Next Article in Journal
Endovascular Thrombectomy Following Acute Ischemic Stroke: A Single-Center Case Series and Critical Review of the Literature
Next Article in Special Issue
Early Life Stress, Nicotinic Acetylcholine Receptors and Alcohol Use Disorders
Previous Article in Journal
A Program for Solving the Brain Ischemia Problem
Previous Article in Special Issue
Neuroadaptive Changes Associated with Smoking: Structural and Functional Neural Changes in Nicotine Dependence
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sex-Specific Brain Deficits in Auditory Processing in an Animal Model of Cocaine-Related Schizophrenic Disorders

by
Patricia A. Broderick
1,2,3,* and
Taylor Rosenbaum
4
1
Department of Physiology, Pharmacology & Neuroscience, The Sophie Davis School of Biomedical Education, The City College of New York, The City University of New York, New York, NY 10031, USA
2
Doctoral Program, Department of Biology, The City University of New York Graduate Center, New York, NY 10016, USA
3
Department of Neurology, NYU Langone Medical Center, NYU Comprehensive Epilepsy Center, New York, NY 10016, USA
4
School of Arts and Sciences, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2013, 3(2), 504-520; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci3020504
Submission received: 23 January 2013 / Revised: 4 March 2013 / Accepted: 20 March 2013 / Published: 10 April 2013
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Addiction and Neuroadaptation)

Abstract

:
Cocaine is a psychostimulant in the pharmacological class of drugs called Local Anesthetics. Interestingly, cocaine is the only drug in this class that has a chemical formula comprised of a tropane ring and is, moreover, addictive. The correlation between tropane and addiction is well-studied. Another well-studied correlation is that between psychosis induced by cocaine and that psychosis endogenously present in the schizophrenic patient. Indeed, both of these psychoses exhibit much the same behavioral as well as neurochemical properties across species. Therefore, in order to study the link between schizophrenia and cocaine addiction, we used a behavioral paradigm called Acoustic Startle. We used this acoustic startle paradigm in female versus male Sprague-Dawley animals to discriminate possible sex differences in responses to startle. The startle method operates through auditory pathways in brain via a network of sensorimotor gating processes within auditory cortex, cochlear nuclei, inferior and superior colliculi, pontine reticular nuclei, in addition to mesocorticolimbic brain reward and nigrostriatal motor circuitries. This paper is the first to report sex differences to acoustic stimuli in Sprague-Dawley animals (Rattus norvegicus) although such gender responses to acoustic startle have been reported in humans (Swerdlow et al. 1997 [1]). The startle method monitors pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) as a measure of the loss of sensorimotor gating in the brain's neuronal auditory network; auditory deficiencies can lead to sensory overload and subsequently cognitive dysfunction. Cocaine addicts and schizophrenic patients as well as cocaine treated animals are reported to exhibit symptoms of defective PPI (Geyer et al., 2001 [2]). Key findings are: (a) Cocaine significantly reduced PPI in both sexes. (b) Females were significantly more sensitive than males; reduced PPI was greater in females than in males. (c) Physiological saline had no effect on startle in either sex. Thus, the data elucidate gender-specificity to the startle response in animals. Finally, preliminary studies show the effect of cocaine on acoustic startle in tandem with effects on estrous cycle. The data further suggest that hormones may play a role in these sex differences to acoustic startle reported herein.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

1.1. Cocaine

Cocaine is found in the leaves of the coca shrub; it is an ester of benzoic acid and methyl-ecgonine; ecgonine is an amino acid alcohol closely related to tropine, the amino alcohol of atropine. Atropine is a muscarinic anticholinergic agent used to block nerve impulses similarly to cocaine’s mechanism of action. In fact, atropine is sometimes called “possibly addictive”. However, to emphasize the role of cocaine as a local anesthetic, cocaine does have the same fundamental structure as the other classical synthetic local anesthetics given the caveat that its dramatic difference from the other local anesthetics lies in its “tropane” component (Figure 1). It is thought that tropane is the source of cocaine’s addictive properties.
Figure 1. The figure depicts the cocaine molecule (A) and the atropine molecule (B) as seen in [3]. Note the tropane ring in bold. Atropine, although not a schedule II drug listed in the Drug Enforcement Agency Codes as is cocaine, is reported to be a “possibly addictive drug”. Atropine addiction occurs especially if it is used with another drug which has inherent addictive properties.
Figure 1. The figure depicts the cocaine molecule (A) and the atropine molecule (B) as seen in [3]. Note the tropane ring in bold. Atropine, although not a schedule II drug listed in the Drug Enforcement Agency Codes as is cocaine, is reported to be a “possibly addictive drug”. Atropine addiction occurs especially if it is used with another drug which has inherent addictive properties.
Brainsci 03 00504 g001
Cocaine blocks nerve impulses to produce local anesthetic properties. In this regard, cocaine possesses local vasoconstrictor properties secondary to inhibition of local norepinephrine reuptake; this enables cocaine to be the only local anesthetic that has its own vasoconstrictor properties. These unique properties make cocaine an effective local anesthetic. However, its cardiovascular effects and its proven potential for abuse have steadily decreased its clinical use [4].
Cocaine acts in the mesocorticolimbic neuronal pathway to produce euphoria, brain reward (joie de vivre). Cocaine is a reinforcer and reinforcement is a cyclic phenomenon. Cocaine-induced euphoria is due at least in part to the inhibition of dopamine reuptake in mesocorticolimbic neuronal circuitry of brain and the dopamine transporter is believed to perform the mechanism by which dopamine remains in the synapse. Molecular pharmacology studies show that the tropane ring influences the reinforcing properties of cocaine [5,6].
Moreover, the reinforcing brain reward properties of cocaine are due, at least in part, to increased extracellular release of dopamine and serotonin in mesocorticolimbic circuitry, as detected by electron transfer with Neuromolecular Imaging (NMI). Also, with NMI, dopamine and serotonin antagonists e.g., atypical and typical antipsychotic agents were shown to block cocaine-induced release of monoamines while blocking psychostimulant behavior at the same time [7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15]. Similar effects of cocaine on extracellular levels of dopamine and serotonin using the method of microdialysis, are published [16,17,18,19].

1.2. Schizophrenia and Cocaine Psychoses

A study by Chapman et al. [20] brought about a view of schizophrenia which related schizophrenia to the process of cognition in the cerebral areas of attention and perception. Thus, the notion of schizophrenia as a “breakdown of the ego” [21] was revised. Schizophrenia is currently defined as a basic disorder of cognition [22,23,24], likely leading to sensory overload. To paraphrase what schizophrenic patients report as sensory overload, a schizophrenic patient may remark, “too many things are coming into my mind”; “the thoughts feel bigger”; “the person I am talking to looks bigger”. Therefore, the inability to gate sensory stimuli and channel the flow of sensory information to avoid sensory overload may well describe the schizophrenic/cocaine brain [25].
The hypothesis that correlates schizophrenic and cocaine psychoses is rooted in their neurochemical and behavioral similarities. Indeed, reports of cocaine-induced psychotic patients are happening at an alarming rate [26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39]. According to the mental health clinician’s handbook, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, the broad category of cocaine-related disorders can be subdivided into cocaine use and cocaine disorders e.g. delusions (hanging on to beliefs that are clearly nonexistent) and hallucinations (hearing and seeing things) [40]. A clinical case of cocaine psychosis presenting with hallucinations and delusions, is reported [41]. This article reports a patient in the emergency department who had binged crack cocaine. Patient heard “voices in his head who were telling him to kill himself and the patient continued to believe there was a plot against him”. These auditory hallucinations have been given the definition, “strange perception of sounds” [42].
Thus, cocaine psychosis is a major psychopathology [43] and hyperfunction of dopaminergic systems is critical to drug-induced psychosis [44]. Too, what complicates the situation further, are data which show that about 50% of the patients who suffer from schizophrenia are also substance abusers at some time during their illness [45,46]. It is now known that substances with psychomimetic properties such as cocaine are used and abused worldwide and consumption of such substances can and will induce psychotic reactions including primary psychotic disease [47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58].

1.3. Responses to Auditory Stimuli, Startle and Pre-Pulse Inhibition (PPI)

Acoustic startle is said to, be protective [59,60], involve emotion [61], be aversive [62], and be used as a simple defensive reflex behavior [63]. This response or reflex is elicited by a sudden and intense acoustic stimulus. Facial and skeletal muscles are activated within a few milliseconds leading to a whole body flinch in rodents [59]. Although startle responses are reflective responses that can be reliably elicited, they are not stereotypic [60]. It is interesting to note that the startle response began with studies of the classical inhibitory tactile reflex and was further advanced by studying tactile reflexes in the sensory system of brain [64,65,66,67,68].
The term “pre-pulse inhibition” (PPI) was proposed by Ison [69]. Startle modification via auditory modulation of brain by startle became more interesting over ensuing decades due to its effects on sensorimotor gating [64]. Several psychiatric disorders, e.g., generalized anxiety disorder [70] and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [71] are associated with deficits in sensorimotor gating and in particular, schizophrenic-related disorders are significantly affected by sensorimotor gating mechanism [72,73]. Normal PPI is a decrease in the startle amplitude in response to a strong sensory stimulus preceded 30–500 ms by a weak stimulus which is the “pre-pulse” [74]. It is important to note that it is a further reduction in PPI that leads to a brain deficit in the auditory system of the central nervous system.

1.4. Auditory Stimuli-Startle Tests: Sex-Related Differences

Faraday and Grunberg performed animal studies on auditory stimuli wherein no drug was injected; instead the effects of proper acclimation times were studied and the use of proper acclimation times to reduce stress factors were emphasized [75]. The present paper is the first study of responses to auditory stimuli directly focused on sex differences and sex differences in the presence of cocaine in Sprague-Dawley animals. Thus, since cocaine and schizophrenia are known to be linked behaviorally and by neurochemical means, the purpose of this paper is to further study this link using a simple behavioral paradigm called Acoustic Startle. Moreover we explored the acoustic startle paradigm in male versus female Sprague-Dawley animals to discriminate possible sex differences in the auditory response to startle.

2. Experimental Section

2.1. Animals

Adult male and female Sprague-Dawley rats (2 months of age, weight matched for sex) were purchased from Charles River Laboratories, USA. Animals were housed in the Animal Care Facility under the auspices of the City College Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) in compliance with National Institute of Health (NIH) guidelines. Food (Purina Rat Chow) and water was available ad libitum.
Males were housed separately from females, three animals (male or female) per chamber. Studies were performed in groups of six, equal numbers of females and males during two seasons (Fall/Spring), with the exception of an additional group consisting only of six females, isolated from males during the experimental period.
The auditory stimulus, acoustic startle response, termed pre-pulse inhibition (PPI), consisted of 4 trials, comprised of three tests, Habituation, Injection and Post-injection. The habituation period which was the first test was preceded by a 5 min acclimation period and each of the three tests was preceded by an acute injection of cocaine or physiological saline (animal weight in kilograms). The cocaine dosages administered were 10.0 mg/kg, 20.0 mg/kg and 40.0 mg/kg (dissolved in distilled water) and injected intraperitoneally (i.p.).
The acoustic startle chamber is an SR-LAB™ model (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA, USA) automated to record the startle response of small animals. The chamber consisted of a cylindrical enclosure on a plexiglass base. Background noise, pre-pulse stimuli and startle stimuli were controlled by the SR-LAB™ software. Startle responses were transduced by a piezoelectric accelerometer mounted on a seismic sensor plate platform, located below the cylinder in which the animal is placed.
Startle response to auditory stimuli via muscle movements was studied by pre-pulse inhibition (PPI). Data were recorded as potential in voltage and were concatenated for compatibility with Excel software. %PPI was calculated using the following formula:
%PPI = 100 − [(mean PPI/mean startle only) × 100]
There were 25 sessions in each trial with each trial consisting of 3 tests; there was a 15 min interval between each trial. Each session consisted of 5 types which were randomly placed throughout the trial, but each trial used the same format, depicted below in tabular form (Table 1).
Table 1. Protocol for Delivery of Auditory Stimuli in the Acoustic Startle Paradigm.
Table 1. Protocol for Delivery of Auditory Stimuli in the Acoustic Startle Paradigm.
TypeNo StimulusPrepulsePulse
175 decibels (broadband)--
2--120 decibels
3-95 decibels120 decibels
4-105 decibels120 decibels
5-115 decibels120 decibels

2.2. Data Analysis

Data were analyzed by Graph Pad Software Inc. and PRISM (La Jolla, CA, USA). Statistical significance between and within gender (sex) and dosage of cocaine was delineated via Two-Way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA). Data were considered significant at p < 0.05 and above. The expected baseline for acoustic startle response is defined by SR-LAB™ as 90th percentile.

3. Results

3.1. Sex Differences in Responses during the Acoustic Startle Paradigm after Cocaine

Group A (Figure 2) showed statistically significant differences in the dose response interaction of cocaine in females and males (F(2,12) = 11.15, p < 0.002). In Group A, in females and males, the 20 mg/kg dose of cocaine showed a maximum effect in pre-pulse attenuation of acoustic startle. Group B (Figure 3) female versus male analysis showed dosage × gender interaction significance (F(2,12) = 6.81 and p < 0.01). Gender alone significance was analyzed, significance was found (F(1,12) = 11.90, p < 0.005). Cocaine dosage alone was significant as well (F(2,12) = 8.00, p < 0.006). In Group B, in females, the 20 mg/kg i.p. dose of cocaine resulted in a maximum attenuation of pre-pulse inhibition in the injection test. The within gender data gave the following results; Group A males versus Group B males showed between group significance, male × male (F(1,12) = 14.91, p < 0.002). Group A females with Group B females, females × females, showed cocaine dosage significance; (F(2,12) = 15.76, p < 0.0004).
Figure 2. (Fall, Group A) (n = 6) shows the effect of cocaine on females (left) and males (right). Statistically significant differences occurred in the interaction between and within dosage.
Figure 2. (Fall, Group A) (n = 6) shows the effect of cocaine on females (left) and males (right). Statistically significant differences occurred in the interaction between and within dosage.
Brainsci 03 00504 g002
Figure 3. (Spring, Group B) (n = 6) shows the effect of cocaine on females (left) and males (right). Statistically significant differences occurred in the interaction between and within dosage and gender (sex).
Figure 3. (Spring, Group B) (n = 6) shows the effect of cocaine on females (left) and males (right). Statistically significant differences occurred in the interaction between and within dosage and gender (sex).
Brainsci 03 00504 g003
A significant dosage effect of cocaine in its attenuation of pre-pulse inhibition in Group C (isolated females) was seen at 20 mg/kg in the injection and post-injection tests in summation of 4 trials [95% Confidence Limits (p < 0.05)] (Figure 4). Given the caveat that different numbers of subjects comprised Group C and Group A females, still no female × female significance occurred. Nonetheless, Group C versus Group B, female × female comparison, exhibited a cocaine interaction significance in dosage (F(2,12) = 6.80, p < 0.01).
Figure 4. Group C (n = 6) shows the effect of cocaine in the acoustic startle paradigm in females. Females were isolated from males during the experimental period.
Figure 4. Group C (n = 6) shows the effect of cocaine in the acoustic startle paradigm in females. Females were isolated from males during the experimental period.
Brainsci 03 00504 g004

3.2. Saline Control

The below line graph (Left)/bar graph (Right) is representative of an n value of 4 female and 4 male animals treated with saline (n = 8) (Figure 5). Analysis of the saline data shows an interaction between gender and trials that is not significant, (F(2,12) = 0.94, p < 0.42). This represents 10.63% of variance. Gender was not significant either, (F(1,12) = 3.42, p < 0.1). Gender was 19.27% of variance. Trials were not significant after administration of saline, (F(2,12) = 0.23, p < 0.80). Trials represent 2.55% of total variance.
Figure 5. Shows the effect of saline administration to animals during acoustic startle.
Figure 5. Shows the effect of saline administration to animals during acoustic startle.
Brainsci 03 00504 g005

3.3. Summary of Groups A and B: Are There Seasonal Sex Differences in Acoustic Startle Responses?

Simply stated, the animals in the A group exhibited greater dosage interaction than sex interaction, whereas the animals in group B, exhibited significant dosage and sex differences. The studies were performed in different sets of animals seasonally. Nonetheless, whether or not the terminology, seasons, can be applied to the present studies, remains to be seen until further studies, using season as a variable, are explored.

4. Discussion

4.1. Sex Differences, Cocaine and the Estrous Cycle

Cocaine suppressed the pre-pulse inhibitory response to acoustic startle stimuli in females and males. The data confirm reports of cocaine’s ability to suppress pre-pulse inhibition in male animals ([76], see review [2]). This is the first report to show pre-pulse attenuation to acoustic startle by cocaine in female Sprague-Dawley animals. Females were more sensitive to startle than were males supporting results reported in female schizophrenic patients [77].
The data confirm previous studies that show sex differences to the effects of cocaine using a multiple of different methods [78,79,80,81,82]. Moreover, studies using the self-administration paradigm reported that estrogen plays a significant role in acquiring a desire for cocaine [83]. Ovarian hormones also play a significant role in the response to auditory stimuli via startle and stages of the estrous cycle stages have different responses to acoustic startle stimuli dependent upon the particular stage. In one study, pre-pulse inhibition was significantly reduced in the luteal as compared with the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle [84]. The suppression in the inhibitory response to auditory startle stimuli as measured by pre-pulse inhibition was most notable in the period corresponding to midluteal elevations in both estrogen and progesterone. A role for hormonal interaction during acoustic startle responses in females is suggested by preliminary studies in this laboratory. We performed tests of acoustic startle in concert with pre- and post-studies of estrous cycle stages, using vaginal smears. In these preliminary studies, administration of cocaine (20 mg/kg i.p.), changed the estrous cycle stage from metestrus to proestrus while pre-pulse inhibition in the same animal at the same time was dramatically suppressed. It is previously reported by others that estrous cycle stages are role players in cocaine addiction [85,86,87,88]. Strain differences are also critical to acoustic startle responses [85,86,87,88,89,90,91].

4.2. Pharmacological Manipulation of Startle

Dopamine, serotonin, somatostatin and neurotensin among others are involved in the startle response [92,93,94,95,96]. Importantly, depletions of neurotransmitters, receptor subtypes and genetic alterations of neurotransmitters provide excellent means to study acoustic startle mechanisms. Insofar as dopamine is concerned, D1R was found to be necessary for disrupting cocaine startle whereas D2R only partially contributes to the disruptive effects of cocaine-induced startle suppression [76]. Humby et al. [16] studied extracellular concentrations of dopamine in nucleus accumbens in male animals using the microdialysis method.

4.3. Brain Circuitry Mechanisms for the Auditory Startle Stimulus and Response

Rudomin in 2002 [97] reported that sensorimotor gating often occurs via presynaptic inhibition of the activated sensory neurons [97,98,99]. Neural circuits for acoustic startle to auditory stimuli have been postulated by Koch et al. [100,101,102,103,104], Fendt et al. [105] and Leumann et al. [106]. Neuronal links between ventral tegmental area and the causal pontine reticular nucleus, leading to a link between the mesocorticolimbic brain reward circuitry, and the pons was suggested by the Koch group. The Fendt group included the mesocorticolimbic and nigrostriatal pathway as participants in the acoustic startle circuitry. Leumann et al. proposed a major role for acetylcholine and glutamine for respectively inhibiting and exciting the acoustic startle response [106].
The brain stem is of the utmost importance since lesion studies show that the inferior and superior colliculi, the pons and the caudal pontine reticular nucleus are critically important in the execution of the startle response. Lentner and Cohen studied the role of the inferior colliculus in the startle reflex as early as the late nineteen eighties [107]. All these neuronal networks, whether studied empirically or postulated theoretically, underlie the mechanism of action for acoustic startle and all must rely on the anatomic brain structure called the pons, seen below in Figure 6A,B.
Figure 6. (A) Depicts a longitudinal section of murine and/or human brain that shows the position of the pons in relation to cerebellum, midbrain and forebrain structures. (B) Depicts a microscopic cross section of the pons in the human brain that shows the position of the acoustic nerve.
Figure 6. (A) Depicts a longitudinal section of murine and/or human brain that shows the position of the pons in relation to cerebellum, midbrain and forebrain structures. (B) Depicts a microscopic cross section of the pons in the human brain that shows the position of the acoustic nerve.
Brainsci 03 00504 g006
Thus, the neural network underlying the mechanism of action of pre-pulse inhibition in the acoustic startle paradigm may include the nigrostriatal pathway, mesocorticolimbic circuitry linking the limbic cortex, striatum, pallidum and pontine reticular formation by monoamine, indoleamine, peptide and amino acid neurotransmitters. Of particular interest and germaine to this discussion is the close anatomic relationship between the ventrolateral tegmental nucleus and the pons. Such neuronal connections allow a rationale for associating emotions of reward and fear with the inhibitory and conversely, enhancing actions of pre-pulse inhibition during acoustic startle. Finally, further work is needed to delve into the neurochemical, hormonal and anatomical reasons for differences in the acoustic startle response in females and males.

5. Conclusions

This is the first report of sex-specific differences in the acoustic startle paradigm in the presence of cocaine in Sprague-Dawley animals (Rattus norvegicus). The results show that cocaine attenuated pre-pulse inhibition in females and males; females exhibited greater attenuation in pre-pulse inhibition than did males. Statistically significant responses to acoustic startle were seen in females and males within and between sex (gender) and dosage regimens. A role for hormonal interaction during acoustic startle responses is suggested by preliminary tests of acoustic startle in concert with pre- and post-studies of estrous cycle stages. The data are important because new strategic inroads for sex-specific diagnosis and treatment of cocaine addiction and related schizophrenic diseases are clarified.

Acknowledgements

Special thanks to the F.M. Kirby Foundation and the Broderick Brain Foundation for financial support. The authors appreciate the technical assistance of James Hordof, Lauren Malave, Waqas Saleem (Sophie Davis School of Biomedical Education, CCNY, CUNY) and Idaliene Hernandez (Hunter College, CUNY). Thanks also to Diego Buenaventura, CUNY Graduate Center PhD Biology Program and Stuyvesant High School student, Srilu Garikapati for manuscript preparation. The co-author, Taylor Rosenbaum, is presently an undergraduate student at Emory University. Taylor performed her high school Intel experience in our laboratory under the guidance of Broderick and her Intel mentor, Paul Gilmore of Millburn High School, NJ, USA.

References

  1. Swerdlow, N.R.; Hartman, P.L.; Auerbach, P.P. Changes in sensorimotor inhibition across the menstrual cycle: Implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 1997, 41, 452–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Geyer, M.A.; Kerbs-Thomson, K.; Braff, D.L.; Swerdlow, N.R. Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: A decade in review. Psychopharmacology 2001, 156, 117–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Atropine. Available online: http://www.newworldencyclopedia.org/entry/Atropine (accessed on 28 December 2012).
  4. Goodman, L.S.; Gilman, A.G.; Limbird, L.E.; Milinoff, P.B.; Ruddon, R.W. Goodman and Gilman’s: The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics, 9th ed; Mcgraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  5. Parnas, M.L.; Gaffaney, J.D.; Zou, M.F.; Lever, J.R.; Newman, A.H.; Vaughan, R.A. Labeling of Dopamine Transporter Transmembrane Domain 1 with the Tropane Ligan N-[4-Azido-3-[125I]iodophenyl)butyl]-2β-carbomethoxy-3β-(4-chlorophenyl)tropane Implicates Proximity of Cocaine and Substrate Active Sites. Mol. Pharmacol. 2008, 73, 1141–1150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Kopaitic, T.A.; Liu, Y.; Surratt, C.K.; Donavan, D.M.; Newman, A.H.; Katz, J.L. Dopamine Transporter-Dependent and -Independent Striatal Binding of the Benztropine Analog JHW 007, a cocaine Antagonist with Low Abuse Liability. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2010, 335, 703–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Broderick, P.A. Cocaine: On-line analysis of an accumbens amine neural basis for psychomotor behavior. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1991, 40, 959–968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Broderick, P.A. In vivo voltammetric studies on release mechanisms for cocaine with γ-butyrolactone. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1991, 40, 969–975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Broderick, P.A. Cocaine’s colocalized effects on synaptic serotonin and dopamine in ventral tegmentum in a reinforcement paradigm. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1992, 42, 889–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Broderick, P.A. Distinguishing effects of cocaine (IV) and (SC) on mesoaccumbens dopamine and serotonin release with chloral hydrate anesthesia. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1992, 43, 929–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Broderick, P.A. In vivo electrochemical studies of gradient effects of (SC) cocaine on dopamine and serotonin release in dorsal striatum of conscious rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1993, 46, 973–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Broderick, P.A.; Kornak, E.P.; Eng, F.; Wechsler, R.W. Real time detection of acute (IP) cocaine-enhanced dopamine and serotonin release in ventrolateral nucleus accumbens of the behaving Norway rat. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1993, 46, 715–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Broderick, P.A.; Hope, O.; Okonji, C.; Rahni, D.N.; Zhou, Y. Clozapine and cocaine effects on dopamine and serotonin release in nucleus accumbens during psychostimulant behavior and withdrawal. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2004, 28, 157–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Broderick, P.A.; Olabisi, O.A.; Rahni, D.N.; Zhou, Y. Cocaine acts on accumbens monoamines and locomotor behavior via a 5-HT2A/2C receptor mechanism as shown by ketanserin: 24 hr follow-up studies. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2004, 28, 547–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Broderick, P.A.; Hope, O. Monoamines and motor responses are co-deficient in the Fawn-Hooded depressed animal model. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2006, 30, 887–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Humby, T.; Wilkinson, L.S.; Robbins, T.W.; Geyer, M.A. Prepulses inhibit startle-induced reductions of extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens of rat. J. Neurosci. 1996, 16, 2149–2156. [Google Scholar]
  17. Bradberry, C.W.; Nobiletti, J.B.; Elsworth, J.D.; Murphy, B.; Jatlow, P.; Roth, R.H. Cocaine and cocaethylene: Microdialysis comparison of brain drug levels and effects on dopamine and serotonin. J. Neurochem. 1993, 60, 1429–1435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Parsons, L.H.; Justice, J.B., Jr. Serotonin and dopamine sensitization in the nucleus accumbens, ventral tegmental area, and dorsal raphe nucleus following repeated cocaine administration. J. Neurochem. 1993, 61, 1611–1619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Morgan, A.E.; Porter, S.P.; Clarkson, F.A.; Volkow, N.D.; Fowler, J.S.; Dewey, S.L. Direct approach for attenuating cocaine’s effects on extracellular dopamine: Targeting the dopamine transporter. Synapse 1997, 26, 423–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Chapman, T.; Freeman, T.; McGhie, A. Clinical research in schizophrenia; the psychotherapeutic approach. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 1959, 32, 75–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Bleuler, M. The concept of schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 1954, 5, 382–383. [Google Scholar]
  22. Freeman, T.; McGhie, A. The relevance of genetic psychology for the psychopathology of schizophrenia. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 1957, 30, 176–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. McGhie, A.; Chapman, J.; Lawson, J.S. Disturbances in Selective Attention in Schizophrenia. Proc. R. Soc. Med. 1964, 57, 419–422. [Google Scholar]
  24. McGhie, A.; Chapman, J. Disorders of attention and perception in early schizophrenia. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 1961, 34, 103–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hetrick, W.P.; Smith, D.A.; Spuriell, E.; Sandman, C.A.; Bunney, W.E. The putative phenomenological correlates of sensory gating: Factor analyses and a self-report rating scale. Schizophr. Res. 1997, 24, 17. [Google Scholar]
  26. Brady, K.T.; Lydiard, R.B.; Malcolm, R.; Ballenger, J.C. Cocaine-induced psychosis. J. Clin. Psychiatry 1991, 52, 509–512. [Google Scholar]
  27. Harris, D.; Batki, S.L. Stimulant psychosis: Symptom profile and acute clinical course. Am. J. Addict. 2000, 9, 28–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lysaker, P.; Bell, M.; Beam-Goulet, J.; Milstein, R. Relationship of positive and negative symptoms to cocaine abuse in schizophrenia. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1994, 182, 109–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Mendoza, R.; Miller, B.L.; Mena, I. Emergency room evaluation of cocaine-associated neuropsychiatric disorders. Recent Dev. Alcohol. 1992, 10, 73–87. [Google Scholar]
  30. Miller, B.L.; Mena, I.; Giombetti, R.; Villanueve-Meyer, J.; Djenderdjian, A.H. Neuropsychiatric effects of cocaine: SPECT Measurements. J. Addict. Dis. 1992, 11, 47–58. [Google Scholar]
  31. Mitchell, J.; Vierkant, A.D. Delusions and hallucinations of cocaine abusers and paranoid schizophrenics: A comparative study. J. Psychol. 1991, 125, 301–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Nambudin, D.E.; Young, R.C. A case of late-onset crack dependence and subsequent psychosis in the elderly. J. Subst. Abuse Treat. 1991, 8, 253–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Rosenthal, R.N.; Miner, C.R. Differential diagnosis of substance-induced psychosis and schizophrenia in patients with substance use disorders. Schizophr. Bull. 1997, 23, 187–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Rosse, R.B.; Collins, J.P., Jr.; Fay-McCarthy, M.; Alim, T.M.; Wyatt, R.J.; Deutsch, S.I. Phenomenologic comparison of the idiopathic psychosis of schizophrenia and drug-induced cocaine and phencyclidine psychoses: A retrospective study. Clin. Neuropharmacol. 1994, 17, 359–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Satel, S.L.; Edell, W.S. Cocaine-induced paranoia and psychosis proneness. Am. J. Psychiatry 1991, 148, 1708–1711. [Google Scholar]
  36. Serper, M.R.; Chou, J.C.; Allen, M.H.; Czobor, P.; Cancro, R. Symptomatic overlap of cocaine intoxication and acute schizophrenia at emergency presentation. Schizophr. Bull. 1999, 25, 387–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Sherer, M.A.; Kumor, K.M.; Cone, E.J.; Jaffe, J.H. Suspiciousness induced by four-hour intravenous infusions of cocaine. Preliminary findings. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1998, 45, 673–677. [Google Scholar]
  38. Taylor, W.A.; Staby, A.E. Acute treatment of alcohol and cocaine emergencies. Recent Dev. Alcohol. 1992, 10, 179–191. [Google Scholar]
  39. Tueth, M.J. High incidence of psychosis in cocaine intoxication and preventing violence. Am. J. Emerg. Med. 1993, 11, 676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Cocaine and related disorders. Available online: http://www.minddisorders.com/Br-Del/Cocaine-and-related-disorders.html (accessed on 28 December 2012).
  41. Nunes, J.V.; Broderick, P.A. Novel research translates to clinical cases of schizophrenic and cocaine psychosis. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2007, 3, 475–485. [Google Scholar]
  42. Waters, F. Auditory Hallucinations in Psychiatric Illness. Psychiatr. Times 2010, 27, 3. [Google Scholar]
  43. Satel, S.L.; Seibyl, J.P.; Charney, D.S. Prolonged cocaine psychosis implies underlying major psychopathology. J. Clin. Psychiatry 1991, 52, 349–350. [Google Scholar]
  44. Lieberman, J.A.; Kinon, B.I.; Loebel, A.D. Dopaminergic mechanisms in idiopathic and drug-induced psychoses. Schizophr. Bull. 1990, 16, 97–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Buckley, P.F. Substance abuse in schizophrenia: A review. J. Clin. Psychiatry 1998, 59, S26–S30. [Google Scholar]
  46. Muesser, K.T.; Nishith, P.; Tracy, H.; de Girolamo, J.; Molinaro, M. Expectations and motives for substance use in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1995, 21, 367–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Caton, C.L.; Samet, S.; Hasin, D.S. When acute-stage psychosis and substance use co-occur: Differentiating substance-induced and primary psychotic disorders. J. Psychiatr. Pract. 2000, 6, 256–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Batki, S.L.; Harris, D.S. Quantitative drug levels in stimulant psychosis: Relationship to symptom severity, catecholamines and hyperkinesia. Am. J. Addict. 2004, 13, 461–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Rosse, R.; Deutsch, S.; Chilton, M. Cocaine addicts prone to cocaine-induced psychosis have lower body mass index than cocaine addicts resistant to cocaine-induced psychosis—Implications for the cocaine model of psychosis proneness. Isr. J. Psychiatry Relat. Sci. 2005, 42, 45–50. [Google Scholar]
  50. Boutros, N.N.; Gooding, D.; Sundaresan, K.; Burroughs, S.; Johanson, C.E. Cocaine-dependence and cocaine-induced paranoia and mid-latency auditory evoked responses and sensory gating. Psychiatry Res. 2006, 145, 147–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Tang, Y.L.; Kranzler, H.R.; Gelernter, J.; Farrer, L.A.; Cubells, J.F. Comorbid psychiatric diagnoses and their association with cocaine-induced psychosis in cocaine-dependent subjects. Am. J. Addict. 2007, 16, 343–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Merlo, L.J.; Carnes, P.J.; Gold, M.S. Response to “Comorbid psychiatric diagnoses and their association with cocaine-induced psychosis in cocaine-dependent subjects”. Am. J. Addict. 2008, 17, 247–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Tang, Y.L.; Kranzler, H.R.; Gelernter, J.; Farrer, L.A.; Pearson, D.; Cubells, J.F. Transient Cocaine-Associated Behavioral Symptoms Rated with a New Instrument, the Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms for Cocaine-Induced Psychosis (SAPS-CIP). Am. J. Addict. 2009, 18, 339–345. [Google Scholar]
  54. Roncero, C.; Daigre, C.; Gonzalvo, B.; Valero, S.; Castells, X.; Grau-López, L.; Eiroa-Orosa, F.J.; Casas, M. Risk factors for cocaine-induced psychosis in cocaine-dependent patients. Eur. Psychiatry 2013, 28, 141–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Vorspan, F.; Bloch, V.; Brousse, G.; Bellais, L.; Gascon, J.; Lépine, J.P. Prospective assessment of transient cocaine-induced psychotic symptoms in a clinical setting. Am. J. Addict. 2011, 20, 535–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Fiorentini, A.; Volonteri, L.S.; Dragogna, F.; Rovera, C.; Maffini, M.; Mauri, M.C.; Altamura, C.A. Substance-Induced Psychoses: A Critical Review of the Literature. Curr. Drug Abuse Rev. 2011, 4, 228–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Vorspan, F.; Brousse, G.; Bloch, V.; Bellais, L.; Romo, L.; Guillem, E.; Coeuru, P.; Lépine, J.P. Cocaine-induced psychotic symptoms in French cocaine addicts. Psychiatry Res. 2013, 200, 1074–1076. [Google Scholar]
  58. Delavenne, H.; Garcia, F.D.; Lacoste, J.; Cortese, S.; Charles-Nicolas, A.; Ballon, N. Psychosis in a cocaine-dependent patient with ADHD during treatment with methylphenidate. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Davis, M. The mammalian startle response. In Neural Mechanisms of Startle Behavior; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1984; pp. 287–342. [Google Scholar]
  60. Valasamis, B.; Schmid, S. Habituation and Prepulse Inhibition of Acoustic Startle in Rodents. J. Vis. Exp. 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Lang, P.J.; Davis, M. Emotion, motivation, and the brain: Reflex foundations in animal and human research. Prog. Brain Res. 2006, 156, 3–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Ilango, A.; Shumake, J.; Wetzel, W.; Scheich, H.; Ohl, F.W. The role of dopamine in the context of aversive stimuli with particular reference to acoustically signaled avoidance learning. Front. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 132. [Google Scholar]
  63. Parker, K.J.; Hyde, S.A.; Buckmaster, C.L.; Tanaka, S.M.; Brewster, K.K.; Schatzberg, A.F.; Lyons, D.M.; Woodward, S.H. Somatic and neuroendocrine responses to standard and biologically salient acoustic startle stimuli in monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011, 36, 547–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Hoffman, H.S.; Ison, J.R. Reflex modification in the domain of startle: I. Some empirical findings and their implications for how the nervous system processes sensory input. Psychol. Rev. 1980, 87, 175–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Peak, H. Time order error in successive judgments and in reflexes. I. Inhibition of the judgment and the reflex. J. Exp. Psychol. 1939, 25, 535–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Hoffman, H.S.; Fleshler, M. Startle reaction: Modification by background acoustic stimulation. Science 1963, 141, 928–930. [Google Scholar]
  67. Hoffman, H.S.; Searle, J.L. Acoustic variables in the modification of startle reaction in the rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1965, 60, 53–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Pickney, L.A. Inhibition of the startle reflex in the rat by prior tactile stimulation. Anim. Learn. Behav. 1976, 4, 467–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Ison, J.R.; Hammond, G.R. Modification of the startle reflex in the rat by changes in the auditory and visual environments. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1971, 75, 435–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Ray, W.J.; Molnar, C.; Aikins, D.; Yamasaki, A.; Newman, M.G.; Castonguay, L.; Borkovec, T.D. Startle response in generalized anxiety disorder. Depress Anxiety 2009, 26, 147–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Conzelmann, A.; Mucha, R.F.; Jacob, C.P.; Weyers, P.; Romanos, J.; Gerdes, A.B.; Baehne, C.G.; Boreatti-Hümmer, A.; Heine, M.; Alpers, G.W.; et al. Abnormal affective responsiveness in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Subtype differences. Biol. Psychiatry 2009, 65, 578–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Geyer, M.A.; Braff, D.L. Startle habituation and sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia and related animal models. Schizophr. Bull. 1987, 13, 643–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Swerdlow, N.R.; Caine, S.B.; Braff, D.L.; Geyer, M.A. The neural substrates of sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex: A review of recent findings and their implications. J. Psychopharmacol. 1992, 6, 176–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Ralph, R.J.; Paulus, M.P.; Fumagalli, F.; Caron, M.G.; Geyer, M.A. Prepulse inihibition deficits and perseverative motor patterns in dopamine transporter knock-out mice: Differential effects of D1 and D2 receptor antagonists. J. Neurosci. 2001, 21, 305–313. [Google Scholar]
  75. Faraday, M.M.; Grunberg, N.E. The importance of acclimation in acoustic startle amplitude and pre-pulse inhibition testing of male and female rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2000, 66, 375–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Doherty, J.M.; Masten, V.L.; Powell, S.B.; Ralph, R.J.; Klamer, D.; Low, M.J.; Geyer, M.A. Contributions of dopamine D1, D2, and D3 receptor subtypes to the disruptive effects of cocaine on prepulse inhibition in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008, 33, 2648–2656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Meincke, U.; Light, G.A.; Geyer, M.A.; Braff, D.L.; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E. Sensitization and habituation of the acoustic startle reflex in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2004, 126, 51–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Quiñones-Jenab, V.; Ho, A.; Schlussman, S.D.; Franck, J.; Kreek, M.J. Estrous cycle differences in cocaine-induced stereotypic and locomotor behaviors in Fischer rats. Behav. Brain Res. 1999, 101, 15–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Quiñones-Jenab, V.; Perrotti, L.I.; Fabian, S.J.; Chin, J.; Russo, S.J.; Jenab, S. Endocrinological basis of sex differences in cocaine-induced behavioral responses. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2001, 937, 140–171. [Google Scholar]
  80. Broderick, P.A.; Ho, H.; Huang, J.; Wat, K.; Shahidullah, J.; Murthy, V.; Wenning, L.; Buchheim, L. Caffeine may neuroprotect against cocaine-induced dopamine release in accumbens of female rats. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 2007, 33. Program Poster 767.7. [Google Scholar]
  81. Nesbitt, J.; Raju, C.; Vidal, L.; Alimova, A.; Steiner, J.; Broderick, P.A.; Friedman, E. Sexual dimorphism studies of cocaine and caffeine in conditioned place preference: Estrous cycle stage and open-field behaviors. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 2008, 34. Program Poster 561.23/FF33. [Google Scholar]
  82. Broderick, P.A. Studies of oxidative stress mechanisms using a morphine/ascorbate animal model and novel N-stearoyl cerebroside and laurate sensors. J. Neural Transm. 2008, 115, 7–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Lynch, W.J.; Roth, M.E.; Mickelberg, J.L.; Carroll, M.E. Role of estrogen in the acquisition of intravenously self-administered cocaine in female rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2001, 68, 641–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Marcondes, F.K.; Bianchi, F.J.; Tanno, A.P. Determination of the estrous cycle phases of rats: Some helpful considerations. Braz. J. Biol. 2002, 62, 609–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Becker, J.B.; Molenda, H.; Hummer, D.L. Gender Differences in the Behavioral Responses to Cocaine and Amphetamine. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2001, 937, 172–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Becker, J.B.; Hu, M. Sex differences in drug abuse. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2008, 29, 36–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Wissman, A.M.; McCollum, A.F.; Huang, G.-Z.; Nikrodhanond, A.A.; Woolley, C.S. Sex differences and effects of cocaine on excitatory synapses in the nucleus accumbens. Neuropharmacology 2011, 61, 217–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Jackson, L.; Robinson, T.; Becker, J. Sex Differences and Hormonal Influences on Acquisition of Cocaine Self-Administration in Rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006, 31, 129–138. [Google Scholar]
  89. Lehmann, J.; Pryce, C.; Feldon, J. Sex differences in the acoustic startle response and prepulse inhibition in Wistar rats. Behav. Brain Res. 1999, 104, 113–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Błaszczyk, J.; Tajchert, K. Sex and strain differences of acoustic startle reaction development in adolescent albino Wistar and hooded rats. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. (Wars.) 1996, 56, 919–925. [Google Scholar]
  91. Plappert, C.F.; Rodenbücher, A.M.; Pilz, P.K. Effects of sex and estrous cycle on modulation of the acoustic startle response in mice. Physiol. Behav. 2005, 84, 585–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Fletcher, P.J.; Selhi, Z.F.; Azampanah, A.; Sills, T.L. Reduced brain serotonin activity disrupts prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex. Effects of 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine and p-chlorophenylalanine. Neuropsychopharmacology 2001, 24, 399–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Prinssen, E.P.; Assié, M.B.; Koek, W.; Kleven, M.S. Depletion of 5-HT disrupts prepulse inhibition in rats: Dependence on the magnitude of depletion, and reversal by a 5-HT precursor. Neuropsychopharmacology 2002, 26, 340–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Semenova, S.; Hoyer, D.; Geyer, M.A.; Markou, A. Somatostatin-28 modulates prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response, reward processes and spontaneous locomotor activity in rats. Neuropeptides 2010, 44, 421–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Kinkead, B.; Yan, F.; Owens, M.J.; Nemeroff, C.B. Endogenous neurotensin is involved in estrous cycle related alterations in prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex in female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2008, 33, 178–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Metherate, R.; Intskirveli, I.; Kawai, H.D. Nicotinic filtering of sensory processing in auditory cortex. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  97. Rudomin, P. Central control of information transmission through the intraspinal arborizations of sensory fibers examined 100 years after Ramón y Cajal. Prog. Brain Res. 2002, 136, 409–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Rose, P.K.; Scott, S.H. Sensory-motor control: A long-awaited behavioral correlate of presynaptic inhibition. Nat. Neurosci. 2003, 6, 1243–1245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Seki, K.; Perlmutter, S.I.; Fetz, E.E. Sensory input to primate spinal cord is presynaptically inhibited during voluntary movement. Nat. Neurosci. 2003, 6, 1309–1316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Koch, M. The neurobiology of startle. Prog. Neurobiol. 1999, 59, 107–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Koch, M.; Friauf, E. Glycine receptors in the caudal pontine reticular formation: Are they important for the inhibition of the acoustic startle response? Brain Res. 1995, 671, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Koch, M.; Schnitzler, H.U. The acoustic startle response in rats—circuits mediating evocation, inhibition and potentiation. Behav. Brain Res. 1997, 89, 35–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Koch, M.; Kungel, M.; Herbert, H. Cholinergic neurons in the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus are involved in the mediation of prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response in the rat. Exp. Brain Res. 1993, 97, 71–82. [Google Scholar]
  104. Koch, M.; Fendt, M.; Kretschmer, B.D. Role of the substantia nigra pars reticulata in sensorimotor gating, measured by prepulse inhibition of startle in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2000, 117, 153–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Fendt, M.; Li, L.; Yeomans, J.S. Brain stem circuits mediating prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2001, 156, 216–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Leumann, L.; Sterchi, D.; Vollenweider, F.; Ludewig, K.; Früh, H. A neural network approach to the acoustic startle reflex and prepulse inhibition. Brain Res. Bull. 2001, 56, 101–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Leitner, D.S.; Cohen, M.E. Role of the inferior colliculus in the inhibition of acoustic startle in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 1985, 34, 65–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Broderick, P.A.; Rosenbaum, T. Sex-Specific Brain Deficits in Auditory Processing in an Animal Model of Cocaine-Related Schizophrenic Disorders. Brain Sci. 2013, 3, 504-520. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci3020504

AMA Style

Broderick PA, Rosenbaum T. Sex-Specific Brain Deficits in Auditory Processing in an Animal Model of Cocaine-Related Schizophrenic Disorders. Brain Sciences. 2013; 3(2):504-520. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci3020504

Chicago/Turabian Style

Broderick, Patricia A., and Taylor Rosenbaum. 2013. "Sex-Specific Brain Deficits in Auditory Processing in an Animal Model of Cocaine-Related Schizophrenic Disorders" Brain Sciences 3, no. 2: 504-520. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci3020504

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop