Next Article in Journal
Why and How Women in Business Can Make Innovations in Light of the Sustainable Development Goals
Previous Article in Journal
The Role of Universities in Sustainability-Oriented Competencies Development: Insights from an Empirical Study on Polish Universities
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Evaluation of Consumers’ WTP for Service Recovery in Restaurants: Waiting Time Perspective

1
Department of Tourism, Food and Beverage Management, Chang Jung Christian University, Tainan City 71101, Taiwan
2
Business School, Yulin Normal University, Yulin 537000, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Adm. Sci. 2019, 9(3), 63; https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci9030063
Submission received: 15 June 2019 / Revised: 25 July 2019 / Accepted: 21 August 2019 / Published: 23 August 2019

Abstract

:
Restaurant service quality assessment has generally focused on the dining service. However, restaurant service begins with waiting for the meal. When service failure occurs due to a long waiting time, restaurant managers should adopt appropriate service recovery measures to retain consumers and reduce loss. This approach would allow them to mitigate customer dissatisfaction and negative impressions. In this study, we explore the importance of pre-processing service in the context of possible restaurant service crises and construct a restaurant service recovery model for willingness to pay (WTP) through the contingent valuation method (CVM) to measure the effects before and after implementing service recovery. Overall, several psychological and tangible service recovery measures are provided to enhance people’s willingness to extend their waiting time. Within our study group, relatively young participants showed a high willingness to extend their waiting time. Furthermore, the opportunity cost of high-income people in the waiting process was relatively high. Compared with psychological service recovery programs, our results show that tangible service recovery programs prompt respondents to wait for an additional ~10 min. This paper offers support for restaurant managers to manage consumer relationships.

1. Introduction

According to Executive Yuan’s office of the Taiwanese government, in 2016, almost 93% of Taiwanese people habitually ate out, of which 33.74% had dinner outside their homes more than four times a week, with the frequency of people dining in restaurants increasing significantly. In Taiwan, it is common for consumers to queue in restaurants waiting for meals, especially during holidays. Jones and Dent (1994) showed that waiting is a ubiquitous consumer experience, and that ~70% of consumers considered the duration of the waiting time. With technological development, some restaurants are using technology-assisted mobile apps to reduce waiting times; however, this does not apply to the majority of restaurants. For example, Taiwan’s well-known chain restaurant “Ding Tai Feng1” accounts for waiting time based on on-site number plates. During meal times, many consumers are always waiting for food at restaurant doors. Therefore, restaurant waiting is still important for the food culture phenomenon.
Usually, waiting periods are acceptable, with the length of the waiting period affecting customer satisfaction levels (Taylor 1994). For consumers, waiting is a negative experience that not only affects their intention to repurchase or revisit, but also increases their total outlay in terms of money, time, and effort (Taylor 1994). Waiting may also interrupt scheduled work. Generally, the uncertainty of the waiting time duration leads to negative associated emotions such as uneasiness, tension, and sometimes anger, thereby reducing consumer satisfaction and loyalty, leading to consumer loss (Taylor 1994; Seawright and Sampson 2007). Although waiting is expected for certain services (Bielen and Demoulin 2007), excessive waiting times affect customers’ evaluation of service quality and the company itself (Baker and Cameron 1996; Houston et al. 1998). Queuing has a negative effect on customers’ service quality perception and satisfaction (Chuo and Heywood 2014). Moreover, excessive waiting time is negatively correlated with overall satisfaction (Bailey and Areni 2006), which may arouse public complaints, thereby damaging a company’s reputation.
For restaurant operators, restaurants are only successful when consumers’ perceptions of their service quality are positive, and queuing is the beginning of service acceptance (Ford et al. 2012). When restaurant measures often entail extensive queuing times, consumers often find the waiting process time-consuming (Leclerc et al. 1995), which may affect their service experience, sojourn time, and ultimately spending, reneging, and return behaviors (De Vries et al. 2018). Zakay and Hornik (1991) found that proper environmental incentives can affect consumers’ waiting process evaluations. In fact, consumers may even expect to wait for some services. Therefore, consumers’ willingness to wait (WTW) depends on the quality of a store’s waiting experience. Not meeting customers’ needs causes consumer dissatisfaction and service failure2, which indirectly affect consumers’ satisfaction and loyalty (Wang et al. 2011). Thus, effectively controlling waiting times can offset their negative effects (Dellaert and Kahn 1999), which reveals that ameliorating waiting times can reduce consumers’ negative experiences regarding satisfaction and service quality (Dubé et al. 1991). However, even outstanding business strategies can result in service failure. Therefore, appropriate service recovery3 measures must be adapted to reduce consumer loss, such as offering apologies or utilizing promotional activities to reduce dissatisfaction and negative word-of-mouth communication4 (Keaveney 1995). Most studies on consumer waiting focus on their perceptions (Bielen and Demoulin 2007; Giebelhausen et al. 2011; Van Riel et al. 2012) to establish waiting schedules and apply the queuing theory as an operations management technique to explore waiting for processes, spatial configurations (Pillay et al. 2011; Zhao et al. 2014) and consumer behavioral responses.
In the present study, we explore whether implementing service recovery measures at restaurants for consumer waiting can extend consumers’ WTW. We used the contingent valuation method (CVM) to explore restaurant consumers’ WTW. Using the scenario assumption in the contingent valuation method (CVM), we relied on questionnaires to examine consumer waiting times and evaluate consumers’ WTW in various conditions of service recovery. Consumer willingness to pay (WTP) is used as an analysis tool to assess the time cost of consumers’ WTW in line, unlike previous studies, in which WTP research was commonly formulated on the amount consumers are willing to pay of the overall value for one year; this is difficult for the individual to do and can lead to erroneous responses caused (Arrow et al. 1993). Stejskal and Hájek (2015) highlighted that if the respondents are asked about the value of individual services without having to remember the frequency of their use for the entire year, then they give accurate results. In this study, we focus on consumers’ willingness to wait for the concept, which is given as a description of the evaluated good and the expected change of the restaurants’ service quality. Queue waiting times were used to measure the restaurants’ service quality acceptance, and the time cost for consumers waiting in queues was indirectly assessed. Our results serve as a reference for restaurant operators to improve practical management, with the objective of alleviating mistakes during service provision that can cause unintentional consumer loss. It must be said that principles of WTP and CVM are applicable in public and private sectors in many decision-making processes and show the cases where these principles have already been applied.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Waiting Time

Waiting is a time cost. This study uses the concept to measure not only the duration of consumers’ WTW, but also the value of WTW. In the waiting process, consumers are willing to give up other things and focus on waiting in line. However, the negative impact that consumer waiting time has on restaurants has been associated with many studies in the literature addressing this problem (Hernandez-Maskivker et al. 2019). In the service process, waiting is the activity before entering a service system, revealing that waiting is rarely avoidable (Bordoloi et al. 2019). Consumers now regard “time” as an essential factor of service satisfaction (Hirschman 1987) and have begun reducing their waiting times (Berry and Parasuraman 1991). Consequently, time has become a vital factor for consumers when choosing services or products and evaluating service quality (Hui et al. 2006).
De Vries et al. (2018) studied the waiting time in Indian restaurants, showing that waiting time related to rebellious behavior, and longer waiting times would in fact result in customer returns. Restaurant and theme park waiting times are similar. Theme parks are usually associated with waiting and queuing (Hernandez-Maskivker et al. 2019); consumers want to enjoy the service and are willing to wait. Theme park operators use a variety of strategies to reduce waiting time and enhance tourist experiences (Zhang et al. 2017). Taylor (1994) regards waiting time as the period between a consumer being ready to receive a service and the actual service provision. Waiting time is divided into pre-process, in-process, and post-process waiting. Pre-process waiting is the period before a consumer’s acceptance of a service. In-process waiting refers to the start of service acceptance and before it ends. Post-process waiting is the wait after a service ends. As an example, queuing at a restaurant is considered pre-process waiting, the waiting period after ordering is in-process waiting, and the waiting period after the meal before paying the bill is post-process waiting. De Vries et al. (2018) showed that consumers may be dissatisfied or never return due to excessive waiting times. Waiting times can appear relatively long to consumers who consider it imperative, thereby affecting their judgment (Sumaedi and Yarmen 2015). Using environmental and personal factors can divert consumers’ attention from waiting times (Zakay 1989). Mechanisms for controlling waiting times mainly comprise distracting consumers using recreational facilities (Baker and Cameron 1996) and developing waiting area strategies, such as providing menus in advance or offering complimentary beverages (Taylor 1995). Taylor (1994) shows that the measures for controlling customer waiting times can reduce their anger and uncertainty and improve their emotional responses to waiting. Lee and Lambert (2000) found in their research that customers feel that “expected reasonable waiting time” is longer than “perceived waiting time”, which affects their satisfaction. If customers wait longer than expected, their satisfaction will decline (Iqbal et al. 2013; De Vries et al. 2018). Customers do not want to waste time waiting and hope to use their time effectively (Lew and McKercher 2006).
In this study, we focus on waiting time during queuing, which is an assessment of pre-process waiting. Establishing measures for controlling consumer waiting times effectively alleviates their negative emotional responses toward waiting, even in problematic situations.

2.2. Service Recovery

Groth and Gilliland (2001) think that waiting time and the waiting process are important factors of service delivery. When a consumer’s service process experience does not meet their expectations, this implies service failure (Bell and Zemke 1987; Bitner et al. 1990). Rafaeli et al. (2002) indicate that service failure results from customers waiting in queues, as a result of intentions regarding the connection between the design of a queue and the emotions and attitudes of people waiting. Lahap et al. (2018) found that waiting times affect customer satisfaction and post-purchase behavior. Zeithaml et al. (1993) claim that service failure occurs when service behavior either does not satisfy consumer expectations or exceeds a tolerable range. If customers wait longer than expected, their satisfaction will decline (Lahap et al. 2016). Thus, service failure may trigger anger or dissatisfaction in consumers. If a service provider fails to take timely compensation measures or offer explanations, consumer dissatisfaction intensifies, inevitably affecting subsequent consumer behaviors.
When faced with service failure, service providers should take action to recover the service (Grönroos 1988), and change the negative perceptions of displeased consumers (Miller et al. 2000). Albrecht and Zemke (1985) report that if consumer complaints are properly handled, 95% of discontented customers can be retained. Otherwise, only 64% can be retained. In other words, the absence or improper implementation of service recovery increases consumer dissatisfaction (Hart et al. 1990), while successful service recovery improves consumer satisfaction (Bitner et al. 1994). Homburg et al. (2005) believe that satisfied customers who feel they have received a high-quality service will not only be replication customers but are also willing to pay more in the future, which has important implications for setting prices.
Smith et al. (1999) propose apology, assistance and compensation as service recovery measures. Miller et al. (2000) divide service recovery into psychological and tangible strategies: psychological recovery relates directly to consumers’ psychological demand (e.g., apologies or empathy), whereas tangible service recovery relates to tangible or value-added compensation. According to Wirtz and Mattila (2004), tangible service recovery improves consumer satisfaction more effectively compared to psychological recovery. In general, service recovery consists of complementary products, such as free food, discounts, coupons, interventions from upper management, and replacements (Hwang and Lambert 2008; Colgate and Norris 2001). The fact is that customers still wait during peak meal times, thus emphasizing that waiting in restaurants is a common occurrence in department stores. It is therefore important to understand customer behavior to manage queues and waiting times (Bennett 1998) to ultimately improve service quality. Therefore, managing waiting times is a priority for restaurant management.
In summary, this study focuses on the pre-process recovery of restaurants. Based on the definition of service recovery in the literature and the mechanism for controlling waiting times, we designed various service recovery programs for different situations to assess consumers’ WTW at restaurants. Examples of service recovery include the integration of psychological and tangible strategies, with apologies being mandatory for psychological service recovery, and dining discounts or free food being tangible service recovery measures.

2.3. Willingness to Pay (WTP)

WTP plays a key role when customers choose a product from different alternatives. Gupta and Çakanyıldırım (2016) pointed that WTP is the maximum amount of money that a customer would be willing to pay in order to receive a product or service. Most earlier WTP studies with the application of CVM have focused mainly on non-market goods and examined the environmental resources and conservation regarding preferences and perceptions. Naidoo and Adamowicz (2005) estimate the demand for improved biodiversity among foreign visitors in a Ugandan forest park to maximize park revenue. Uyarra et al. (2005) found that tourists were willing to pay an environmental (conservation) levy for improved sewage treatment that would improve water quality in the Folkestone Marine Reserve. Jin et al. (2006) examined residents’ preferences for alternative solid waste management policy changes in Macau. Mmopelwa et al. (2007) estimate overseas and South African visitors’ WTP to visit Moremi Game Reserve in Botswana under a hypothetical park management scenario of high-cost low-volume tourism. Thur (2010) estimates that the WTP is significantly more than the existing US$10 annual user fee for access to a marine park.
In recent years, many studies have increasingly focused on customers’ willingness to pay for service quality (Hernandez-Maskivker et al. 2019; Kuo and Jou 2018; Merkert and Beck 2017; Sukhu et al. 2017). A study by Hernandez-Maskivker et al. (2019) confirmed that theme park visitors are willing to pay a high price for fast access. Kuo and Jou (2018) found that people’s willingness to pay over a long distance is higher than for a short distance when choosing counter check-in services. Merkert and Beck (2017) studied travel time savings and willingness to pay for regional airfares. Their results showed that regional aviation services were of high value for people in regional, rural, and remote areas. Sukhu et al. (2017) studied a restaurant’s service pricing strategy, showing that the customer’s WTP has a significant positive relationship with food quality, the ambiance of the restaurant and the service encounter value.

3. Methods

3.1. CVM

In this study, we used the CVM to assess how restaurant service recovery affects waiting times. Using questionnaire-based surveys, we explored a hypothetical question concerning variations in WTP according to the number of products and their quality. The CVM, which has been employed to investigate natural resources and environment-related cases without market values, can be used to evaluate unrealized market benefits, which is suitable for this study examining consumer WTW.
CVM is a hypothetical market that can be used to discuss individual expressions for a given activity (such as restaurant waits in this study) or product (the study’s improved waiting quality) to give prices. CVM has been extensively used to estimate the value of non-marketed goods and services through interviews using questionnaire surveys. The CVM uses SP preferences, whereby the respondent evaluates the public services in a hypothetical market through their WTW for service. The method elicits respondents’ preference for non-marketed goods and services by asking them how much they would be willing to pay to acquire improvements or to avoid negative aspects in them.
Following Cameron and James (1987), the standard model of assessing consumer WTW for service recovery provided by restaurants is as follows:
W T W i = f ( x i ) + ε i = x i β + ε i
where W T W i denotes consumer WTW, ε i represents the residual term and conforms with the hypothesis of N (0, σ 2 ) and x i defines the explanatory variable vector of the ith respondent. β is a vector of estimated coefficients.
A close-ended bidding version of CVM has been widely applied to evaluate various environmental and public goods. Estimation methods can be divided into single and double-bounded dichotomous choice models. Although dichotomous and double-bounded dichotomous choice questions have been used less frequently in novel food product valuation, these contingent valuation techniques are easily extendable to this application (Wertenbroch and Skiera 2002). The double-bounded dichotomous choice model developed by Hanemann et al. (1991) is used to estimate changes in people’s WTW after implementing restaurant service recovery. The model is not only effective at estimating but also at reducing respondents’ stress and avoids problems such as starting-point bias and range deviation (Hoehn and Randall 1987; Duffield and Patterson 1991).
The double-bounded dichotomous choice model is based on the threshold value of the first bid (T). When people agree to wait at the first inquiry, they are surveyed for the second question, where they are requested to double their waiting time (2T). If participants are unwilling to wait at the first inquiry, the second inquiry provides a waiting time half that of the original threshold time. Therefore, four combinations of inquiry results are obtained, namely responses of YES–YES (positive reply to both questions), NO–YES (negative reply to the first question but a positive reply to the second), YES–NO (positive reply to the first question but a negative reply to the second), and NO–NO (negative reply to both questions).
Let y i represent the respondent’s vote: 1 if “YES” and 0 if “NO”. Assume ε i is independent and normally distributed with a mean 0 and standard deviation σ, and Bidi is the randomly assigned bid amount for each respondent i. The probability of a “YES” vote given xi and ε i is equal to the probability that the individual’s WTW is greater than the bid amount, as follows:
Pr ( y i = 1 | x i ) = P r [ W T W > B i d i ] = P r [ x i β + ε i > B i d i ] = P r [ ε i > B i d i x i β ] = Pr [ Z i > ( B i d i x i β ) / σ ]
where Z i is the standard normal random variable and σ is a variance parameter.

3.2. Questionnaire Design

The questionnaire primarily focuses on general situations that people may encounter when queuing at a restaurant, namely waiting experience, waiting pattern and time, and satisfaction with the waiting service. Using situation design, this study constructs physical and psychological behaviors and emotional reactions that often occur when the waiting time exceeds consumers’ expectations while queuing. Such behaviors and reactions might inadvertently create service failure at restaurants. Therefore, the questionnaire item regarding WTW when restaurant operators provide service recovery was divided into two hypothetical situations: (1) restaurant staff sincerely apologize without providing tangible service recovery and (2) in addition to apologizing, restaurant staff provide tangible service recovery such as restaurant discounts or free food.
The waiting time in this study is set according to the double-bounded dichotomous choice model. Random interview and open inquiry methods are adopted in a trial interviews to enable participants to express their WTW in hypothetical situations. The pre-test was conducted in June 2015 to ensure the validity and reliability of the questionnaire. A total of 62 questionnaires were distributed, and 50 valid questionnaires were obtained, as shown in Table 1. Based on the feedback from the pilot test, the content of the questionnaire has been modified. In accordance with Alberini (1995), the respondents’ WTW values were arranged in ascending order, where the highest and lowest 10% of the bid values were deleted to reduce observational error. Selected from the remaining bid values, the 20th, 40th, 60th and 80th percentiles served as the basis for the first inquiry about the WTW. In Table 1, the WTW for an apology (without tangible service recovery) was 5 min, 10 min, 15 min and 20 min, whereas the WTW for relevant tangible service recovery was 5 min, 10 min, 20 min and 30 min.
The last section of the questionnaire collected participants’ demographic information (e.g., sex, age, marital status, education level, occupation and mean monthly income) to explore the essential factors of the model.

4. Results

4.1. Descriptive Statistics

The formal questionnaire survey was conducted from 1 October 2015 to 30 November 2015, and is based on Tainan City5 Department Store restaurants, including chain or non-chain restaurants. In general, department store restaurants often have on-site queuing, especially during peak hours. Therefore, waiting time length indirectly affected consumers’ impression of restaurant service quality, and consumers of different types also had different cognitive responses to restaurant waiting queues. The target sample for this study was focused on people who had prior experience of being patronized in department store restaurants. Overall, 596 questionnaires were distributed, 13 of which were invalid. Moreover, 43 samples were deducted because they were protest observations. Therefore, 540 questionnaires were validly obtained. In terms of the demand to return, 583 questionnaires were collected.
Respondent demographics are shown in Table 2. The majority of participants were female (54.7%), with the largest proportion aged 31–40 years (31.4%), followed by 21–30 years (29.2%), and 41–50 years (22.8%). Overall, 49.9% of the respondents were married (47.9% were unmarried). Regarding education level, the majority (41.3%) of participants had a university degree, 29.0% had a senior high school degree, and 16.5% had a junior college degree. Moreover, 31.9% of the participants worked in the service sector, and 22.6% of the participants worked in the industrial or business sector. Most respondents had a monthly income of NT$20,001–40,000 (44.6%), followed by NT$40,001–60,000 (25.4%).
The restaurant waiting service satisfaction analysis results are shown in Table 3. Overall, the respondents’ satisfaction scores for each item of the restaurant waiting service were similar; the item “I clearly understand the language of the service personnel” has a slightly higher mean (3.76) compared to the other items.

4.2. Setting of the Model for Restaurant Waiting Service Recovery

A hypothetical questionnaire was employed in this study. Using the CVM situational design, this study utilized the double-bounded dichotomous choice model to interview respondents queuing at restaurants before the dining service who received psychological service recovery (without tangible service recovery) or tangible service recovery to develop an empirical model for assessing individual WTW values. During the interview, establishing hypothetical situations explained the common phenomenon during the wait before dining, which inadvertently and gradually led to service failure. The model also further accounted for the timely service recovery measures provided by the restaurant to alleviate consumers’ negative impressions developed while waiting.
This section discusses the results of the respondents’ WTW in response to the service recovery provided by the restaurant. The model considers WTW factors that potentially affect the provision of psychological service recovery and tangible service recovery. For example, demographic variables, such as respondents’ income and age can influence their feelings about waiting. Furthermore, experiences and attitudes associated with waiting for restaurants may also affect WTW cognitive behavior through service recovery measures. Therefore, this study constructs a model that considers WTW associated with service recovery measures (psychological and tangible) provided by the restaurant, as shown below:
ln W T W 1 = f ( A g e ,   I n c o m e , D i n n e r ,   W a i t i n g   w a y ,   Q u e u e d   s t a t u s ,   S a t i s f a c t i o n ,   F a c i l i t y , C o m p l a i n e d   e x p e r i e n c e ) ln W T W 2 = f ( A g e ,   I n c o m e , D i n n e r ,   W a i t i n g   w a y ,   Q u e u e d   s t a t u s ,   S a t i s f a c t i o n ,   F a c i l i t y , C o m p l a i n e d   e x p e r i e n c e )
where lnWTW1 and lnWTW2 represent the WTW for restaurants with or without tangible service recovery. The model’s variable descriptions are shown in Table 4 and are as follows: (1) the demographic variable age denotes the mean age of the respondents, and income denotes the mean monthly income of the respondents; (2) experiences and attitudes associated with queuing for restaurants: dinner is a dummy variable, whereby 1 = dining with more than five people and 0 = other; waiting pattern is a dummy variable, whereby 1 = on-site queuing and 0 = other; the waiting experience is a dummy variable, whereby 1 = negative experience associated with queuing and 0 = other; satisfaction refers to the satisfaction of the respondents toward service recovery; facility is a dummy variable, whereby 1 = restaurant facilities need improvement and 0 = other; and complaint filing experience is a dummy variable, whereby 1 = respondents have had experience filing a customer complaint and 0 = other. The evaluation model was established based on the mentioned variable data, and maximum likelihood estimation was used to determine Weibull, lognormal, and gamma distribution functions regarding the empirical analysis of consumer WTW associated with waiting service recovery.
According to the mentioned model settings and variable descriptions, the results of the evaluation model are shown in Table 5 and Table 6. In the next section, we discuss the results of WTW associated with an apology as a psychological service recovery measure and tangible service recovery measure.

4.2.1. Estimated Results of an Apology Given by Restaurant Managers as a Psychological Service Recovery Measure

According to the log likelihood ratio of Weibull, gamma, and lognormal models and the t values of the scales, the explanatory powers of the model pass a goodness of fit test. Regarding the demographic variables, the coefficient values of age are negative and significant in all three models at the 5% significance level, revealing that older respondents were relatively unwilling to wait even after the restaurant staff had apologized. Moreover, the coefficient values of income were negative in all three models but not significant, revealing that people with a high income are often unwilling to wait. For restaurant waiting experience and cognition, the coefficient values of waiting experience were positive and significant in all three models, showing that if restaurant operators offer a timely apology to their customers who experience service failure during queuing, the customers were relatively willing to continue waiting. The coefficient values of satisfaction were positive and significant in all three models at the 1% significance level; even when the service was inadequate, the respondents who felt relatively satisfied when waiting were willing to continue waiting because of the apology given by the restaurant. The coefficient values of facility were positive and significant in all three models at the 5% significance level. This suggests that consumers who value the facilities provided to control the waiting time exhibit a relatively high WTW after restaurant operators sincerely apologize. Finally, the coefficient values of complaint filing experience were negative and significant in all three models at the 10% significance level, revealing that the respondents who had experience filing a customer complaint were relatively unwilling to wait even after receiving an apology from the restaurant operators.

4.2.2. Estimated Result of the Tangible Recovery Service Provided by Restaurant Managers

According to the log likelihood ratio of Weibull, gamma, and lognormal models and the t values of the scales, the explanatory powers of the model pass a goodness of fit test. In terms of the demographic variables, the coefficient values of age were negative and significant in all three models and at the 1% significance level, indicating that older people did not tend to queue for restaurants even when restaurant operators provided dining discounts. The coefficient values of income were negative and non-significant in all three models, indicating that those with a high income did not tend to spend time waiting in a queue at the restaurant. Regarding variables associated with restaurant waiting experience and cognition, the coefficient values of dinner were negative and significant for all three models at the 10% significance level. Therefore, having exceeded the mean customer number for a table, those who dined with more than five people were relatively willing to accept the offered tangible service recovery and continue waiting in a queue at the restaurant. The coefficient values of waiting patterns were positive and significant for all three models at the 5% significance level, indicating that consumers queuing on the spot were usually unable to determine their waiting time. If restaurant operators provided tangible service recovery in a timely manner, consumers were relatively willing to extend their waiting time. Similarly, the coefficient values of waiting experience and satisfaction for waiting were positive and significant in the three models at the 5% significance level. This indicates that people who perceive service failure as serious are willing to extend their waiting time in response to the tangible service recovery provided by restaurants, whereas those who are relatively satisfied with restaurant waiting services are likely to spend more time waiting. The coefficient value of facility was positive and significant in the Weibull model at the 10% significance level, implying that those who considered that the facilities of a specific restaurant required improvement were willing to continue waiting after receiving tangible service recovery. Finally, the coefficient value of complaint filing experience was negative and significant in the Weibull model at the 5% significance level, indicating that people who filed consumer complaints were relatively unwilling to extend their waiting time.
Regarding the demographic variables, younger respondents were relatively willing to extend their waiting time when provided with psychological or tangible service recovery. However, respondents with high income were relatively unwilling to extend their waiting time regardless of either service recovery. For the variables concerning restaurant waiting experience and cognition, the respondents who dined with more than five people and those who queued on the spot were relatively responsive to tangible service recovery and were willing to extend their waiting time, whereas those who regarded service failure as serious and those who queued on the spot were willing to accept service recovery in any form and extend their waiting time.
After estimating the WTW evaluation model for the relevant waiting service recovery, this study further estimates consumers’ WTW associated with an apology (psychological service recovery) and dining discount or free food (tangible service recovery) based on the obtained results. Conventional assessment of bid values is often based on median values for calculation, which is relatively less susceptible to extreme values. The use of mean values to determine effective values has a relatively high bias error rate (Cooper et al. 2002). Therefore, in this study, we employed the coefficient estimation results of both service recovery measures and the WTW evaluation model to estimate the WTW for psychological and tangible service recovery, as follows:
log ( WTW ) = X i γ + δ ε x
where γ denotes the parameter for estimation, δ is the scale of a location parameter and WTW represents the mean of the respondents’ average WTW. Supposing that the restaurant operator provides timely service recovery measures to compensate for a service failure that occurs during the waiting process, the estimated results of the evaluation models based on an apology (without tangible service recovery) and tangible service recovery can be jointly employed with the mentioned WTW estimation equation to estimate the WTW and confidence interval of the respondents for the various types of waiting service recovery at restaurants.

4.2.3. Evaluation of WTW Associated with Service Recovery Measures Provided by the Restaurant

Estimated through Weibull distribution, the WTW values for the 540 participants associated with waiting service recovery measures (e.g., sincere apologies and dining discounts) of the restaurant are compiled in Table 7. When the respondents waited for longer than they expected, the mean additional waiting time was approximately 21 min after accepting a sincere apology from the restaurant operator—under the 95% confidence level, the confidence interval is 18–25 min. Tangible service recovery measures of dining discounts or free food prompted each of the respondents to wait for an additional 31 min on average—under the 95% confidence level, the confidence interval is 27–38 min. The results reveal that respondents had a relatively high waiting tolerance for tangible service recovery measures. Thus, direct tangible service recovery in addition to psychological service recovery provides relatively strong incentives for consumers to continue waiting.
From the empirical results, it can be observed that psychological recovery measures are an important service for consumer waiting, especially for consumers who have had poor experience in service quality and who are relatively concerned about the quality of waiting. The quality of consumers’ waiting time may not only affect the mood of the meal but may even affect the willingness of the next purchase.
In addition, the restaurants can provide consumers with tangible service recovery, and this is likely to make consumers willing to spend time waiting. Especially in Eastern food culture, “queuing” is a unique culture and phenomenon, and consumers are willing to wait in line. However, if you exceed the time you are willing to wait, this will inevitably affect the willingness to continue waiting. Therefore, psychological service recovery can effectively delay the waiting time and ease the waiting mood; however, tangible service recovery can further enable consumers to obtain actual satisfaction, reverse the lack of service errors and extend the waiting time.

5. Conclusions

Taylor (1994) and Bordoloi et al. (2019) claim that waiting is an inevitable condition for consumers and a preceding operation before entering a service system. In reality, an individual’s perception toward services is part of the waiting process. The propositions obtained here are by no means meant to be an extensive list of all the psychological considerations involved in managing customers’ acceptance of waiting times.
Therefore, waiting time is often closely associated with perceptions of service. For service-oriented restaurant managers, queuing is no longer a marketing strategy to attract a crowd but the beginning of consumer loss. Sasser et al. (1979) observed that “the feeling that somebody has successfully ‘cut in front’ of you causes even the most patient customer to become furious; one of the most frequent irritants mentioned by customers at restaurants is prior seating”. Our study explores pre-process service importance in the context of possible service crises in restaurants and constructs the restaurant service recovery model for the WTW through the CVM to measure the effects before and after implementing service recovery.
Service businesses would benefit from future research examining the influence of distributive and interactional justice dimensions on the service delivery process (e.g., cost and level of service). The results serve as a crucial reference for restaurant managers to manage consumer relationships.

5.1. Factors of WTW Associated with the Service Recovery Provided by Restaurants

People’s WTW varies with age. Relatively young respondents are willing to extend their waiting time when provided with service recovery. Generally, young people in groups are relatively willing to queue for restaurants. Some studies also show that young people had more tolerance for waiting times than elderly people at restaurants (Hwang and Lambert 2008) and at theme parks (Hernandez-Maskivker et al. 2019). Conversely, relatively old people are likely to make reservations at restaurants (Hwang and Lambert 2008) to avoid waiting and therefore do not tend to wait.
The coefficient value of income is negative and not significant in the evaluation model of the WTW, revealing that income does not have an imperative explanatory power under the hypothesis of this model. Researchers apply the CVM to the WTP value of a certain target, whereby income is an essential indicator, showing that people with high income have a relatively high WTP ability. However, the value of income is negative and displays no explanatory power in the model. The key reason for this might be the nature of different situations. First, people with a high income have a relatively high opportunity cost in the waiting process; therefore, they are reluctant to wait.
The external behavioral conditions and responses that affect waiting factors (Bielen and Demoulin 2007) cause the variable of income to be less essential as an indicator, which indirectly affects the estimated result of the WTW.

5.2. Results of the WTW Associated with the Waiting Service Recovery of Restaurants

When provided with psychological service recovery, each respondent is willing to wait an additional 20 min per occasion. Tangible service recovery prompts each person to wait an extra 30 min per occasion. The results indicate that tangible service recovery is a vital incentive that compensates for consumer loss, which indirectly indicates that successful service recovery improves consumer satisfaction (Bitner et al. 1994).
To summarize, the empirical results of this study show that restaurant queues are inevitable. That perceived value affects tolerance or waits can be demonstrated by our common experience in restaurants—we will accept a much longer waiting time in restaurants with good service. Giebelhausen et al. (2011) suggest that management practitioners propose a better strategy involving “optimized” waiting to reduce negative service quality. If waiting times exceed customer expectations, negative emotional responses will occur. Such a condition also applies to well-known gourmet restaurants that attract crowds and draw queues for entry. If restaurants do not handle waiting situations properly, service failure may occur, which reduces the perceived attractiveness of such restaurants. Such a condition not only influences consumers’ WTW on the spot but also affects their subsequent willingness to revisit. Consequently, restaurant managers are bound to lose consumers. Restaurant managers are advised to consider the importance of waiting service quality, such as establishing compensation mechanisms for waiting. As a primary basis of service, restaurant staff can express sincere apologies for long waiting times. They can also offer tangible service recovery accordingly to alleviate negative responses from consumers due to the long wait. The objective should be to make consumers feel valued, which restaurant managers can achieve by expressing sincerity for the inconvenience caused due to long waiting times, thereby facilitating their consumers’ willingness to extend their waiting time.
The results of this study have a common point. Regardless of the type of consumer, compensation and services (both psychological and tangible) are required in the queuing process. Therefore, restaurant operators should be aware that improving the restaurant’s hardware and software facilities indirectly affects consumers’ perception of waiting time; for example, adding dining seats and setting up waiting devices. However, the upgrade of hardware and software facilities requires additional cost investment. Further improvement in the quality of service personnel is an important investment in long-term restaurant management. As our research emphasizes, the service before restaurant entry is part of the entire service. Finally, through this study, we learn that different types of consumers have different perceptions of restaurant waiting. In order to accommodate crowds and provide high-quality services and experiences, restaurant operators need to assess service quality and establish management standards for consumer waiting. Restaurant operators can further understand the needs of different customers and make up for the psychological anxiety and emotional dissatisfaction that consumers may experience during the waiting process.

5.3. Management Implications and Suggestion

(1)
The establishment of a waiting service mechanism: In the past, restaurant service was mostly studied during the meal or after the meal. This study focused on the waiting service before the meal. Sincerely expressed apologies are a necessary basis for service. If there is a major waiting situation, a restaurant can provide appropriate substantial compensation to improve the service quality of frontline service personnel and reduce the negative impact of service failure on restaurants.
(2)
Establish the importance of service recovery: The results showed that customers are willing to wait at least 20 min or more with compensation. Therefore, if the restaurant industry can determine customers’ inner tolerance, this can help in easing the customer’s emotions and frustration with waiting in a timely manner.
(3)
Restaurant equipment updates: With the advancement of technology, it is important for restaurants to invest in technology, letting customers know their waiting time by developing query technology software, or letting them determine this with an electronic queue waiting system. Understanding the relevant waiting processes and information can effectively improve the waiting quality of the restaurant, especially for chain restaurants with relatively rich resources.
(4)
This study takes general restaurants as a research target and focuses on the service quality for waiting. It does not deeply distinguish between the types of restaurant operation and the type of the restaurant. In view of the operation cost of the restaurant, if future proposals can first analyze a restaurant in the chain system, this will improve the overall service quality when resources and equipment are relatively abundant. In addition, in terms of sample size, if it is possible to use the entire Taiwanese restaurants as the scope of investigation, this will further enhance the value of restaurant waiting in service quality. Finally, operators should give the customer a reasonable and satisfactory impression of service recovery and authorize employees to deal with service failures for the customer.
(5)
In the past, the study into public goods was mainly focused on WTP of preservation sites (cultural heritage, monuments, etc.), but few studies estimate the WTW of public goods on the queue. This paper studies the WTW of private sectors. Under this concept, we suggest that the WTW of public goods can be evaluated in the future.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.-T.T.; Formal analysis, Y.-S.H.; Investigation, S.-C.P.; Methodology, C.-T.T. and C.-T.L.; Project administration, C.-T.L.; Resources, S.-C.P.; Software, Y.-S.H.; Supervision, C.-T.L.; Validation, S.-C.P.; Writing—original draft, Y.-S.H.; Writing—review & editing, C.-T.T.

Funding

This research was funded by Ministry of Science and Technology, Taiwan (ROC): MOST 103-2410-H-309-012.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alberini, Anna. 1995. Optimal designs for discrete choice contingent valuation surveys: Single-bound, double-bound, and bivariate models. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 28: 287–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Albrecht, Karl, and Ron Zemke. 1985. The Service People in Service America-Doing Business in the New Economy. Homewood: Dow Jones-IRWIN. [Google Scholar]
  3. Arrow, Kenneth, Robert Solow, Paul R. Portney, Edward E. Leamer, Roy Radner, and Howard Schuman. 1993. Report of the NOAA panel on contingent valuation. Federal Register 58: 4601–14. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bailey, Nicole, and Charles S. Areni. 2006. When a few minutes sound like a lifetime: Does atmospheric music expand or contract perceived time? Journal of Retailing 82: 189–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Baker, Julie, and Michaelle Cameron. 1996. The effects of the service environment on affect and consumer perception of waiting time: An integrative review and research propositions. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 24: 338–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bell, Chip R., and Ron E. Zemke. 1987. Service breakdown: The road to recovery. Management Review 76: 32–35. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bennett, Roger. 1998. Queues, customer characteristics and policies for managing waiting-lines in supermarkets. International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management 26: 78–87. [Google Scholar]
  8. Berry, Leonard L., and A. Parsu Parasuraman. 1991. Marketing Services: Competing Through Quality. New York: The Free Press. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bielen, Frédéric, and Nathalie Demoulin. 2007. Waiting time influence on the satisfaction-loyalty relationship in services. Managing Service Quality 17: 174–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bitner, Mary Jo, Bernard H. Booms, and Mary Stanfield Tetreault. 1990. The service encounter: Diagnosing favorable and unfavorable incidents. Journal of Marketing 54: 71–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bitner, Mary Jo, Bernard H. Booms, and Lois A. Mohr. 1994. Critical service encounters: The employee’s viewpoint. Journal of Marketing 58: 95–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Bordoloi, Sanjeev, James A. Fitzsimmons, and Mona J. Fitzsimmons. 2019. Service Management: Operations, Strategy, and Information Technology, 9th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cameron, Trudy Ann, and Michelle D. James. 1987. Efficient estimation for “Closed-Ended” contingent valuation surveys. The Review of Economics and Statistics 69: 269–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Chuo, Hsin-You, and John L. Heywood. 2014. An optimal queuing wait for visitor’s most favorite ride at theme parks. Advances in Hospitality and Leisure 10: 57–73. [Google Scholar]
  15. Colgate, Mark, and Melissa Norris. 2001. Developing a comprehensive picture of service failure. International Journal of Service Industry Management 12: 215–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Cooper, Joseph C., Michael Hanemann, and Giovanni Signorello. 2002. One-and-one-half-bound dichotomous choice contingent valuation. The Review of Economics and Statistics 84: 742–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. De Vries, Jelle, Debjit Roy, and René De Koster. 2018. Worth the wait? How restaurant waiting time influences customer behavior and revenue. Journal of Operations Management 63: 59–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Dellaert, Benedict G. C., and Barbara E. Kahn. 1999. How tolerable is delay? Consumers’ evaluations of internet web sites after waiting. Journal of Interactive Marketing 13: 41–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Dubé, Laurette, Bernd H. Schmitt, and France Leclerc. 1991. Consumers’ affective response to delays at different phases of a service delivery. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 21: 810–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Duffield, John W., and David A. Patterson. 1991. Inference and optimal design for a welfare measure in dichotomous choice contingent valuation. Land Economics 67: 225–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Ford, Robert C., Michael C. Sturman, and Cherrill P. Heaton. 2012. Managing Quality Service in Hospitality: How Organizations Achieve Excellence in the Guest Experience. Clifton Park: Demar Cengage Learing. [Google Scholar]
  22. Giebelhausen, Michael D., Stacey G. Robinson, and J. Joseph Cronin Jr. 2011. Worth waiting for: Increasing satisfaction by making consumers wait. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 39: 889–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Grönroos, Christian. 1988. Service quality: The six criteria of good perceived service. Review of Business 9: 10–14. [Google Scholar]
  24. Groth, Markus, and Stephen W. Gilliland. 2001. The role of procedural justice in the delivery of services: A study of customers’ reactions to waiting. Journal of Quality Management 6: 77–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Gupta, Varun, and Metin Çakanyıldırım. 2016. A WTP-Choice Model: Empirical Validation, Competitive and Centralized Pricing. Production and Operations Management 25: 1866–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hanemann, Michael, John Loomis, and Barbara Kanninen. 1991. Statistical efficiency of double- bounded dichotomous choice contingent valuation. American Journal of Agriculture Economics 73: 1225–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hart, Christopher W., James L. Heskett, and W. Earl Sasser Jr. 1990. The profitable art of service recovery. Harvard Business Review 68: 148–56. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  28. Hawkins, Del I., Roger J. Best, and Kenneth A. Coney. 2001. Consumer Behaviour: Building Marketing Strategy, 8th ed. New York: The McGraw-Hill Companies Inc. [Google Scholar]
  29. Hernandez-Maskivker, Gilda-María, Juan L. Nicolau, Gerard Ryan, and Mireia Valverde. 2019. A reference-dependent approach to WTP for priority. Tourism Management 71: 165–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hirschman, Elizabeth. C. 1987. People as Products: Analysis of a Complex Marketing Exchange. Journal of Marketing 51: 98–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hoehn, John P., and Alan Randall. 1987. A satisfactory benefit cost indicator from contingent valuation. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 14: 226–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Homburg, Christian, Nicole Koschate, and Wayne D. Hoyer. 2005. Do satisfied customers really pay more? A study of the relationship between customer satisfaction and willingness to pay. Journal of Marketing 69: 84–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Houston, Mark B., Lance A. Bettencourt, and Sutha Wenger. 1998. The relationship between waiting in a service queue and evaluations of service quality: A field theory perspective. Psychology & Marketing 15: 735–53. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hui, Michael K., Alan C. Tse, and Lianxi Zhou. 2006. Interaction between two types of information on reactions to delays. Market Letter 17: 151–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hwang, Johye, and Carolyn U. Lambert. 2008. The interaction of major resources and their influence on waiting times in a multi-stage restaurant. International Journal of Hospitality Management 27: 541–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Iqbal, Qamar, Lawrence E. Whitman, and Don Malzahn. 2013. Reducing customer wait time at a fast food restaurant on campus. Journal of Foodservice Business Research 15: 319–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jin, Jianjun, Zhishi Wang, and Shenghong Ran. 2006. Comparison of contingent valuation and choice experiment in solid waste management programs in Macao. Ecological Economics 57: 430–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Jones, Peter, and Michael Dent. 1994. Improving service: Managing response time in hospitality operations. International Journal of Operations & Production Management 14: 52–58. [Google Scholar]
  39. Keaveney, Susan M. 1995. Customer switching behavior in service in-dustries: An exploratory study. Journal of Marketing 56: 43–55. [Google Scholar]
  40. Kuo, Chung-Wei, and Rong-Chang Jou. 2018. Air passengers’ willingness to pay for counter check-in services. Transportation Research Part A 107: 203–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Lahap, Johanudin, Nur Safiah Ramlib, Noraslinda Mohd Saidc, Salleh Mohd Radzid, and Razlan Adli Zain. 2016. A study of brand image towards customer satisfaction. Journal of Procedia Social and Behavioral Science 224: 149–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lahap, Johanudin, Rabiatul Iylia Azlan, Khairil Anuar Bahri, Noraslinda Mohd Said, Dahlan Abdullah, and Razlan Adli Zain. 2018. The effect of perceived waiting time on customer’s satisfaction: A focus on fast food restaurant. International Journal of Supply Chain Management 7: 259–66. [Google Scholar]
  43. Leclerc, France, Bernd H. Schmitt, and Laurette Dubé. 1995. Waiting time and decision making: Is time like money. Journal of Consumer Research 22: 110–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Lee, Wonjae, and Carolyn U. Lambert. 2000. Impact of Waiting Time on Evaluation of Service. Journal of Food Service 12: 241–54. [Google Scholar]
  45. Lew, Alan, and Bob McKercher. 2006. Modeling tourist movements: A local destination analysis. Annals of Tourism Research 33: 403–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Maxham, James G., III, and Richard G. Netemeyer. 2002. A longitudinal study of complaining customers’ evaluations of multiple service evaluations of multiple service failures and recovery efforts. Journal of Marketing 66: 57–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Merkert, Rico, and Matthew Beck. 2017. Value of travel time savings and willingness to pay for regional aviation. Transportation Research Part A 96: 29–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Miller, Janis L., Christopher W. Craighead, and Kirk R. Karwan. 2000. Service recovery: A framework and empirical investigation. Journal of Operations Management 18: 387–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Mmopelwa, Gagoitseope, Donald L. Kgathi, and L. Molefhe. 2007. Tourists’ perceptions and their willingness to pay for park fees: A case study of self-drive tourists and clients for mobile tour operators in Moremi Game Reserve, Botswana. Tourism Management 28: 1044–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Naidoo, Robin, and Wiktor Adamowicz. 2005. Biodiversity and nature-based tourism at forest reserves in Uganda. Environment and Development Economics 10: 159–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pillay, Datuk Ir. M. S., Roslan Johari Dato Mohd Ghazali, Noor Hazilah Abd Manaf, Abu Hassan Abdullah, Azman Abu Bakar, Faisal Salikin, Mathyvani Umapathy, Roslinah Ali, Noriah Bidin, and Wan Ismefariana Wan Ismail. 2011. Hospital waiting time: The forgotten premise of healthcare service delivery? International Journal Health Care Quality Assurance 24: 506–22. [Google Scholar]
  52. Rafaeli, Anat, Greg Barron, and Keren Haber. 2002. The effects of queue structure on attitudes. Journal of Service Research 5: 125–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Sasser, W. Earl, Richard Paul Olsen, and D. Daryl Wyckoff. 1979. Management of Service Operations: Text, Cases and Readings. New York: Allyn and Bacon. [Google Scholar]
  54. Seawright, Kristie K., and Scott E. Sampson. 2007. A video method for empirically wait-perception bias. Journal of Operations Management 25: 1055–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Smith, Amy K., Ruth N. Bolton, and Janet Wagner. 1999. A model of customer satisfaction with service encounters involving failure and recovery. Journal of Marketing Research 36: 356–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Stejskal, Jan, and Petr Hájek. 2015. Evaluating the economic value of a public service—The case of the municipal library of Prague. Public Money & Management 35: 145–52. [Google Scholar]
  57. Sukhu, Anupama, Anil Bilgihan, and Soobin Seo. 2017. Willingness to pay in negative restaurant service encounters. International Journal of Hospitality Management 65: 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Sumaedi, Sik, and Medi Yarmen. 2015. Measuring Perceived Service Quality of Fast Food Restaurant in Islamic Country: A Conceptual Framework. Procedia Food Science Elsevier 3: 119–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Taylor, Shirley. 1994. Waiting for service: The relationship between delays and evaluations of service. Journal of Marketing 58: 56–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Taylor, Shirley. 1995. The effects of filled waiting time and service provider control over the delay on evaluations of service. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 23: 38–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Thur, Steven M. 2010. User fees as sustainable financing mechanisms for marine protected areas: An application to the Bonaire national marine park. Marine Policy 34: 63–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Uyarra, Maria C., Isabelle M. Cote, Jennifer A. Gill, Rob RT Tinch, David Viner, and Andrew R. Watkinson. 2005. Island-specific preferences of tourists for environmental features: Implications of climate change for tourism-dependent states. Environmental Conservation 32: 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Van Riel, Allard C. R., Janjaap Semeijn, Dina Ribbink, and Yvette Bomert-Peters. 2012. Waiting for service at the checkout: Negative emotional responses, store image and overall satisfaction. Journal of Service Management 23: 144–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Wang, Yi-Shun, Shun-Cheng Wu, Hsin-Hui Lin, and Yu-Yin Wang. 2011. The relationship of service failure severity, service recovery justice and perceived switching costs with customer loyalty in the context of e-tailing. International Journal of Information Management 31: 350–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Wertenbroch, Klaus, and Bernd Skiera. 2002. Measuring consumers’ willingness to pay at the point of purchase. Journal of Marketing Research 39: 228–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Wirtz, Jochen, and Anna S. Mattila. 2004. Consumer responses to compensation, speed of recovery and apology after a service failure. International Journal of Service Industry Management 15: 150–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Zakay, Dan. 1989. An Integrated Model of Time Estimation, Time and Human Cognition: A Life-Span Perspective. Amsterdam: North Holland. [Google Scholar]
  68. Zakay, Dan, and Jacob Hornik. 1991. How much time did you wait in line? A time perception perspective. In Time and Consumer Behavior. Edited by Jean-Charles Chebat and V. Venkatesan. Montreal: Universite du Quebec a Montreal. [Google Scholar]
  69. Zeithaml, Valarie A., Leonard L. Berry, and A. Parsu Parasuraman. 1993. The nature and determinants of customer expectations of service. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 21: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Zhang, Yingsha, Xiang Li, Qin Su, and Xingbao Hu. 2017. Exploring a theme park’s tourism carrying capacity: A demand-side analysis. Tourism Management 59: 564–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Zhao, Xiaofeng, Jianrong Hou, and Kenneth Gilbert. 2014. Measuring the variance of customer waiting time in service operations. Management Decision 52: 296–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
1
Ding Tai Feng is a chain restaurant in Taiwan that specializes in the production and marketing of pasta. It is famous for its Xiaolongbao. It was recommended by the New York Times as one of the “Top Ten Gourmet Restaurants in the World”.
2
Service failure means that customers are dissatisfied with the entire service experience (Colgate and Norris 2001).
3
Service recovery refers to the responsive actions taken by enterprises to eliminate consumer dissatisfaction when the provided products or services do not meet consumers’ expectations (Maxham and Netemeyer 2002).
4
Word-of-mouth communication refers to information shared among consumers. Such communication is not only vital to consumers’ purchase decisions but also determines the success or failure of an enterprise (Hawkins et al. 2001).
5
Located in the south of Taiwan, Tainan City is the earliest developed city in Taiwan and is renowned for its local cuisine. The local people have a cognitive name for the cuisine.
Table 1. Bid values for the double-bounded dichotomous choice model.
Table 1. Bid values for the double-bounded dichotomous choice model.
Survey Order (Questionnaire Version)WTW for a Sincere Apology (Without Tangible Service Recovery) from the Restaurant (min)Valid Questionnaire for Psychological Service RecoveryWTW for Tangible Service Recovery Provided by the Restaurant (min)Valid Questionnaire for Tangible Service Recovery
Group 1 (questionnaire A)5 (2.5/10)1345 (2.5/10)138
Group 2 (questionnaire B)10 (5/20)14110 (5/20)133
Group 3 (questionnaire C)15 (7.5/30)13220 (10/40)137
Group 4 (questionnaire D)20 (10/40)13330 (15/60)132
Note: The bid values for the first inquiry are outside the brackets, which determine the bid values for the second inquiry (within the brackets): if the respondents’ replies are positive in the first inquiry, the bid values in the second inquiry increase. Conversely, negative replies in the first inquiry reduce the bid values in the second inquiry. WTW: willingness to wait.
Table 2. Demographics of participants queuing at a restaurant.
Table 2. Demographics of participants queuing at a restaurant.
Demographic VariablesItemsSample SizePercentage
SexMale26445.3%
Female31954.7%
Age20 years or younger132.2%
21–30 years17029.2%
31–40 years18331.4%
41–50 years13322.8%
51–60 years6811.7%
61 years or older162.7%
Marital statusMarried27947.9%
Unmarried29149.9%
Others132.2%
Education levelJunior high school, lower305.1%
Senior/vocational high school16929.0%
Junior college9616.5%
University24141.3%
Graduate school478.1%
OccupationFarming, forestry, fishery, or animal husbandry183.1%
Industrial or business sector13222.6%
Military personnel, civil servant or teacher356.0%
Service sector18631.9%
Student7072.0%
Household management417.0%
Retired193.3%
Self-employed7613.0%
Other61.0%
Monthly incomeNT$20,000 or less13319.4%
NT$20,001–40,00026044.6%
NT$40,001–60,00014825.4%
NT$60,001–80,000396.7%
NT$80,001–100,000132.2%
NT$100,000–150,00040.7%
More than NT$150,00061.0%
Experience of filing a customer complaintYes20815.7%
No37564.3%
Table 3. Satisfaction with the waiting service of the restaurant.
Table 3. Satisfaction with the waiting service of the restaurant.
Survey QuestionsMeanStandard Error
I feel the sincerity of the service personnel3.670.032
I clearly understand the language of the service personnel3.760.029
I think that empathy would solve any inconvenience I experience3.650.030
I feel that the service personnel are well trained and experienced3.620.030
I will immediately think of coming to this restaurant3.490.031
I will voluntarily recommend the service of this restaurant3.530.032
If anyone asks me to recommend a restaurant, I would recommend this one3.600.032
I will continue holding a positive view toward this restaurant in the future3.670.030
Table 4. Settings and description of variables for WTW associated with waiting service recovery provided by the restaurant.
Table 4. Settings and description of variables for WTW associated with waiting service recovery provided by the restaurant.
ItemsDescriptionMeanStandard Deviation
Demographic variablesAgeAge (year)37.240.481
IncomeMean monthly income (dollar)39,352896.202
Restaurant waiting experience and cognitionDinnerDummy variable; 1 = dining with more than five people and 0 = other0.440.021
Waiting patternDummy variable; 1 = on-site queuing and 0 = other0.110.013
Waiting experienceDummy variable; 1 = regards service failure as severe and 0 = other0.550.021
SatisfactionSatisfaction toward the waiting service3.630.026
FacilityDummy variable; 1 = restaurant facilities need improvement and 0 = other0.300.020
Complaint filing experienceDummy variable; 1= have had experience filing a customer complaint and 0 = other0.360.021
Table 5. Estimated results of the WTW function for psychological service recovery.
Table 5. Estimated results of the WTW function for psychological service recovery.
ItemsProbability Distribution Pattern of Evaluation Functions
WeibullGammaLognormal
Intercept2.06
(9.10) ***
1.81
(7.60) ***
1.79
(7.58) ***
Demographic variablesAge−0.164
(−2.61) ***
−0.161
(−2.38) **
−0.156
(−2.30) **
Income−0.000000595
(−0.41)
−0.00000101
(−0.68)
−0.00000109
(−0.72)
Restaurant waiting experience and cognitionDinner−0.0832
(−1.39)
−0.0757
(−1.18)
−0.0715
(−1.11)
Waiting pattern0.103
(1.04)
0.111
(1.06)
0.1.13
(1.07)
Waiting experience0.133
(2.19) **
0.163
(2.48) **
0.168
(2.56) **
Satisfaction0.323
(5.49) ***
0.329
(5.50) ***
0.327
(5.46) ***
Facility0.188
(2.68) ***
0.148
(1.98) **
0.142
(1.90) **
Complaint filing experience−0.155
(−2.49) **
−0.123
(−1.83) *
−0.117
(−1.74) *
Scale0.488
(21.45) ***
0.602
(17.66) ***
0.616
(22.50) ***
Log likelihood−532.14−522.32−522.56
Log likelihood ratio45.40 ***42.50 ***42.05 ***
Note: t values are within parentheses and coefficient values outside the parentheses. *, **, and *** denote significance levels at 10%, 5% and 1%, respectively. Log likelihood ratio = (−2) × (restricted log likelihood − log likelihood), x 2 ( 8 ,   0.01 ) = 20.09 .
Table 6. Estimated results of the WTW function for tangible service recovery.
Table 6. Estimated results of the WTW function for tangible service recovery.
ItemsProbability Distribution Pattern of Evaluation Functions
WeibullGammaLognormal
Intercept3.12
(14.61) ***
2.75
(12.63) ***
2.77
(13.00) ***
Demographic variablesAge−0.385
(−6.43) ***
−0.359
(−5.52) ***
−0.365
(−5.72) ***
Income−0.000000131
(−0.09)
−0.000000.409
(−0.28)
−0.000000378
(−0.26)
Restaurant waiting experience and cognitionDinner0.109
(1.89) *
0.118
(1.96) *
0.118
(1.97) **
Waiting pattern0.125
(1.80) *
0.152
(2.12) **
0.148
(2.07) **
Waiting experience0.127
(2.18) **
0.168
(2.69) ***
0.163
(2.65) ***
Satisfaction0.138
(2.54) **
0.153
(2.86) ***
0.155
(2.90) ***
Facility0.113
(1.73) *
0.099
(1.39)
0.105
(1.51)
Complaint filing experience−0.13
(−2.19) **
−0.0646
(−1.01)
−0.0712
(−1.13)
Scale0.464
(21.09) ***
0.577
(19.67) ***
0.569
(21.70) ***
Log likelihood−513.55−497.38−497.56
Log likelihood ratio59.14 ***56.35 ***57.19 ***
Note: t values are within parentheses and coefficient values are outside the parentheses. *, **, and *** denote significance level at 10%, 5% and 1%, respectively. Log likelihood ratio = (−2) × (restricted log likelihood − log likelihood), x 2 ( 8 ,   0.01 ) = 20.09 .
Table 7. Estimated result of the WTW associated with restaurant service recovery (Weibull distribution).
Table 7. Estimated result of the WTW associated with restaurant service recovery (Weibull distribution).
Evaluation CriterionMean95% Confidence Interval
Apology (psychological recovery)20.93(17.62–24.89)
Apology and relevant dining discounts (tangible service recovery)30.82(26.94–37.63)
Unit: min/frequency/person.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ting, C.-T.; Huang, Y.-S.; Lin, C.-T.; Pan, S.-C. Evaluation of Consumers’ WTP for Service Recovery in Restaurants: Waiting Time Perspective. Adm. Sci. 2019, 9, 63. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci9030063

AMA Style

Ting C-T, Huang Y-S, Lin C-T, Pan S-C. Evaluation of Consumers’ WTP for Service Recovery in Restaurants: Waiting Time Perspective. Administrative Sciences. 2019; 9(3):63. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci9030063

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ting, Chung-Te, Yu-Sheng Huang, Cheng-Te Lin, and Szu-Chin Pan. 2019. "Evaluation of Consumers’ WTP for Service Recovery in Restaurants: Waiting Time Perspective" Administrative Sciences 9, no. 3: 63. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci9030063

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop