Next Article in Journal
Impact of Staff Localization on Turnover: The Role of a Foreign Subsidiary CEO
Previous Article in Journal
The Individuals’ Willingness to Get the Vaccine for COVID-19 during the Third Wave: A Study on Trust in Mainstream Information Sources, Attitudes and Framing Effect
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Children’s Mental Health: Discrepancy between Child Self-Reporting and Parental Reporting

by
Alejandra Caqueo-Urízar
1,*,
Alfonso Urzúa
2,
Ester Villalonga-Olives
3,
Diego Atencio-Quevedo
4,5,
Matías Irarrázaval
6,7,
Jerome Flores
4,5 and
Cristian Ramírez
4,5
1
Instituto de Alta Investigación, Universidad de Tarapacá, Arica 1000000, Chile
2
Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Católica del Norte, Antofagasta 1270460, Chile
3
Pharmaceutical Health Services Research Department, University of Maryland School of Pharmacy, Baltimore, MD 21201, USA
4
Escuela de Psicología y Filosofía, Universidad de Tarapacá, Arica 1000000, Chile
5
Centro de Justicia Educacional (CJE), Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago 8940855, Chile
6
Departamento de Psiquiatría, Facultad de Medicina, Hospital Clínico Universidad de Chile, Santiago 8380453, Chile
7
Institute for Depression and Personality Research (MIDAP), Santiago 8380453, Chile
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2022, 12(10), 401; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100401
Submission received: 10 September 2022 / Revised: 14 October 2022 / Accepted: 17 October 2022 / Published: 19 October 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Child and Adolescent Psychiatry)

Abstract

:
(1) Background: Discrepancies between children’s self-reports and their parents’ reports on mental health indicators are associated with measurement errors or informant bias. However, they are a valuable tool in understanding the course of child psychopathology. This study aims to determine the level of discrepancies between parents’ perceptions and children’s self-reports in mental health indicators in Northern Chile. (2) Methods: A System of Evaluation of Children and Adolescents self-report (Sistema de Evaluación de Niños y Adolescentes, SENA) was responded to by 408 students between 8 and 13 years old and their parents. (3) Results: Children reported a significantly higher frequency of emotional problems, defiant behavior, and executive functions as compared to their parents’ responses. (4) Conclusions: There is a disjunction between the report of parents and children, which could originate in poor family communication.

1. Introduction

Mental health in children and adolescents is a topic that has gained considerable visibility over the last few years [1]. As stated in the Global Burden of Disease of the World Health Organization, it has become one of the main causes of disability in the population between 10 and 24 years old [2]. The concern about mental health indicators at early ages arises from the interaction of multiple factors that can trigger maladaptive reactions in different contexts [3], for which reason a multi-informant assessment approach has been adopted including the reports of those who share close relationships or significant time with the index patient [4]. In the case of the pediatric population, one of the most widely used sources of information, in addition to the child’s own self-report, is that of their parents or caregivers [5].
Although the use of multiple informants in mental health assessment in the infant population benefits the understanding of the psychological functioning of children, discrepancies have been observed between the self-reports and the data provided by parents [6], and also between parents and teachers [7,8], generating new challenges for clinical practice, research, and theory related to child psychiatry and psychopathology [9].
These discrepancies have been extensively studied [10,11], showing that children as young as 6 years old may report independently regarding their health, as compared to parental reporting [12,13]. Furthermore, studies in the 1970s indicated that girls tend to be more reliable informants than boys [14]. Moreover, when externalized problems are analyzed, it seems that parents tend to be more precise than their children; however, when internalized symptoms are analyzed, there seems to be less agreement about which group reports symptoms better [15]. This disagreement stems from the fact that children value their behavior more positively than parents do [10]; although, it becomes more complicated towards adolescence, where they seem to report poorer health than parents, especially in emotional health [16].
Various studies support that these discrepancies are based on an underestimation of anxious and depressive symptoms by the parents [17,18,19,20]. In this sense, it has been observed that children tend to report more internalized symptoms such as depression and anxiety than their parents do.
However, when externalizing symptoms such as defiant behavior or hyperactivity are under evaluation, some studies have found a higher level of agreement between both groups [9,21].
In that sense, a meta-analysis carried out by Los Reyes et al. [4] including 341 studies published between 1989 and 2014, observed low-to-moderate correspondence between children’s self-report and parents’ report (mean internalizing: r = 0.25; mean externalizing: r = 0.30; mean overall: r = 0.28).
Additionally, there are certain contextual factors that associate with higher agreement or discrepancy between reports. It has been observed that when there is a higher socioeconomic level, parents tend to underestimate the problems related to their children’s mental health, while in lower socioeconomic levels, the opposite happens [22]. Furthermore, family factors, such as parenting style, lack of communication, and conflict between parents and children, have been associated with higher levels of discrepancy in both reports [23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32], while family cohesion and parental acceptance have shown fewer discrepancies [9,25].
Traditionally, these observed differences have been interpreted as a function of measurement errors and informant bias [26]. However, such discrepancies may be significant to understand the nature and course of child and adolescent psychopathology, as they may reflect underlying family problems, which potentially contribute to the development of psychopathologies [27].
Regarding Chilean culture, the parent–child relationship is significantly framed in a pedagogic–affective relationship of unidirectional transmission, both of values and of interpersonal experiences of recognition and affection, even reaching a parental determinism [28]. This is complemented with a complex relationship of interdependence between children and parents [28,29], which would lead the children to act as “good child” or “without problems” as a support response to the parents, leading to a more limited and biased view of their children’s symptoms [30].
Likewise in Chile, there has been little research on the differences between the reports of the respondents [1]. A study by Urzúa et al. [31] addressed the discrepancies and similarities between parents and children, concluding that the different groups were more likely to have different scores, especially in the psychological dimensions. The aim of this study is to determine the level of discrepancies in mental health indicators in parents’ perception and self-reporting of their children in Northern Chile. The hypothesis of the study is the existence of discrepancies between the children’s report versus parents’ perception.
The findings are intended to contribute to the adequate detection of mental health problems in these children and lay the groundwork for the implementation of mental health promotion, prevention, and treatment programs in the child population.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A convenient sample was used. The initial sample was composed of 5203 elementary and high school students from public, subsidized, and private educational institutions in Arica. For this study, 408 elementary school students were selected, of whom 56.8% (n = 232) were girls. In total, 40% (n = 163) belonged to the fourth grade, 25% (n = 102) to the fifth grade, and 35% (n = 143) to the sixth grade. The mean age was 9.97 (SD = 0.96). Sociodemographics can be seen in Table 1.

2.2. Instuments

Ad-Hoc Socio-Demographic Scale: Used to Identify the Sex, Age, and Grade of Students

The System of Evaluation of Children and Adolescents (Sistema de Evaluación de Niños y Adolescentes, SENA) [32]: The instrument is composed of 9 questionnaires, to assess a broad spectrum of emotional and behavioral problems at three age levels: Infant (3–6 years), Primary (6–12 years), and Secondary (12–18 years). The different questionnaire models have a multi-dimensional approach made up of three blocks of scales: problem scales, vulnerability scales, and personal resources scales.
For this study, the results are reflected in different problem indexes:
(a)
Global Index of Problems: general summary of the level of discomfort and general affectation presented by people under evaluation. It is calculated from the average of the scales of internalized and externalized problems.
(b)
Index of Emotional Problems: it summarizes the level of internalized problems presented by the person evaluated, constituting an indicator of emotional alterations and symptoms related to affective or mood disorders. It is composed of the scales of Anxiety (It’s hard for me to make decisions), Depression (It’s hard for me to find things that I really enjoy), Social Anxiety (I get nervous when there are too many people near me), and Somatic Complaints (I wake up tired in morning).
(c)
Index of Behavioral Problems: indicator of external and disruptive behavioral manifestations that burst into the environment generating interpersonal conflicts and hindering the normal development of activities. It is composed of the scales of Anger Control Problems (When I get angry, I scream to others), Aggression (I make fun of other people for fun), and Defiant Behavior (I do what I want even if I get grounded).
(d)
Index of Problems in Executive Functions: difficulties in the set of cognitive processes that intervene in goal-directed behavior, allowing planning, goal setting, performance monitoring, and inhibition of ineffective responses. It is composed of the scales of Attention Problems (My teachers say I don’t pay attention in classes), Hyperactivity-Impulsivity (It’s hard for me to stay seated), and Emotional Regulation Problems (It’s hard for me to understand my feelings).
Recently, Sánchez-Sánchez, Fernández-Pinto, Santamaría, Carrasco y del Barrio [33] found that subscales’ reliability was adequate (α > 0.7) in Spain. For this investigation, the versions Self-report—Elementary (134 items) and Family—Elementary (124 items) were used for the parents’ report.

2.3. Procedure

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Tarapacá and was carried out in compliance with the ethical principles from the Helsinki Declaration for Research in Humans. Informed consent was requested from parents after explaining the purpose and scope of the study, and later the consent was given by the students themselves. Later, at least two trained interviewers were present to answer questions, in the company of the main teacher of the class. The duration of each session was approximately 45 min.

2.4. Data Analysis

In the first instance, descriptive analyses of the study variables were carried out. In comparing groups, the condition of normality was tested with the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, and it was considered that the criteria were met, so the mean comparison analyses were performed using t-tests for paired-samples [34]. First, t-tests were conducted to compare the averages of children’s and parents’ reports of problem rates. Second, the comparison of means between children’s self-report and their parents’ report of the subscales that composed each index of problems was carried out. Student and parent observations were considered as groups, so parents’ scores were not paired with their children’s.
All analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) software version 25.0 [35].

3. Results

As shown in Table 2, the mean reported by students tends to be higher in all indexes as compared to their parents. The Global Index shows the greatest differences in averages. Moreover, both the Index of Emotional Problems, and the Index of Executive Functions have significant differences. On the contrary, the differences between the scores reported in the Index of Behavioral Problems and Personal Resources do not present significant differences between the student and parents’ groups.
As shown in Table 3, for the Index of Emotional Problems, the variables appear higher in the child’s self-report compared to what is reported by the parents, except for the anxiety scale. The highest statistically significant difference is observed in the somatic complaints. Regarding the Index of Behavioral Problems, the mean difference in the scale of defiant behavior is statistically significant, where the reported symptomatology is higher for the child than for his or her parents. Finally, for the Index of Problems in Executive Functions, the reports from the students are higher than the reports from parents, being that the Index of Problems in Emotional Regulation is the only one that presents a statistically significant difference.

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to determine the level of discrepancies in mental health indicators between parents’ perceptions and children’s self-reports for students in Northern Chile. The findings of this research show significant differences between children’s self-reporting compared to their parents’ reporting, where children perceive more difficulties on emotional and executive functions compared to their parents.
Regarding emotional problems, it was observed that children report more symptoms than their parents can perceive. This is in line with previous studies that have systematically shown the existence of significant differences between children’s and their parents’ reporting of mental health indicators, particularly in internalized problems [17,18,19,20,36]. The existing literature has hypothesized that, due to the intrapersonal nature of internalized symptomatology, children are more sensitive to it than their parents, making it difficult for parents to correctly assess these difficulties [10]. Nevertheless, this could also imply that due to problems in communication between children and their parents, there are difficulties in detecting their children’s emotional problems.
In terms of problems in executive functions, it is observed that the children reported significantly more difficulties regulating their emotions than their parents did. This may be due to the age range of the sample (9 to 11 years, on average), since this period is characterized by an increased academic demand and body changes, affecting their levels of anxiety and discomfort, so it is to be expected that emotional regulatory skills may be particularly compromised [37].
When analyzing parent–child discrepancies in behavioral problems, no significant differences were observed overall, which is consistent with the previous research [4,9,21,38]. These results imply that the behavioral elements are the most relevant when predicting the level of agreement between parents and children, since these symptoms are usually concrete, observable, severe, and unpleasant, so parents tend to be more willing to report such behaviors.
Finally, it is worth mentioning that this study has limitations. The first is related to the representativeness of the population, both because of the small sample size and the non-probability sampling technique. Secondly, sociodemographic variables such as gender or age were not assessed as a source of discrepancy, while there is evidence that suggests that those factors play a role in the levels of discrepancy between parents and children [39,40]. Finally, the cross-sectional nature of the study did not allow the trajectories of these discrepancies to be determined over time. Future research should be conducted on larger and more diverse samples to improve the generalizability of the results and should consider sociodemographic characteristics of the sample, focusing on understanding the trajectories of parent–child discrepancies over time.
Within the implications of this study, the findings appear to account for a certain “disconnection” between parents and children. Possible reasons for this lack of congruence could be triggered by the prevailing economic system in which both parents work outside the home and in some cases with long working hours, which determines that children spend more time alone, generating fewer instances of time for the family system [41,42]. In the local reality, this is accentuated by the mining activity in the area, since parents move to these mining centers by shift systems that generally imply seven by seven (seven days of work at the mine and seven days of rest at home), which could be negatively impacting the dynamics of the family group [43,44].
This disconnection has also been associated by several studies with adverse effects on children’s mental health indicators, suggesting that poor parental supervision is associated with less behavioral self-control in adolescents [45], as well as less parent–child connection, and leads to lower orientation towards the success of the latter [46]. Finally, the existence of family–work conflicts has been associated as a critical mediating effect on the emergence of maladaptive perfectionist behaviors in children, especially among females [47].
These results suggest the design of interventions that promote greater and better communication within the family, since one of the main causes for consulting for mental health problems in children and adolescents is that their caregivers cannot perceive such difficulties [48], being that this domain is a protective factor in the appearance of mental health problems in children and adolescents [9,25].

5. Conclusions

In the studied sample, there are significant differences between children’s self-reporting and their parents’ reporting of mental health indicators, with a tendency for children to perceive greater symptomatology as compared to their parents. These discrepancies could be reduced in the future with the design of interventions that promote communication and optimize interactions in the parent–child relationship.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.C.-U., A.U., E.V.-O., D.A.-Q., M.I. and C.R.; methodology, A.C.-U., D.A.-Q., J.F. and C.R.; formal analysis, A.U., M.I., J.F. and C.R.; investigation, A.C.-U., A.U., E.V.-O., D.A.-Q. and C.R.; writing—original draft preparation, A.C.-U., A.U., E.V.-O., D.A.-Q. and C.R.; writing—review and editing, A.C.-U., A.U., E.V.-O., M.I., D.A.-Q. and J.F.; supervision, A.C-U. and A.U. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Agencia Nacional de Investigación y Desarrollo de Chile (ANID), grant ANID PIA CIE 160007.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of Universidad de Tarapacá (26-2017).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Gaete, J.; Montero-Marin, J.; Valenzuela, D.; Rojas-Barahona, C.A.; Olivares, E.; Araya, R. Mental health among children and adolescents: Construct validity, reliability, and parent-adolescent agreement on the ‘Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire’ in Chile. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0191809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Gore, F.M.; Bloem, P.J.; Patton, G.C.; Ferguson, J.; Joseph, V.; Coffey, C.; Sawyer, S.; Mathers, C.D. Global burden of disease in young people aged 10–24 years: A systematic analysis. Lancet 2011, 377, 2093–2102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Sanislow, C.A.; Pine, D.S.; Quinn, K.J.; Kozak, M.J.; Garvey, M.A.; Heinssen, R.K.; Wang, P.; Cuthbert, B.N. Developing constructs for psychopathology research: Research domain criteria. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2010, 119, 631–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. De Los Reyes, A.; Augenstein, T.M.; Wang, M.; Thomas, S.A.; Drabick, D.A.; Burgers, D.E.; Rabinowitz, J. The validity of the multi-informant approach to assessing child and adolescent mental health. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 141, 858–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Hunsley, J.; Mash, E.J. Evidence-based assessment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2007, 3, 29–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. De Los Reyes, A. Strategic objectives for improving understanding of informant discrepancies in developmental psychopathology research. Dev. Psychopathol. 2013, 25, 669–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Montiel-Nava, C.; Peña, J.A. Discrepancia entre padres y profesores en la evaluación de problemas de conducta y académicos en niños y adolescentes. Rev. Neurol. 2001, 32, 506–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Angulo, R.; Jané, M.; Bonillo, A.; Viñas, F.; Corcoll-Champredonde, A.; González, G.; Zepeda, R.; Dos Santos, N.; Carbonès, J. Evaluación de la sintomatología negativista desafiante en niños de seis a ocho años: Concordancia entre padres y maestros. Psicothema 2010, 22, 455–459. [Google Scholar]
  9. Chen, Y.Y.; Ho, S.Y.; Lee, P.C.; Wu, C.K.; Gau, S.S.F. Parent-child discrepancies in the report of adolescent emotional and behavioral problems in Taiwan. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0178863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Achenbach, T.M.; McConaughy, S.H.; Howell, C.T. Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychol. Bull. 1987, 101, 213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Van Roy, B.; Groholt, B.; Heyerdahl, S.; Clench-Aas, J. Understanding discrepancies in parent-child reporting of emotional and behavioural problems: Effects of relational and socio-demographic factors. BMC Psychiatry 2010, 10, 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Riley, A.W. Evidence that school-age children can self-report on their health. Ambul. Pediatr. 2004, 4, 371–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Lundqvist, C.; Rugland, E.; Clench-Aas, J.; Bartonova, A.; Hofoss, D. Children are reliable reporters of common symptoms: Results from a self-reported symptom diary for primary school children. Acta Paediatr. 2010, 99, 1054–1059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Herjanic, B.; Herjanic, M.; Brown, F.; Wheatt, T. Are children reliable reporters? J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 1975, 3, 41–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Silverman, W.K.; Eisen, A.R. Age differences in the reliability of parent and child reports of child anxious symptomatology using a structured interview. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1992, 31, 117–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Waters, E.; Stewart-Brown, S.; Fitzpatrick, R. Agreement between adolescent self-report and parent reports of health and well-being: Results of an epidemiological study. Child Care Health 2003, 29, 501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rescorla, L.A.; Ginzburg, S.; Achenbach, T.M.; Ivanova, M.Y.; Almqvist, F.; Begovac, I.; Bilenberg, N.; Bird, H.; Chahed, M.; Dobrean, A.; et al. Cross-informant agreement between parent-reported and adolescent self-reported problems in 25 societies. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2013, 42, 262–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Rescorla, L.A.; Bochicchio, L.; Achenbach, T.M.; Ivanova, M.Y.; Almqvist, F.; Begovac, I.; Bilenberg, N.; Bird, H.; Dobrean, A.; Erol, N.; et al. Parent–teacher agreement on children’s problems in 21 societies. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2014, 43, 627–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Salbach-Andrae, H.; Lenz, K.; Lehmkuhl, U. Patterns of agreement among parent, teacher and youth ratings in a referred sample. Eur. Psychiatry 2009, 24, 345–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Wang, J.; Liu, L.; Wu, H.; Yang, X.; Wang, Y.; Wang, L. Agreement between parents and adolescents on emotional and behavioral problems and its associated factors among Chinese school adolescents: A cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry 2014, 14, 114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Ederer, E.M. Mental health problems in young children: Self-reports and significant others as informants. Psychol. Test Assess. Modeling 2004, 46, 123–140. [Google Scholar]
  22. Tzeng, W.C.; Lipson, J.G. The cultural context of suicide stigma in Taiwan. Qual. Health Res. 2004, 14, 345–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. De Los Reyes, A.; Kazdin, A.E. Informant discrepancies in assessing child dysfunction relate to dysfunction within mother-child interactions. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2006, 15, 643–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. García, J.A.; Sánchez, J.M.R. Prácticas educativas familiares y autoestima. Psicothema 2005, 17, 76–82. [Google Scholar]
  25. Treutler, C.M.; Epkins, C.C. Are discrepancies among child, mother, and father reports on children’s behavior related to parents’ psychological symptoms and aspects of parent–child relationships? J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2003, 31, 13–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Al Ghriwati, N.; Winter, M.A.; Greenlee, J.L.; Thompson, E.L. Discrepancies between parent and self-reports of adolescent psychosocial symptoms: Associations with family conflict and asthma outcomes. J. Fam. Psychol. 2018, 32, 992–997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Guion, K.; Mrug, S.; Windle, M. Predictive value of informant discrepancies in reports of parenting: Relations to early adolescents’ adjustment. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2009, 37, 17–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Chávez, P.; Vergara, A. Ser Niño y Niña en el Chile de Hoy. La Perspectiva de sus Protagonistas Acerca de la Infancia, la Adultez y las Relaciones Entre Padres e Hijos; Ceibo: Santiago, Chile, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  29. Ibarra, P.; Del Solar, A. El problema del cuidado desde la perspectiva de los niños (as): Análisis crítico del discurso sobre el cuidado entre padres e hijos (as). Univ. Psychol. 2018, 17, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Vergara, A.C.; Sepúlveda, M.A.; Chávez, P.B. Parentalidades intensivas y éticas del cuidado: Discursos de niños y adultos de estrato bajo de Santiago, Chile. Psicoperspectivas 2018, 17, 67–77. [Google Scholar]
  31. Urzúa, A.; Cortés, E.; Vega, S.; Prieto, L.; Tapia, K. Propiedades psicométricas del cuestionario de auto reporte de la calidad de vida KIDSCREEN-27 en adolescentes chilenos. Ter. Psicológica 2009, 27, 83–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Fernández-Pinto, I.; Santamaría, P.; Sánchez-Sánchez, F.; Carrasco, M.A.; del Barrio, V. SENA. Sistema de Evaluación de Niños y Adolescentes. Manual de Aplicación, Corrección e Interpretación; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  33. Sánchez-Sánchez, F.; Fernández-Pinto, I.; Santamaría, P.; Carrasco, M.; y del Barrio, V. SENA, Sistema de Evaluación de Niños y Adolescentes: Proceso de desarrollo y evidencias de fiabilidad y validez. Rev. Psicol. Clínica Con Niños Adolesc. 2016, 3, 23–34. [Google Scholar]
  34. Forero, C.G.; Maydeu-Olivares, A.; Gallardo-Pujol, D. Factor analysis with ordinal indicators: A monte Carlo study comparing DWLS and ULS. Struct. Equ. Modeling Multidiscip. J. 2009, 16, 625–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0.; IBM Corp.: Armonk, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  36. Rescorla, L.A. Cross-Cultural Perspectives on Parent–Adolescent Discrepancies: Existing Findings and Future Directions. J. Youth Adolesc. 2016, 45, 2185–2196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Punaro, L.; Reeve, R. Relationships between 9-year-olds’ math and literacy worries and academic abilities. Child Dev. Res. 2012, 2012, 359089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Grills, A.E.; Ollendick, T.H. Issues in parent-child agreement: The case of structured diagnostic interviews. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2002, 5, 57–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Fernández, M.A.; Arias, R.M.; Sutil, C.R. Prevalencia de trastornos psicológicos en niños y adolescentes, su relación con la edad y el género. Psicothema 2000, 12, 525–532. [Google Scholar]
  40. Van der Meer, M.; Dixon, A.; Rose, D. Parent and child agreement on reports of problem behaviour obtained from a screening questionnaire, the SDQ. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2008, 17, 491–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gracia, P.; Kalmijn, M. Parents’ Family Time and Work Schedules: The Split-Shift Schedule in Spain. J. Marriage Fam. 2015, 78, 401–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lee, S.; McHale, S.M.; Crouter, A.C.; Hammer, L.B.; Almeida, D.M. Finding time over time: Longitudinal links between employed mothers’ work–family conflict and time profiles. J. Fam. Psychol. 2017, 31, 604–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Silva, J.; Salinas, P. “Me canso de ser hombre”: Paternidad periférica en el trabajo minero chileno. Sociol. Probl. Práticas 2020, 93, 31–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Stefanovic, A.; Saavedra, M. Las Mujeres en el Sector Minero de Chile. Propuestas Para Políticas Públicas de Igualdad. Cepal, Cooperación Alemana. 2016. Available online: http://repositorio.cepal.org/bitstream/handle/11362/40405/4/S1600926_es.pdf (accessed on 10 August 2022).
  45. Li, J.B.; Dou, K. Low Involvement and Ineffective Monitoring Link Mothers’ Work-Family Conflict and Adolescent Self-Control. J. Fam. Issues 2021, 42, 1384–1398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Masa, R.; Bates, C.; Chowa, G. Direct and Indirect Associations of Food Insecurity, Adolescent–Parent Relationship, and Adolescent Future Orientation. J. Fam. Issues 2021, 42, 1285–1307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Ekmekci, O.T.; Metin Camgoz, S.; Bayhan Karapinar, P. Path to Well-being: Moderated Mediation Model of Perfectionism, Family–Work Conflict, and Gender. J. Fam. Issues 2021, 42, 1852–1879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ezpeleta, L.; Granero, R.; De La Osa, N.; Domenech, J.M.; Guillamón, N. Perception of need for help and use of mental health services in children and adolescents. Do they share the same predictors? Psicothema 2002, 14, 532–539. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Sociodemographics.
Table 1. Sociodemographics.
nPercentage (%)
GenderMale17643.2
Female23256.8
Grade4th16340
5th10225
6th14335
MinMaxMeanMedianSD a
Age8139.97100.96
Notes: a SD = Standard Deviation.
Table 2. T-test of Global Indexes reported by children and their parents.
Table 2. T-test of Global Indexes reported by children and their parents.
VariableReportTMean DifferenceSig.
ChildrenParents
M (SD) *M (SD) * p
Global Index of Problems52.7 (8.09)39.9 (7.57)2.47412.8280.014
Index of Emotional Problems 54.2 (9.09)48.9 (8.78)1.9673.6600.050
Index of Behavioral Problems 49.9 (9.68)46.3 (8.27)1.3331.1110.183
Index of Problems in Executive Functions52.5 (9.26)38.9 (8.09)2.6312.3280.009
Index of Personal Resources42.5 (10.49)39.2 (8.98)0.8990.7400.369
Note: * M (SD) = Mean (Standard Deviation).
Table 3. T-test between children and parents’ report scores.
Table 3. T-test between children and parents’ report scores.
VariableReportTMean DifferenceSig.
ChildrenParents
Index of Emotional ProblemsM (SD) *M (SD) * p
Anxiety49.1 (9.06)50.9 (10.84)−2.560−1.7940.011
Depression56.3 (13.16)55.3 (11.90)1.1861.0490.236
Social Anxiety52.6 (9.87)47.0 (9.40)8.3435.6420.000
Somatic Complains54.9 (11.03)48.6 (10.89)2.3166.2300.021
Index of Behavioral ProblemsM (SD) *M (SD) * p
Anger Control Problems49.8 (10.94) 49.1 (10.91)0.9560.7370.339
Defiant Behavior51.8 (13.19)49.9 (10.02)2.3791.9380.018
Aggression49.8 (10.93)49.1 (7.46)1.3493.5780.178
Index of Problems in Executive FunctionsM (SD) *M (SD) * p
Attention52.8 (10.20)48.2 (9.42)1.7084.5490.088
Hyperactivity50.4 (10.36)45.3 (9.47)1.9335.1390.054
Emotional Regulation53.5 (11.29)45.1 (10.68)2.2548.3700.024
Note: * M (SD) = Mean (Standard Deviation).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Caqueo-Urízar, A.; Urzúa, A.; Villalonga-Olives, E.; Atencio-Quevedo, D.; Irarrázaval, M.; Flores, J.; Ramírez, C. Children’s Mental Health: Discrepancy between Child Self-Reporting and Parental Reporting. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 401. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100401

AMA Style

Caqueo-Urízar A, Urzúa A, Villalonga-Olives E, Atencio-Quevedo D, Irarrázaval M, Flores J, Ramírez C. Children’s Mental Health: Discrepancy between Child Self-Reporting and Parental Reporting. Behavioral Sciences. 2022; 12(10):401. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100401

Chicago/Turabian Style

Caqueo-Urízar, Alejandra, Alfonso Urzúa, Ester Villalonga-Olives, Diego Atencio-Quevedo, Matías Irarrázaval, Jerome Flores, and Cristian Ramírez. 2022. "Children’s Mental Health: Discrepancy between Child Self-Reporting and Parental Reporting" Behavioral Sciences 12, no. 10: 401. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs12100401

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop