]8.8 [5.6,11.3]14.1 [10.5,18.3]17.2 [12.5,20.8]21.3 [15.0,30.0]27.8 [18.6,35.6]
Word recognition, mean (SD), % correct @ 65dB SPL
Right ear 99.0 (2.1)99.0 (2.1)97.9 (3.2)96.3 (4.3)88.3 (14.4)87.8 (8.2)
Median [25th, 75th percentiles]100 [100,100]100 [100,100]100 [95,100]95 [95,100]95 [80,100]90 [81.3,93,8]
Left ear 99.0 (2.8)98.3 (3.1)98.2 (2.5)96.6 (5.7)90.0 (11.6)84.0 (13.8)
Median [25th, 75th percentiles]100 [100,100]100 [95,100]100 [95,100]100 [95,100]95 [80,100]87.5 [72.5,95]
Abbreviations: SD: standard deviation; dB PTA0.5-4: Decibel pure tone average over 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz; SPL: Sound pressure level.
Table 2. Outcome measures of speech perception and listening effort in noise.
Table 2. Outcome measures of speech perception and listening effort in noise.
Age Group, Years
Outcome 18–3031–4041–5051–6061–7071–80
Speech perception in noise
SRT50 in dB SNR, mean (SD)
Olnoise, S0N0−6.64 (0.75)−6.53 (0.65)−6.54 (0.58)−6.18 (0.78)−5.95 (1.38)−4.91 (0.60)
Olnoise, S0N180−11.43 (1.58)−11.11 (1.63)−11.19 (1.62)−10.46 (2.32)−9.75 (1.48)−9.54 (1.84)
Olnoise, S0N0,45,−45−8.51 (0.74)−8.19 (0.85)−8.14 (0.60)−7.74 (0.71)−7.10 (0.87)−6.91 (0.91)
Olnoise, S0N180,135,−135−12.35 (1.58)−11.57 (1.26)−11.06 (1.47)−11.04 (1.11)−10.40 (1.38)−10.23 (1.69)
Icra5, S0N0−18.53 (2.67)−18.66 (2.27)−17.39 (1.73)−16.39 (2.28)−14.11 (3.47)−11.60 (2.37)
Icra5, S0N180−22.52 (2.34)−21.41 (2.81)−20.44 (3.05)−19.98 (2.58)−16.87 (3.10)−15.78 (3.53)
Icra5, S0N0,45,−45−12.41 (1.27)−11.89 (1.68)−11.01 (1.52)−1.13 (1.37)−9.25 (1.78)−7.95 (1.77)
Icra5, S0N180,135,−135−16.20 (1.74)−14.67 (1.75)−14.26 (1.95)−13.34 (1.57)−12.33 (2.05)−11.20 (2.25)
Listening effort in noise
SNRcut in dB SNR, mean (SD)
Olnoise, S0N0−3.2 (2.4)−2.5 (2.6)−2.4 (1.8)−2.5 (3.0)−3.6 (2.1)−4.6 (2.5)
Olnoise, S0N180−7.9 (2.6)−5.8 (3.6)−6.8 (3.6)−6.8 (3.5)−8.2 (3.1)−7.6 (2.4)
Icra5, S0N0−10.3 (4.6)−7.6 (4.1)−7.9 (3.9)−6.8 (5.1)−9.1 (5.4)−8.7 (4.4)
Icra5, S0N180−13.5 (4.4)−10.4 (4.9)−10.6 (3.7)−10.7 (5.8)−12.8 (4.5)−12.0 (6.0)
Abbreviations: SD: standard deviation; SRT50: 50% speech reception threshold, SNR: signal-to-noise ration; SNRcut: signal to noise ratio at moderate listening effort.
Table 3. Linear regression report for the outcome variables.
Table 3. Linear regression report for the outcome variables.
Dependent VariableAgePTA, Binaural Average
OutcomebSEβTpbSEβTp
Speech perception in noise
Olnoise, S0N00.0250.0070.4383.42<0.0010.0130.0140.1240.970.335
Olnoise, S0N1800.0060.0150.0550.400.6890.0870.0270.4413.230.002
Olnoise, S0N0,45,−450.0180.0073.2402.740.0070.0370.0130.3482.940.004
Olnoise, S0N180,135,−1350.0100.0110.1140.880.3840.0690.0210.4213.220.002
Icra5, S0N00.0350.0190.1721.890.0630.2530.0350.6637.26<0.001
Icra5, S0N1800.0310.0200.1401.490.1400.2760.0380.6897.34<0.001
Icra5, S0N0,45,−450.0420.0120.3413.43<0.0010.1090.0230.4694.70<0.001
Icra5, S0N180,135,−1350.0260.0150.1761.700.0930.1610.0280.5915.70<0.001
Listening effort in noise
Olnoise, S0N0−0.0440.022−0.301−2.040.0440.0260.0410.0940.640.524
Olnoise, S0N180−0.0310.027−0.175−1.160.2480.0150.0500.0450.300.766
Icra5, S0N0−0.0320.041−0.117−0.780.4410.0920.0760.1811.200.232
Icra5, S0N180−0.0420.043−0.149−0.980.3300.0790.0800.1490.980.331
Bold: significant linear regression, p < 0.05.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Rahne, T.; Wagner, T.M.; Kopsch, A.C.; Plontke, S.K.; Wagner, L. Influence of Age on Speech Recognition in Noise and Hearing Effort in Listeners with Age-Related Hearing Loss. J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12, 6133. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12196133

AMA Style

Rahne T, Wagner TM, Kopsch AC, Plontke SK, Wagner L. Influence of Age on Speech Recognition in Noise and Hearing Effort in Listeners with Age-Related Hearing Loss. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2023; 12(19):6133. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12196133

Chicago/Turabian Style

Rahne, Torsten, Telse M. Wagner, Anna C. Kopsch, Stefan K. Plontke, and Luise Wagner. 2023. "Influence of Age on Speech Recognition in Noise and Hearing Effort in Listeners with Age-Related Hearing Loss" Journal of Clinical Medicine 12, no. 19: 6133. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12196133

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop