Next Article in Journal
The Association between ADHD and Obesity: Intriguing, Progressively More Investigated, but Still Puzzling
Previous Article in Journal
Endocannabinoids and Fear-Related Behavior in Mice Selectively Bred for High or Low Alcohol Preference
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Characteristic of Stimulus Frequency Otoacoustic Emissions: Detection Rate, Musical Training Influence, and Gain Function

Department of Biomedical Engineering, School of Life Sciences, Tianjin Polytechnic University, Tianjin 300387, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2019, 9(10), 255; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9100255
Submission received: 14 August 2019 / Revised: 18 September 2019 / Accepted: 25 September 2019 / Published: 26 September 2019

Abstract

:
Stimulus frequency otoacoustic emission (SFOAE) is an active acoustic signal emitted by the inner ear providing salient information about cochlear function and dysfunction. To provide a basis for laboratory investigation and clinical use, we investigated the characteristics of SFOAEs, including detection rate, musical training influence, and gain function. Sixty-five normal hearing subjects (15 musicians and 50 non-musicians, aged 16–45 years) were tested and analyzed at the probe level of 30 and 50 dB sound pressure levels (SPL) in the center frequency of 1 and 4 kHz in the study. The results indicate that (1) the detection rates of SFOAE are sensitive to the gender, (2) musicians reveal enhanced hearing capacity and SFOAE amplitudes compared with non-musicians, and (3) probe frequency has a significant effect on the compression threshold of SFOAE. Our findings highlight the importance of SFOAE in the clinical hearing screening and diagnosis and emphasize the use of musical training for the rehabilitation enhancement of the auditory periphery and hearing threshold.

1. Introduction

Since the discovery of otoacoustic emission (OAE) in 1978 [1], several studies have attempted to demonstrate OAE as a promising non-invasive tool for assessment of cochlear functional status [2,3]. However, the applicability of stimulus-frequency (SF) OAEs in laboratory investigation and clinical use (e.g., the detection rate and gain function under different conditions) is still unclear. Bidelman et al. showed an experience-dependent enhancement in the peripheral frequency resolution for musicians [4]. It needs to be further investigated whether the peripheral threshold (indicated by SFOAE amplitude) improved musical training. To explore the above questions, we investigated detection rate, musical training influence, and gain function of SFOAEs.
OAE is a kind of weak acoustic signal non-invasively recorded in the external auditory canal by a tiny microphone. It is produced from the cochlea and transmitted via the ossicles chain and the tympanic membrane [5]. OAE is a by-product of the active mechanism of the cochlea and can reflect the function of outer hair cells (OHCs) [1,6]. Therefore, it is sensitive to the damage of the inner ear. OAEs are divided into spontaneous otoacoustic emission (SOAE) and evoked otoacoustic emission (EOAE) according to whether the emission is induced by a stimulus or not. SOAEs are recordable in the absence of deliberate external acoustic stimulation [7,8]. EOAEs are measured in the ear canal in the case of external stimulation. Owing to the different types of stimuli, EOAE includes distortion product otoacoustic emission (DPOAE), transient evoked otoacoustic emission (TEOAE), and SFOAE. SFOAE is an active acoustic signal emitted by the inner ear with the same frequency as the stimulus, and is generally considered the result of activities of the cochlear mechano-electrical transducer [9,10]. It can provide salient information about cochlear function and dysfunction objectively and non-invasively [11,12,13]. Since the frequency of SFOAE is identical to the stimulus in terms of time and frequency domains, SFOAEs are expected to show peripheral auditory function at specific sites of cochlea.
Many investigations have revealed that OAE is a sensitive acoustic signal, and it can indicate cochlear function in subjects with normal hearing or moderate hearing impairment [14,15]. To date, the detection rates of TEOAE, DPOAE, and SOAE have been primarily investigated. The detection rate of SOAEs only reaching 50%–74% mainly occurs at a specific frequency, and there are great individual differences in SOAEs [16]. Age and gender affect the DPOAEs prominently in normal hearing subjects. The detection of DPOAEs and mean amplitudes decreases with advancing age [17]. In terms of TEOAE, the study of detection rate is focused on newborns and tinnitus. The detection rate of TEOAEs is nearly 100% in normal-hearing participants at the age <60 [18]. However, the detection rate of TEOAE decreases with increasing age in normal hearing [pure tone audiometry <25 dB hearing level (HL) in 250~8000 Hz], suggesting that TEOAE is more sensitive than pure tone audiometry (PTA) in detecting early cochlear impairment [19]. In newborns, the general prevalence of TEOAE is 89%, with the average prevalence being 74% in newborns 0–3 day old and 97% in newborns 4–8 day old. Moreover, the prevalence of females is higher than males [20]. In tinnitus, the detection rates of TEOAE, DPOAE, and SOAE are all lower than that of the normal hearing group, also revealing that OAEs are sensitive to the early impairment of cochlea [21]. Previous studies have revealed that OAEs are sensitive in normal hearing subjects and abnormal groups [21,22]. It suggests that OAEs can be used for monitoring the progression of cochlear damage during the early stages of hearing impairment. Although SFOAE has a good frequency specificity compared with SOAE, TEOAE, and DPOAE, investigations of the detection rate of SFOAE have been rare to date.
As musical training is more complex and multimodal than most other daily life activities, music has increasingly been used as a tool for the investigation of human cognition and its underlying brain mechanisms [23,24,25]. Musicians exhibit enhanced sensory and cognitive abilities that impart notable advantages compared with non-musicians for pitch discrimination [26,27,28], speech in noise tasks [29,30,31], and auditory attention [32]. Several studies have suggested that such enhanced auditory skills in musicians are ascribed to long-term musical training, which induces functional [33] and structural [34] differences of the central auditory system. In other words, musical training influences the brain plasticity of musicians [35]. As early as 1970, Soderquist et al. reported that non-musicians are inferior to professional musicians in their ability to analyze complex waveforms [36]. In 2011, Bidelman et al. pointed out that musicians have significant tonal resolution advantages compared with non-musicians in both electrophysiological and behavioral tests [37]. This indicates that the enhancement effect of auditory brainstem neurophysiological response, which is induced by long-term musical training starting from childhood, has shifted to the cognitive level [38,39,40]. Although many studies indicate that musical training is related to auditory perception, it remains unclear whether it influences the intensity sensitivity of the auditory periphery. Many studies manifest that music training can enhance medial olivocochlear system (MOC) activity [41,42]. Owing to the nerve axons of the MOC being connected with the outer hair cells of the cochlea, internal changes of the cochlea of musicians may ensue [43]. Consequently, we hypothesize that long-term musical training may enhance the SFOAE amplitude.
A further question that we intended to explore was the gain function of SFOAE under different conditions to explore the possibility of clinical application of SFOAEs. The gain function is considered a measure of emission strength, which is plotted as OAE level in dB as a function of stimulus level [in dB sound pressure levels (SPL)] [44]. It reveals the maximum OAE gain and compression nonlinearity and can reflect the cochlear-mechanical response growth invasively [45]. The nonlinearity is attributed to outer hair cell (OHC) function, and it is present in normal hearing ears. Presumably due to OHC loss, the nonlinearity is decreased or absent in ears with hearing loss [45,46]. Thus, the gain function is of great importance in describing auditory deficits [47].
In this study, we investigated the characteristics of SFOAEs, including the detection rate, the musical training influence, and the gain function, in order to provide a basis for laboratory investigation and clinical use. By detecting and analyzing the PTA and the SFOAE amplitude in 65 normal hearing subjects (15 musicians and 50 non-musicians) at the probe levels of 30 and 50 dB SPL in the center frequency of 1 and 4 kHz, we explored and predicted (1) the detection rate of SFOAE under the conditions of different genders, probe levels, and probe frequencies; (2) that musical training may enhance the hearing threshold and SFOAE amplitude; and (3) that it is possible that the gain function of SFOAE is sensitive to the stimulus frequency.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This study consisted of three experiments in which all participants were native speakers of Chinese, right-handed according to the standard handedness questionnaire [48], and with normal hearing (≤20 dB hearing level for 250–8000 Hz); none had a history of hearing disorders or strong spontaneous optoacoustic emissions. During the experiments, the tested ear was randomly determined. All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the Shandong University institutional review board.
• Experiment 1: Detection Rates
Fifty young adults (26 females and 24 males) aged 16–45 years (mean ± standard deviation 23.94 ± 4.57 years) with normal hearing participated in the first experiment, and no musician was included in the 50 subjects.
• Experiment 2: Musical Training Influence
Thirty young adults including 15 musicians (12 females and 3 males) and 15 non-musicians (10 females and 5 males) aged 21–44 years (mean ± standard deviation 25.20 ± 4.32 years) participated in the second experiment. Based on self-reporting, all musicians commenced their musical training before the age of 10 (9.60 ± 2.77 years) and had consistently played a musical instrument or had vocal music training throughout their lives for 15 years (14.07 ± 2.84 years). The definition of “musician” was consistent with the previous study [49]. Non-musicians had <2 years of formal musical training throughout their lifespan. Musicians and non-musicians were matched in age (musicians, 25.93 ± 5.57 years; non-musicians, 24.47 ± 2.53 years) and formal education [49,50] (musicians, 17.93 ± 1.44 years; non-musicians, 18.13 ± 2.26 years) (for detailed information about musicians and non-musicians, see Supplementary Materials).
• Experiment 3: Gain Function
Ten normal-hearing adults (6 females and 4 males) aged 20–27 years (mean ± standard deviation 22.50 ± 2.72 years) participated in the third experiment.
All the normal hearing participants without musical training in Experiments 2 and 3 were also included in Experiment 1.

2.2. Instruments

All experiments were carried out with participants sitting on a chair quietly in a sound-attenuating booth. Briefly, stimuli were generated by an external soundcard (Fire face 802, RME, Haimhausen, Germany) and delivered by a pair of miniature earphones (ER-3C, Etymotic Research, Elk Grove Village, IL, USA). One miniature earphone produced a constant tone as a probe, and the other miniature earphone produced a tone-burst as a suppressor. The acoustic signals in the ear canal were collected by a miniature microphone (ER-10B+, Etymotic Research) and amplified by 20 dB (ER-10B+ preamplifier, Etymotic Research). A monaural earplug including both miniature earphones and a miniature microphone was inserted into the ear canal of each participant. The measurement system was calibrated with a Total Sound ear simulator (type AE400). During the detection of SFOAE, the participants were able to watch a silent film. In the detection of PTA, each participant was instructed to pay attention to the probe tone by pressing a button.

2.3. Design and Procedure

Before the detection of SFOAE, PTA and SOAE tests were examined to select eligible subjects. It was guaranteed that there were no SOAEs in the frequency range of interest in order to avoid interference with the SFOAE. In Experiment 1, the detection rate was calculated as the number of participants who detected SFOAE divided by the total number of participants. The criteria for the presence of SFOAE was signal-to-noise-ratio ≥6 dB in the SFOAE spectra (an example of the presence of SFOAE is shown in Figure 1). The SFOAE amplitudes were tested under 4 conditions: 2 probe levels × 2 probe frequencies. The probe levels (Lp) were 30 and 50 dB SPL, and center frequencies were 1 and 4 kHz. In Experiment 2, SFOAE fine structure was a high-resolution (40 Hz steps) SFOAE amplitude recording which was evoked in the probe frequency (fp) range of ± 200 Hz (relative to the center frequency, CF). Each subject was tested at the probe level (Lp) of 30 dB SPL and CFs of 1 and 4 kHz. In Experiment 3, SFOAE amplitudes were plotted as a function of Lp (5–50 dB SPL in 5 dB steps) in SFOAE input/output function testing to determine the gain function of SFOAE. The gain function is considered a measure of emission strength and is plotted as OAE level in dB regarding stimulus level (in dB SPL).

2.4. Data Analysis

All data first underwent Kolmogorov–Smirnov testing to verify the compliance of normal distribution. All data analyses were conducted in SPSS 19.0 software (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

2.4.1. Experiment 1: Detection Rates

The detection rates of SFOAE were analyzed with chi-square tests among the factors of gender (female versus male), probe frequency (1 versus 4 kHz), and probe level (30 versus 50 dB SPL). The detection rates of SFOAE between groups of female and male were compared under 4 conditions: 2 probe levels × 2 probe frequencies. In addition to the p value, Cohen’s d was used to describe the effect size (ES) when the data satisfied the normal distribution.

2.4.2. Experiment 2: Musical Training Influence

Mean values were provided first to compare the values of PTA and SFOAE amplitudes. Then, to ascertain whether musical training affects the inner ear, PTA at octave frequencies from 250 to 8000 Hz and SFOAE amplitudes at the center frequency of 4 kHz were analyzed with non-parametric statistics (two-factor Scheirer–Ray–Hare test [51]) as the values of them did not satisfy the normal distribution. Additionally, SFOAE amplitudes at the center frequency of 1 kHz were analyzed with two-factor analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests as the values of them satisfied the normal distribution. One factor was the group difference between musicians and non-musicians, and the other factor was probe frequency (250/500/1000/2000/4000/8000 Hz). Finally, simple effect analysis was conducted when significant interactions existed between the two factors of group difference and probe frequencies. In addition to the p value, partial eta squared (ƞp2) was used to describe the effect size.

2.4.3. Experiment 3: Gain Function

Three SFOAE metrics could be derived from the fitted curve of gain function: (1) peak strength (PS), the SFOAE level of the flat portion of the gain function; (2) compression threshold (CT), the stimulus level at which SFOAE strength began to decrease compressively; and (3) compression slope (CS), which indicated the rate of compression of the SFOAE gain function at probe levels beyond the CT [44]. The above three SFOAE metrics (see illustration in FIG3B of Ref [44]) were analyzed with independent-sample t tests between probe frequencies of 1 and 4 kHz and non-parametric statistics (Mann–Whitney U tests) when the data did not satisfy the normal distribution. In addition to the p value, Cohen’s d was used to describe the ES when the data satisfied the normal distribution.

3. Results

3.1. Detection Rate

Table 1 represents the detection rates of SFOAE that were tested with the different factors of gender, probe frequency, and Lp value. Figure 2a–d illustrates the detection rates of SFOAE between groups of female and male under four conditions: 2 probe levels × 2 probe frequencies. As shown in Table 1 and Figure 2a, among a total of 50 participants (24 females and 26 males), SFOAEs were detected in 17 female participants (71%) and 24 male participants (92%). The male detection rate of SFOAE was obviously higher than that of the female detection rate at the fp of 1 kHz and Lp of 30 dB SPL, and a significant difference existed between them (χ2 = 3.899, p = 0.048, ES = 0.583). As shown in Table 1 and Figure 2b, SFOAEs were detected in 23 female participants (96%) and 25 male participants (96%). The statistical results revealed no significant difference between the female detection rate of SFOAE and that of the male detection rate at the fp of 1 kHz and Lp of 50 dB SPL (χ2 = 0.003, p = 0.954, ES = 0.016). As shown in Table 1 and Figure 2c, SFOAEs were detected in 22 female participants (92%) and 17 male participants (65%). The female detection rate of SFOAE was obviously higher than the male detection rate at the fp of 4 kHz and Lp of 30 dB SPL, and a significant difference existed between them (χ2 = 5.024, p = 0.025, ES = 0.668). As shown in Table 1 and Figure 2d, SFOAEs were detected in 15 female participants (63%) and 23 male participants (88%). At the fp of 4 kHz and Lp of 50 dB SPL, the detection rate of SFOAE in males was obviously higher than that in females, and gender had a significant effect on the detection rate of SFOAE (χ2 = 4.612, p = 0.032, ES = 0.637).

3.2. Musical Training Influence

3.2.1. PTA Test Results between Musicians and Non-Musicians

Figure 3 illustrates the mean values of PTA for musicians and non-musicians as a function of fp value. Except for the PTA at an fp value of 2 kHz, the mean hearing thresholds of musicians were lower than those of non-musicians at the other probe frequencies. A two-factor Scheirer–Ray–Hare test revealed that both group difference (musicians vs. non-musicians) and fp had significant effects on hearing thresholds (group difference—df = 1, H = 5.194, p = 0.023; fpdf = 5, H = 15.243, p = 0.009), and interaction was found between the factors of group difference and frequency (df = 5, H = 11.841, p = 0.037). As shown in Table 2, simple effect analysis revealed that musicianship affected hearing threshold saliently at fp values of 250 and 500 Hz (250 Hz—df = 1, H = 10.276, p = 0.001; 500 Hz—df = 1, H = 6.484, p = 0.011), and probe frequency affected hearing threshold saliently in the group of non-musicians (df = 5, H = 19.141, p = 0.002).

3.2.2. SFOAE Amplitudes between Musicians and Non-Musicians

The mean values of SFOAE amplitudes for musicians and non-musicians as a function of fp value at low [1 kHz; see Figure 4a] and high [4 kHz; see Figure 4b] probe frequencies are illustrated in Figure 4. As shown in Figure 4a, the mean SFOAE amplitudes of musicians were larger than those of non-musicians at all fp values centered 1 kHz. The two-factor ANOVA tests revealed that musicianship had a significant effect on the SFOAE fine structure at the center frequency of 1 kHz (df = 1, F = 25.104, p = 0.000001, ƞp2 = 0.075), while there was no significant difference among the SFOAE amplitudes of different fp values (df = 10, F = 1.681, p = 0.084, ƞp2 = 0.052). No interactions were found between the factors of musical training and frequency (df = 10, F = 0.334, p = 0.971, ƞp2 = 0.011). As shown in Figure 4b, the values of SFOAE amplitudes between musicians and non-musicians at each fp seemed to be slightly different, except for fp values of 4040 Hz and 4160 Hz. The means and the standard deviations (means ± standard deviations) of SFOAE amplitudes of musicians and non-musicians were −7.942 ± 7.469 dB SPL and −9.492 ± 7.363 dB SPL, respectively. The two-factor Scheirer–Ray–Hare tests revealed that musicianship had no significant effect on the SFOAE fine structure at the center frequency of 4 kHz (df = 1, H = 3.107, p = 0.078). In addition, there is no significant difference among the SFOAE amplitudes of different fp values (df = 10, H = 5.285, p = 0.871), and no interactions were found between the factors of musical training and frequency (df = 10, H = 1.547, p = 0.999).

3.3. Gain Function

Figure 5 illustrates the SFOAE input/output function (see Figure 5a) and the gain function (see Figure 5b) at 4 kHz for one subject. The gain function was transformed from the SFOAE input/output function by the SFOAE magnitude at each Lp subtracting (in dB) the corresponding probe level. The red line in Figure 5b represents the fitting curve of SFOAE strength. Three parameters, PS, CT, and CS, which were derived from this fit, are shown in Figure 5b. Both the input/output function and the gain function of SFOAE revealed the compressive characteristic of the cochlea.
All participants’ three parameters and their linear regression fits at center frequencies of 1 and 4 kHz are shown in Figure 6. PS, CT, and CS are plotted as a function of probe frequency, which is displayed from the first to third panels. As shown in Figure 6a,b, the maximum SFOAE gain was similar between low and high frequency. For CT and CS, CT and CS grew larger at the frequency centered at 1 kHz, while they grew smaller at the frequency centered at 4 kHz as the probe frequency increased. The detailed statistical results are presented in Table 3. The independent-simple t tests revealed that probe frequency had a significant effect on CT (t = −3.861, p = 0.001, ES = 1.342) but no significant effect on PS (t = 0.120, p = 0.905, ES = 0.044). Additionally, the Mann–Whitney U tests revealed no significant effect on CS (z = −0.416, p = 0.677).

4. Discussion

In the present study, the PTA and the SFOAE amplitudes were tested in 65 normal hearing subjects (15 musicians and 50 non-musicians) at probe levels of 30 and 50 dB SPL in the center frequencies of 1 and 4 kHz to investigate the characteristics of SFOAEs, including detection rate, musical training influence, and gain function. The results indicated that (1) the detection rates of SFOAE were sensitive to gender, (2) musicians revealed enhanced hearing capacity and SFOAE amplitudes compared with non-musicians, and (3) probe frequency had a significant effect on the compression threshold of SFOAE.

4.1. Detection Rate

In our study, the detection rates of SFOAE were tested for different genders at the conditions of different probe frequencies and probe levels. For gender, the detection rates of SFOAE between groups of females and males were compared under four conditions: 2 probe levels × 2 probe frequencies. Except for the fp of 1 kHz and Lp of 50 dB SPL, gender had a significant effect on the detection rates of SFOAE. Under the condition of fp = 4 kHz and Lp = 30 dB SPL, the female group’s detection rate of SFOAE was larger than that of the male group’s. The detection rate of SFOAE in males was larger than that in females under the conditions: (1) fp = 1 kHz and Lp = 30 dB SPL, (2) fp = 4 kHz and Lp = 50 dB SPL. The results in our study suggest that the detection rate of SFOAE is sensitive to the gender while the influence is irregular as the fp and Lp change. These results are inconsistent with observations of SOAEs that the detection rate for female subjects is significantly higher than that for male subjects [52]. The effect of gender on the detection of SFOAE depends on probe frequency and probe level. This may due to the fact that SFOAE is a reflection emission generated by a single pure tone at a specific probe frequency. For low frequency, the detection rates of both females and males increased as the probe level grew. The reason for low detection rate of SFOAE at low probe level was possibly due to SFOAE, which overlapped the evoking stimulus in both time and frequency and was easily disturbed by background noise. For high frequency, the detection rate of males increased, but that of females decreased with probe level growing. From Figure 4, we can see that the SFOAE amplitude at 4 kHz (−9.492 ± 1.047 dB SPL) was lower than that at 1 kHz (−4.353 ± 1.649 dB SPL) (t = 8.724, p = 0.00000003, ES = 3.562). Thus, the detection rate of SFOAE may have become lower and more sensitive at high probe frequency.

4.2. Musical Training Influence

In our study, we measured and analyzed the PTA and the SFOAE amplitudes between musicians and non-musicians to ascertain whether musical training affects the inner ear. Different statistical methods were used according to whether the data satisfied the normal distribution. Our results indicated that musicians exhibited lower hearing thresholds compared with non-musicians at 250 and 500 Hz. Several previous studies reported that musical experience improves perceptual and physiological frequency selectivity at the level of the cochlea [4,50]. PTA is a type of active listening that determines the individual’s minimum threshold of audibility. Our findings revealed that musical experience can also improve musicians’ audiometric thresholds as well as frequency resolution. For SFOAE, we observed that musicians exhibited larger amplitude when fp = 1 kHz, while there was no significant difference for PTA between musicians and non-musicians at a probe frequency of 1 kHz. This suggested that it could be improved by musical training at the cochlear level. The factor that may account for these phenomena is the regulation of prestin, which is a motor protein responsible for outer-hair-cell electromotility [53]. As PTA contains the influence of behavior of the participant, and the SFOAE is an objective measure evoked within cochlea, it suggests that the SFOAE reflects a real cochlear difference compared with PTA. For SFOAE amplitude at high probe frequency, musical training had no significant effect. This may be related to hearing loss often occurring at high frequency because of noise and aging for both non-musicians and musicians [54].

4.3. Gain Function

Our results indicated that probe frequency had a significant effect on the compression threshold of SFOAE, revealing that the stimulus level at which SFOAE strength began to decrease was significantly higher at high frequency. It may have accounted for the response of the basilar membrane to sounds of different frequencies being strongly affected by its mechanical properties; at the base (high frequency), it was relatively narrow and stiff, while at the apex (low frequency), it was wider and much less stiff. Therefore, at high frequency, the nonlinearity of SFOAE appeared at higher probe level.
As described in a previous study, SFOAE and DPOAE differ significantly in their compression features [44]. Owing to there being a different relationship of SFOAE and DPOAE metrics between normal and impaired hearing ears, the metrics combining SFOAE and DPOAE gain function are promising means to indicate hearing loss.

5. Conclusions

In summary, in the present study, the PTA and the SFOAE amplitudes were tested to investigate the characteristics of SFOAEs, including detection rate, musical training influence, and gain function. Our results provide a basis for laboratory investigation and clinical use of SFOAEs and demonstrate that musical training is beneficial to the rehabilitation enhancement of the auditory periphery and hearing threshold.

Supplementary Materials

Detailed information of musicians and non-musicians can be downloaded from: https://www.mdpi.com/2076-3425/9/10/255/s1.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Y.W.; Funding acquisition, Y.W.; Methodology, Y.W. and Z.Q.; Supervision, J.W.; Writing–original draft, Y.W., Z.Q. and M.Y.; Writing–review and editing, Y.W. and R.C.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China under the grant No.61701342, the Science & Technology Development Fund of Tianjin Education Commission for Higher Education under the grant No. 2018KJ212.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to our participants. We thank LetPub (www.letpub.com) for its linguistic assistance during the preparation of this manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Kemp, D.T. Stimulated acoustic emissions from within the human auditory system. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1978, 64, 1386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Karolina, K.C.; Pamela, E.S.; Jonathan, H.S. Exploration of stimulus-frequency otoacoustic emissionsuppression tuning in hearing-impaired listeners. Int. J. Audiol. 2015, 54, 96–105. [Google Scholar]
  3. Radha, K.; Carolina, A. Stimulus-frequency otoacoustic emissions in human newborns. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2015, 137, 78–84. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bidelman, G.M.; Nelms, C.; Bhagat, S.P. Musical experience sharpens human cochlear tuning. Hear. Res. 2016, 335, 40–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kemp, D.T. Otoacoustic Emissions: The basics, the science and the future potential. In Otoacoustic Emissions: Clinical Applications, 3rd ed.; Robinette, M.S., Glattke, T.J., Eds.; Thieme: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  6. Gold, T. Hearing. II. The Physical Basis of the Action of the Cochlea. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B Biol. 1948, 135, 492–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Shera, C.A. Mammalian spontaneous otoacoustic emissions are amplitude-stabilized cochlear standing waves. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2003, 114, 244–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Baiduc, R.R.; Lee, J.; Dhar, S. Spontaneous otoacoustic emissions, threshold microstructure, and psychophysical tuning over a wide frequency range in humans. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2014, 135, 300–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Kemp, D.T.; Chum, R.A. Observations on the Generator Mechanism of Stimulus Frequency Acoustic Emissions—Two Tone Suppression. In Psychophysical, Physiological and Behavioural Studies in Hearing; Springer Science and Business Media LLC: Berlin, Germany, 1980; pp. 34–42. [Google Scholar]
  10. Souter, M. Stimulus frequency otoacoustic emissions from guinea pig and human subjects. Hear. Res. 1995, 90, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Goodman, S.S.; Withnell, R.H.; Shera, C.A. The origin of SFOAE microstructure in the guinea pig. Hear. Res. 2003, 183, 7–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Brass, D. Time-domain observation of otoacoustic emissions during constant tone stimulation. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1991, 90, 2415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Bentsen, T.; Harte, J.M.; Dau, T. Human cochlear tuning estimates from stimulus-frequency otoacoustic emissions. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 129, 3797–3807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Wang, Y.; Gong, Q.; Zhang, T. The influence of probe level on the tuning of stimulus frequency otoacoustic emissions and behavioral test in human. Biomed. Eng. Online 2016, 15, 51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Ellison, J.C.; Keefe, D.H. Audiometric predictions using SFOAE and middle-ear measurements. Ear. Hear. 2005, 26, 487–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Bright, K.E. Spontaneous Otoacoustic Emissions in Populations with Normal Hearing Sensitivity. In Otoacoustic Emissions: Clinical Applications, 3rd ed.; Robinette, M.S., Glattke, T.J., Eds.; Thieme: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  17. Ni, D.F.; Dan, H.C. Effect of age and gender on distortion product otoacoustic emissions in normal hearing human. In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Prevention and Rehabilitation of Hearing Impairment, Beijing, China, 26–28 April 2007; pp. 310–315. [Google Scholar]
  18. Shi, Y.B.; Gu, R.; Jiang, S. Acoustic emission examination in normal young ear. Chin. J. Otorhinolaryngol. Head. Neck. Surg. 1989, 24, 349–351. [Google Scholar]
  19. Yu, L.M.; Li, X.; Qiu, C.Y.; Jiang, S.C. Clinical use of Click Evoked Otoacoustic Emissions in Detection Early Cochlear Impairment. J. Audiol. Speech Pathol. 1996, 3, 18–19. [Google Scholar]
  20. Yu, H.; Yu, L.; Li, Q.; Ding, Y.; Ma, X.; Ding, G. Measurement of transient evoked otoacoustic emissions and follow-up of 215 healthy newborns. Lin Chuang Er Bi Yan Hou Ke Za Zhi 2003, 17, 22–24. [Google Scholar]
  21. Yang, L.; Li, W.R.; Qian, L.; Wang, Y.B. Otoacoustic Emission in Tinnitus Patients with Normal Hearing. J. Audiol. Speech Pathol. 2003, 11, 187–188. [Google Scholar]
  22. Botelho, C.T.; Carvalho, S.A.D.S.; Silva, I.N. Increased prevalence of early cochlear damage in young patients with type 1 diabetes detected by distortion product otoacoustic emissions. Int. J. Audiol. 2014, 53, 402–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Pantev, C.; Herholz, S.C. Plasticity of the human auditory cortex related to musical training. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011, 35, 2140–2154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Herholz, S.C.; Zatorre, R.J. Musical Training as a Framework for Brain Plasticity: Behavior, Function, and Structure. Neuron 2012, 76, 486–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Brown, R.M.; Zatorre, R.J.; Penhune, V.B. Expert music performance: Cognitive, neural, and developmental bases. In Motivation—Theory, Neurobiology and Applications; Elsevier BV: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2015; Volume 217, pp. 57–86. [Google Scholar]
  26. Kraus, N.; Chandrasekaran, B. Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 11, 599–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Kishon-Rabin, L.; Amir, O.; Vexler, Y.; Zaltz, Y. Pitch Discrimination: Are Professional Musicians Better than Non-Musicians? J. Basic Clin. Physiol. Pharmacol. 2001, 12, 125–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Bianchi, F.; Santurette, S.; Wendt, D.; Dau, T. Pitch Discrimination in Musicians and Non-Musicians: Effects of Harmonic Resolvability and Processing Effort. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 2016, 17, 69–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Parbery-Clark, A.; Skoe, E.; Lam, C.; Kraus, N. Musician Enhancement for Speech-In-Noise. Ear Hear. 2009, 30, 653–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Parbery-Clark, A.; Strait, D.L.; Anderson, S.; Hittner, E.; Kraus, N. Musical Experience and the Aging Auditory System: Implications for Cognitive Abilities and Hearing Speech in Noise. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e18082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Coffey, E.B.; Mogilever, N.B.; Zatorre, R.J. Speech-in-noise perception in musicians: A review. Hear. Res. 2017, 352, 49–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Strait, D.L.; Kraus, N.; Parbery-Clark, A.; Ashley, R. Musical experience shapes top-down auditory mechanisms: Evidence from masking and auditory attention performance. Hear. Res. 2010, 261, 22–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Oechslin, M.S.; Van De Ville, D.; Lazeyras, F.; Hauert, C.A.; James, C.E. Degree of musical expertise modulates higher order brain functioning. Cereb. Cortex. 2013, 23, 2213–2224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Schneider, P.; Scherg, M.; Dosch, H.G.; Specht, H.J.; Gutschalk, A.; Rupp, A. Morphology of Heschl’s gyrus reflects enhanced activation in the auditory cortex of musicians. Nat. Neurosci. 2002, 5, 688–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Habib, M.; Besson, M. What do music training and musical experience teach us about brain plasticity? Music. Percept. 2009, 26, 279–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Soderquist, D.R. Frequency analysis and the critical band. Psychon. Sci. 1970, 21, 117–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Bidelman, G.M.; Gandour, J.T.; Krishnan, A. Musicians and tone-language speakers share enhanced brainstem encoding but not perceptual benefits for musical pitch. Brain Cogn. 2011, 77, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Innes, K.E.; Selfe, T.K.; Khalsa, D.S.; Kandati, S. Meditation and Music Improve Memory and Cognitive Function in Adults with Subjective Cognitive Decline: A Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2017, 56, 899–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Sihvonen, A.J.; Särkämö, T.; Leo, V.; Tervaniemi, M.; Altenmüller, E.; Soinila, S. Music-based interventions in neurological rehabilitation. Lancet Neurol. 2017, 16, 648–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Coutinho, E.; Schuller, B. Correction: Shared acoustic codes underlie emotional communication in music and speech—Evidence from deep transfer learning. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0191754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Brashears, S.M.; Morlet, T.G.; Berlin, C.I.; Hood, L.J. Olivocochlear efferent suppression in classical musicians. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 2003, 14, 314–324. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  42. Micheyl, C.; Carbonnel, O.; Collet, L. Medial Olivocochlear System and Loudness Adaptation - Differences between Musicians and Non-Musicians. Brain Cogn. 1995, 29, 127–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Perrot, X.; Collet, L. Function and plasticity of the medial olivocochlear system in musicians: A review. Hear. Res. 2014, 308, 27–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Abdala, C.; Kalluri, R. Towards a joint reflection-distortion otoacoustic emission profile: Results in normal and impaired ears. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2017, 142, 812–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Schairer, K.S.; Keefe, D.H. Simultaneous recording of stimulus-frequency and distortion-product otoacoustic emission input-output functions in human ears. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2005, 117, 818–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Schairer, K.S.; Fitzpatrick, D.; Keefe, D.H. Input-output functions for stimulus-frequency otoacoustic emissions in normal-hearing adult ears. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2003, 114, 944–966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Abdala, C.; Kalluri, R. Exploiting dual otoacoustic emission sources. In Proceedings of the 12th International Workshop on the Mechanics of Hearing, Cape Sounio, Greece, 23–29 June 2014; Corey, D.P., Karavitaki, K.D., Eds.; AIP: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  48. Jäncke, L. The Hand Performance Test with a Modified Time Limit Instruction Enables the Examination of Hand Performance Asymmetries in Adults. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1996, 82, 735–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Bidelman, G.M.; Krishnan, A. Effects of reverberation on brainstem representation of speech in musicians and non-musicians. Brain Res. 2010, 1355, 112–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Bidelman, G.M.; Schug, J.M.; Jennings, S.G.; Bhagat, S.P. Psychophysical auditory filter estimates reveal sharper cochlear tuning in musicians. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2014, 136, EL33–EL39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Sokal, R.R.; Rohlf, F.J. Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Research, 4th ed.; WH.Freeman: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  52. Bilger, R.C.; Matthies, M.L.; Hammel, D.R.; Demorest, M.E. Gentic implication of gender differences in the prevalence of spontaneous otoacoustic emissions. J. Speech. Hear. Res. 1990, 33, 418–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Xia, A.; Song, Y.; Wang, R.; Gao, S.S.; Clifton, W.; Raphael, P.; Chao, S.-I.; Pereira, F.A.; Groves, A.K.; Oghalai, J.S. Prestin Regulation and Function in Residual Outer Hair Cells after Noise-Induced Hearing Loss. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e82602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Lüders, D.; Gonçalves, C.G.D.O.; Lacerda, A.B.D.M.; Ribas, A.; De Conto, J. Music students: Conventional hearing thresholds and at high frequencies. Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. 2014, 80, 296–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. An example of the presence of stimulus frequency otoacoustic emission (SFOAE). Probe level was 50 dB sound pressure levels (SPL), and the center frequency (CF) is 4 kHz. Red line indicates SFOAE and blue line indicates the background noise. The presence of SFOAE is represented in the black dashed box.
Figure 1. An example of the presence of stimulus frequency otoacoustic emission (SFOAE). Probe level was 50 dB sound pressure levels (SPL), and the center frequency (CF) is 4 kHz. Red line indicates SFOAE and blue line indicates the background noise. The presence of SFOAE is represented in the black dashed box.
Brainsci 09 00255 g001
Figure 2. The detection rates of SFOAE between groups of female and male under four conditions: 2 probe levels × 2 probe frequencies. (A) fp = 1 kHz; Lp = 30 dB SPL; (B) fp = 1 kHz; Lp = 50 dB SPL; (C) fp = 4 kHz; Lp = 30 dB SPL; (D) fp = 4 kHz; Lp = 50 dB SPL. * p < 0.05, n.s., not significant (chi-square tests).
Figure 2. The detection rates of SFOAE between groups of female and male under four conditions: 2 probe levels × 2 probe frequencies. (A) fp = 1 kHz; Lp = 30 dB SPL; (B) fp = 1 kHz; Lp = 50 dB SPL; (C) fp = 4 kHz; Lp = 30 dB SPL; (D) fp = 4 kHz; Lp = 50 dB SPL. * p < 0.05, n.s., not significant (chi-square tests).
Brainsci 09 00255 g002
Figure 3. The mean values of pure tone audiometry (PTA) for musicians and non-musicians as a function of fps. * p < 0.05, n.s., not significant (two-factor Scheirer–Ray–Hare test). Vertical bars represent the standard errors of mean.
Figure 3. The mean values of pure tone audiometry (PTA) for musicians and non-musicians as a function of fps. * p < 0.05, n.s., not significant (two-factor Scheirer–Ray–Hare test). Vertical bars represent the standard errors of mean.
Brainsci 09 00255 g003
Figure 4. The mean values of SFOAE amplitudes for musicians and non-musicians as a function of fps. (A) Mean SFOAE fine structure at the probe frequency centered low frequency: 1 kHz; (B) mean SFOAE fine structure at the probe frequency centered high frequency: 4 kHz * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, n.s., not significant (two-factor Scheirer–Ray–Hare test). Vertical bars represent the standard errors of mean.
Figure 4. The mean values of SFOAE amplitudes for musicians and non-musicians as a function of fps. (A) Mean SFOAE fine structure at the probe frequency centered low frequency: 1 kHz; (B) mean SFOAE fine structure at the probe frequency centered high frequency: 4 kHz * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, n.s., not significant (two-factor Scheirer–Ray–Hare test). Vertical bars represent the standard errors of mean.
Brainsci 09 00255 g004
Figure 5. The SFOAE input/output function and the gain function at 4 kHz for one subject. (A) The SFOAE input/output function; (B) the SFOAE gain function. The gain function was transformed from the SFOAE input/output function by the SFOAE magnitude at each Lp subtracting (in dB) corresponding probe level. The red line represents the fitting curve of SFOAE strength. Three parameters—peak strength (PS), compression threshold (CT), and compression strength (CS)—were derived from this fit.
Figure 5. The SFOAE input/output function and the gain function at 4 kHz for one subject. (A) The SFOAE input/output function; (B) the SFOAE gain function. The gain function was transformed from the SFOAE input/output function by the SFOAE magnitude at each Lp subtracting (in dB) corresponding probe level. The red line represents the fitting curve of SFOAE strength. Three parameters—peak strength (PS), compression threshold (CT), and compression strength (CS)—were derived from this fit.
Brainsci 09 00255 g005
Figure 6. All participants’ three parameters and their linear regression fits at the CFs of 1 (left) and 4 kHz (right). PS, CT, and CS are plotted as functions of probe frequencies (from first to last panel). The triangles denote the values of PS, CT, and CS at the CFs of 1 kHz. The circles denote the values of PS, CT, and CS at the CFs of 4 kHz. (A) PS at 1 kHz; (B) PS at 4 kHz; (C) CT at 1 kHz; (D) CT at 4 kHz; (E) CS at 1 kHz; (F) CS at 4 kHz.
Figure 6. All participants’ three parameters and their linear regression fits at the CFs of 1 (left) and 4 kHz (right). PS, CT, and CS are plotted as functions of probe frequencies (from first to last panel). The triangles denote the values of PS, CT, and CS at the CFs of 1 kHz. The circles denote the values of PS, CT, and CS at the CFs of 4 kHz. (A) PS at 1 kHz; (B) PS at 4 kHz; (C) CT at 1 kHz; (D) CT at 4 kHz; (E) CS at 1 kHz; (F) CS at 4 kHz.
Brainsci 09 00255 g006
Table 1. The detection rates of SFOAE with the different factors of gender at the conditions of probe frequency and probe level.
Table 1. The detection rates of SFOAE with the different factors of gender at the conditions of probe frequency and probe level.
FactorConditionGroup aDetection Ratio (%)χ2 ValueP ValueES
Genderfp = 1 kHz Lp = 30 dB SPLF713.8990.048 *0.583
M92
fp = 1 kHz Lp = 50 dB SPLF960.0030.9540.016
M96
fp = 4 kHz Lp = 30 dB SPLF925.0240.025 *0.668
M65
fp = 4 kHz Lp = 50 dB SPLF634.6120.032 *0.637
M88
a F denotes female, M denotes male; ES, effect size; * p < 0.05.
Table 2. Simple effect results between the factors of group difference and frequency.
Table 2. Simple effect results between the factors of group difference and frequency.
Frequency (Hz)GroupdfH Valuep Value
250 /110.2760.001 **
500 16.4840.011 *
100010.0770.781
200010.1790.672
400010.0760.783
800010.0120.913
/musicians57.9430.159
non-musicians519.1410.002 **
* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
Table 3. Statistics results of PS, CT, and CS at fp values centered 1 and 4 kHz.
Table 3. Statistics results of PS, CT, and CS at fp values centered 1 and 4 kHz.
MetricsMean (SD) at 1 kHz Mean (SD a) at 4 kHzt/z Valuep Value ES
PS −22.773(4.080)−22.985(3.789)0.1200.9050.054
CT 28.627(1.969)32.062(2.010)−3.8610.001 **1.754
CS0.648(0.233)0.720(0.266)−0.4160.677/
a SD denotes standard deviation. ** p < 0.01.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wang, Y.; Qi, Z.; Yu, M.; Wang, J.; Chen, R. Characteristic of Stimulus Frequency Otoacoustic Emissions: Detection Rate, Musical Training Influence, and Gain Function. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 255. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9100255

AMA Style

Wang Y, Qi Z, Yu M, Wang J, Chen R. Characteristic of Stimulus Frequency Otoacoustic Emissions: Detection Rate, Musical Training Influence, and Gain Function. Brain Sciences. 2019; 9(10):255. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9100255

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wang, Yao, Zhihang Qi, Mengmeng Yu, Jinhai Wang, and Ruijuan Chen. 2019. "Characteristic of Stimulus Frequency Otoacoustic Emissions: Detection Rate, Musical Training Influence, and Gain Function" Brain Sciences 9, no. 10: 255. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9100255

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop