Next Article in Journal
Beliefs and Experiences of Individuals Following a Zero-Carb Diet
Previous Article in Journal
Football as an Alternative to Work on the Development of Social Skills in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder with Level 1
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Teachers’ Irrational Belief Scale: Psychometric Properties of the Greek Version and Measurement Invariance across Genders

by
Angelos Gkontelos
1,
Julie Vaiopoulou
2 and
Dimitrios Stamovlasis
1,*
1
Department of Philosophy and Education, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 54124 Thessaloniki, Greece
2
Department of Education, University of Nicosia, Nicosia 2417, Cyprus
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2021, 11(11), 160; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11110160
Submission received: 3 October 2021 / Revised: 10 November 2021 / Accepted: 19 November 2021 / Published: 22 November 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Educational Psychology)

Abstract

:
Irrationality refers to human thoughts and beliefs that signify lack of rationality and entail erroneous perceptions about situational, personal, or collective idiosyncrasies, while it is independent of one’s intellectual ability. Irrational beliefs are ubiquitous in all social and cultural groups and attract a special interest in behavioral sciences, where the primary concern is the development of instruments for identifying and measuring them. The present study evaluates the psychometric properties of Greek version of Teachers’ Irrational Belief Scale (TIBS-G), a 25-item self-reported instrument using data collected from 835 participants. The exploratory procedure, implementing scree plot with parallel analysis, demonstrated the dimensionality of four factors, namely: Self-downing (SD), Authoritarianism (A), Demands for Justice (DJ), and Low Frustration Tolerance (LT). The corresponding reliability measures using Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega were ranged between 0.70 and 0.80. Subsequently, confirmatory factor analysis showed an adequate fit of the measurement model [χ2 = 579.98, df = 183, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.960; TLI = 0.956; RMSEA = 0.051]. In addition, measurement invariance was performed, which demonstrated differences between genders. Finally, discussion on the importance of irrational beliefs and the possible implementation of the TIBS instrument in educational research is provided.

1. Introduction

Irrational beliefs (IB) are the beliefs adhered to cognitions that signify lack of rationality and often entail misconceptions or erroneous perceptions about situational, personal, or collective idiosyncrasies. The irrational thoughts affect cognitive processes, decision-making and farther human behavior, while they are also associated with emotional states such as frustration and anxiety [1,2]. They are often characterized as dysfunctional beliefs or dysfunctional myths, which manifest themselves under various situations and in different forms [3]. Researchers have shown a special interest in studying them and have attempted to develop means for identifying and measuring those thoughts. Irrationality, as manifested by human mind is not an easy issue to theoretically define and empirically measure. However, it is omnipresent, and it attracts a considerable attention in many fields. The current work presents a study on the psychometric properties of the Greek version of Teachers’ Irrational Belief Scale (TIBS-G) [4].

1.1. Irrationality and REBT

Irrationality is culturally ubiquitous and detected in all social and cultural groups studied historically and anthropologically. Often, human behavior shows evidence of irrational thinking and beliefs, independently of one’s intellectual ability and intelligence. The term of irrational beliefs is encountered in many fields of behavioral sciences. Rational-Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT) [5] defines those beliefs as non-pragmatic, illogical and absolutist, non-reality-based and/or rigid leading to dysfunctional consequences. Conversely, rational beliefs are defined as pragmatic, logical and non-absolutist, reality-based and/or flexible followed by functional consequences [6,7]. In literature, rational beliefs are often characterized as low levels of irrational beliefs. However, research has shown that rational and irrational beliefs are not a simple bipolar construct, but they are distinct dimensions in a more complex structure [8]. Irrational beliefs generate dysfunctional feelings and behaviors because of biasing information processing of specific activating input [9]. In that process, the social-cultural environment has a large share of responsibility since it is the main source of predisposed beliefs of any kind, which could affect individuals [10].
Albert Ellis [5], the REBT’s founder, vehemently states that people, when confronting events which avert their goals, despite their tendencies to use rational thinking, encounter difficulty in dismissing their irrational beliefs that are related to their life’s demands. Irrationality appears resistant to change. In the REBT framework, it is stated that the individuals’ unrealistic and irrational beliefs about themselves or others can provoke unhealthy negative emotions, such as stress, anger and depression [11,12]. In addition, it is pointed out that irrational beliefs bring about not only dysfunctional emotional consequences, but also self-collapsing behaviors, and thus their study is fundamental for understanding human psychological health [13]. To this end, REBT as a cognitive-behavioral approach to psychotherapy had proposed processes-model for confronting related issues [14].

1.2. IB’s Dimensions and Their Behavioral Consequences

Ellis’s earlier work describes eleven types of IB [14], however, four main categories prevail: (a) demandingness, (b) self/global downing, (c) awfulizing and (d) low frustration tolerance. These types of IB are associated with various content areas and are relevant to aspects of one’s or other people’s life [15]. Specifically, demands are considered to be the root cause of IB, from which all other beliefs come. They play an key role in creating a series of dysfunctional emotions linked with behavioral consequences [13,16]. That is, when a person’s IB relate to self-destruction, the individual evaluates a particular trait, behavior, or action according to his/her desire, and fosters this attitude constantly [17]. Individual self-downing show the highest correlation with emotional disorders, affecting anger suppression, along with its violent expressions [18,19]. Awfulized IB are referred to the extreme evaluation of a negative event as manipulative [13] and encompass an exaggeration in the description of people or facts as incredibly devastating and destructive [17]. A person possessing such perceptions cannot accept that worse things can happen [20]. Individual awfulized beliefs are associated with anxiety [8], depression, shame, and guilt [21]. Finally, social interactions could not be left unaffected by the individual’s destructive beliefs. Thus, the correlation of these beliefs with the submissive interpersonal attitude of the individual [13], as well as with social isolation [22] is observed. Finally, low frustration tolerance discourages people from experiencing unpleasant circumstances [21] and it is associated with inability to suppress anger [23]. These beliefs cause problems of self-control [24], delayed gratification of the individual and attempt self-punishment [25].
As forenamed, IB are defined as evaluative beliefs that are not empirically supported, hindering individuals from achieving their basic aims and causing emotional disruptions [26,27]. Note also that the individual’s evaluations of the beliefs are the cause of the emotional affects and not the situations per se, that people are involved in [6].

1.3. Irrationality in the School Framework

The present work focuses on teachers’ irrational beliefs (TIB) which relate to educational processes and school practice. TIB refer to intense and rigid in nature thoughts, comprised of extreme beliefs or evaluations. Specifically, irrational beliefs are evaluative cognitions that are identified as the smallest unit of knowledge that can stand as a distinct assertion [28] and are organized in complex schemas representing the person’s constructed concepts of reality, while they affect behavioral responses to that environment [18]. IB definitely appear to be illogical and non-pragmatic, and prominently without empirical support [29].
Although the origin of IB has not been profoundly understood, some speculations based on empirical evidence posit them, as mentioned earlier, within the emotional destruction context. In a career decision-making process, IB or dysfunctional beliefs or myths are by far influential and both practitioners and researchers attempt to identify and assess them and subsequently discharge or reconstruct them, so that career decision becomes sensible and effective [30]. An environmental stressor has emotional impact on individuals’ beliefs, and especially for teachers, and the unbalance between teaching demands and teaching resources provokes occupational stress. This stress can cause teachers to be susceptible to IB [2]. Indicatively, “Teachers always need a great deal of help from others to solve school-related problems”, “When children have problems, it’s their parent’s fault”, “I must be a perfect teacher and never make mistakes” etc., are examples of listed irrational beliefs specific to teachers [31].
In school practice, some additional demands posited by new situations, such as curriculum innovation, usually intensify TIB, because of the increased fear of failure under the unfamiliar circumstances [32]. Teachers having strong pre-conceived beliefs about a variety of educational issues are challenged by a cognitive dissonance, when the new conditions differ from their initial standard practices. In handling the ensuing dissention, teachers are inclined towards dysfunctional thoughts by fostering overgeneralizations. For instance, when teachers believe “I must effectively instruct this student because I have taught students effectively in the past”, they attempt to rationalize by comparing the present situation with a previous experience. This reasoning adheres obviously to emotional state associated with fear of failure [33].
The conceptual development and theoretical foundation of TIB, as distinct latent constructs, require also to address the measurement issue. The latest developed instrument is the Teacher Irrational Belief Scale (TIBS) [4], a self-reporting questionnaire which appraises the different types/dimensions of the TIB. TIB have been examined in several past surveys along with teachers’ stress [2], efficacy beliefs [33], job satisfaction [34], emotional intelligence and psychological hardiness [35], and burnout [36]. Furthermore, dimensional issues are addressed in various populations [37], whereas other researchers have employed the TIBS scale to investigate stress and distress, respectively [2,38].
The TIBS scale entails four dimensions: a) Self-downing, b) Authoritarianism, c) Demands for Justice, and d) Low Frustration Tolerance. More specifically, the self-downing subscale refers to high standards that teachers set for themselves, which lead them in overblowing needs for social approval, creating dysfunctional thoughts about reduced personal value because of making mistakes. The authoritarianism subscale is associated with teachers’ obstinacy towards their students’ misbehaving. Teachers who adopt authoritarian beliefs support the stern punishment of the students on account of their own disability to cope with students’ behavioral attitudes. The third dimension, demands for justice, lays emphasis on teachers’ needs for active involvement in the school functioning. These demands pertain to their concern that they should be listened to and that their collaboration with the administrators be ideal, while they should be considered in decision-making processes etc. Finally, the low frustration tolerance dimension indicates the expectations of the teaching difficulties. Many teachers tend to look upon teaching as a complicated procedure, which requires hard work and effort from them.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedures

The survey’s participants (N = 839) were educators employed in primary (n = 478) and secondary (n = 361) education, 75.7% female, with ages varied from 22 to 68 years old (median = 45, mean = 43.96, SD = 10.62). In most cases, their school region was located in a city (city = 74%, town = 11.8% country = 14.2%), while the teaching experience of participating teachers varied from 1 to 39 years (median = 15, mean = 15.77, SD = 10.29) and half of them (57.7%) held a postgraduate degree. The implemented self-completion questionnaire (Teacher Irrational Belief Scale–Greek, TIBS-G) was uploaded on a web-based form via LimeSurvey Forms. Participants were contacted via email to their school email addresses, and responded anonymously at their own time and place. No participants’ responses were excluded from the analyses conducted. Before completion, an accompanying cover letter explained the confidentiality and purpose of the study, the potential objectives, and that the participation was voluntary. No financial incentives were provided for participating in the study.

2.2. Instrument

The Greek version, TIBS-G, is an adapted scale from the original TIBS instrument [4], an instrument designed for measuring teachers’ irrational beliefs. The original questionnaire was translated to Greek by the authors followed by an intensive back translation procedure by six bilingual English-Greek speakers who did not know the original English text. The final stage of a collaborative and iterative translation [39] finalized TIBS-G, assuring the proper adaptation, and the questionnaire was used for data collection and the subsequent factor analyses.
For the initial TIBS four dimensions were proposed, namely: Self-downing (SD), Authoritarianism (A), Demands for Justice (DJ), and Low Frustration Tolerance (LT). The scale consists of 25 items classified as follows: Self-downing {1, 3, 5, 6, 8, 10, 22, 23}, Authoritarianism {2, 4, 7, 11, 12, 16, 17, 19}, Demands for Justice {13, 14, 18, 21, 25}, Low Frustration Tolerance {9, 15, 20, 24}. In responding to the TIBS-G, the participants chose their degree of agreement in a 7-point Likert scale (See Appendix A).
Besides the TIBS-G items, the questionnaire consisted of demographic variables, from which only gender was used in this study for the measurement invariance evaluation.

2.3. Analyses

Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) by means of Principal Axis Factoring (PAF) was implemented to determine the dimensionality of the TIBS-G, which is expected to conform the four-factor structure. Subsequently, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was carried out to test the goodness-of-fit of the measurement model for the theoretically assumed four-dimensional structure. The four-factor model was also compared with a one-factor solution. To evaluate model fit, multiple fit statistics were used: χ2, comparative fit index (CFI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). The accepted values are CFI ≥ 0.95, TLI ≥ 0.95 and RMSEA ≤ 0.05 which indicate a good fit [40]. Some scholars suggest that the values ≤ 0.06 for RMESA and CFI values ≥ 0.90 could also be acceptable, given that CFI values have been shown sensitive to lack of invariance [41,42]. Additionally, the internal consistency of the four scales was examined by calculating Cronbach’s alpha and McDonald’s omega coefficients.
After CFA, the measurement invariance was evaluated following a stepwise procedure, starting with configural invariance, which is the least restrictive model, and then proceeded with more restricted models. In each of the next evaluation steps, the model was compared to the previous one. In this process, every next model was of increasing constraints. The configurational invariance, known as pattern invariance, is used as the baseline model. The configurational invariance assures that the factorial structure is the same in the two genders, that is, both male and female share the same goodness of fit statistics, and the same items loads on the same factors. Next, the metric invariance is evaluated, which concerns the values of factor loadings in the two groups. If attaining metric invariance, then the construct has the same meaning to participants across sexes. To evaluate the metric invariance, the fit indexes of the metric model are compared to the corresponding configural model, using a χ2 difference test, where no significance suggests that the factor loadings are invariant across groups, and it advocates that gender-groups share factor variances and covariances. However, it does not account for the comparisons of group means. Then, the scalar invariance is evaluated, which, based on metric invariance, tests if the item intercepts are equivalent across the two groups. The item intercepts are considered the starting point/value of the scale, thus, the participants should share the same value if they have the same values on the latent construct. The evaluation of scalar invariance is made by comparing the fit indexes of the scalar model with the corresponding fit of the metric model. If this evaluation resulted in non-invariance of intercepts, then measurement bias might exist, which is suggestive of possible differentiated effects, due to individual differences (between men and women). In other cases, it could be attributed to social or even cultural differences. The last evaluation step is testing for strict invariance, which concerns the invariance of factor variances. It includes the overall error in the prediction of the construct, and each variable’s error terms, that is the strict invariance evaluates if the residual errors are equivalent across the two groups.
It is relevant to recall at this point that since the χ2 difference test is used, in cases of large samples, even very small deviations lead to significant results. For this reason, some scholars have suggested that differences of ΔCFI < 0.01 and ΔRMSEA < 0.015 should be ignored [41,43].

3. Results

3.1. Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA)

In the first step, EFA was applied to the empirical data (N = 839) using PAF with Oblique/Promax rotation, to reveal the underlying dimensionality of the TIBS-G scale. Bartlett’s test of sphericity (χ2 = 5126.09, p < 0.0001) and the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin index (0.860) indicated adequate variance for factor analysis. The number of factors was decided taking into consideration the Kaiser’s Criterion (eigenvalue greater than 1), the scree plot and the parallel analysis carried out with simulated data. Figure 1 shows clearly that the factorial structure is four-dimensional.
Note, the first EFA run also showed that four items (SD22, SD23, A2, A19) did not conform to the initially proposed structure demonstrating multiple or low factor loadings (<0.40). These were excluded from the factor model. Table 1 shows the four-dimensional structure, the factor loadings, and the uniqueness of the items, i.e., the variance that is not shared with other variables (the unique variance).
The four factors correspond to Self-downing (SD), Authoritarianism (A), Demands for Justice (DJ) and Low Frustration Tolerance (LT), with eigenvalues 2.54, 2.26, 2.01 and 1.74, respectively, while the corresponding portions of variance explained were 12.1%, 10.87%, 9.60%, and 8.30%, respectively.

3.2. Reliability Analysis

Reliability measures of the four TIBS-G’s factors were computed using Cronbach’s Alpha (α) and McDonald’s omega (ω) as following: Self-downing (α = 0.777/ω = 0.780), Authoritarianism (α = 0.779/ω = 0.782), Demands for Justice (α = 0.754/ω = 0.751), and Low Frustration Tolerance (α = 0.695/ω = 0.704). The overall internal reliability of the TIBS is α = 0.848/ω = 0.848. These reliability indices suggest that the present measurements with the TIBS-G sub-scales have a satisfactory degree of internal consistency.

3.3. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA)–The Measurement Model

Subsequently, CFA was applied to TIBS-G for providing the measurement factor model of TIB. CFA is used to verify the factor structure of a set of observed variables, and it allows testing the hypothesis that a relationship between observed variables and their underlying latent constructs exists. CFA results for the single-factor model are χ2 = 1815.20, df = 275, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.891, RMSEA = 0.082, SRMR = 0.088, NFI = 0.874, however, the proposed four-dimensional model fitted satisfactorily to the empirical data possessing the following fit measure indices: χ2 = 579.98, df = 183, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.960; TLI = 0.956; RMSEA = 0.051; 90% CI of RMSEA = [0.046; 0.056]; SRMR = 0.058; NFI = 0.942; GFI = 0.974. Comparison of the two models by means of a χ2 test revealed that the four-factor model was substantially improved over the single-factor model (Δχ2 = 1235.22, df = 92, p < 0.001). Thus, the hypothesis of the unidimensional structure of TIBS-G for the present data set was rejected. Moreover, no problems with possible model misspecifications were identified when inspecting the standardized residual covariances matrix, since the absolute values of most standardized covariances of residuals were found to be less than two [44]. All calculations were performed using JASP software. Table 2 shows the CFA measurement model and factors, estimates of factor loadings, standards errors, and statistical significance.
Table 3 shows the correlation matrix of the four dimensions, along with the means and the standard deviations of each factor. Self-downing correlated with Authoritarianism (r = 0.638, p < 0.001) with Demands for Justice (r = 0.352, p < 0.001) and with Low Frustration Tolerance (r = 0.535, p < 0.001). Authoritarianism correlates with Demands for Justice (r = 0.396, p < 0.001) and with Low Frustration Tolerance (r = 0.533, p < 0.001), while Demands for Justice is correlated with Low Frustration Tolerance (r = 0.496, p < 0.001).

3.4. Measurement Invariance for Gender

Subsequently, the measurement invariance for gender was tested following the procedure described in a previous section. A multi-group CFA model was fitted for each group separately, without any equality constraints. This model tests whether the same factorial structure holds across gender-groups. To evaluate the configural model for each group, the following guidelines for the model fit indices were used: CFI > 0.95, TLI > 0.95, and RMSEA < 0.06 [45]. With sufficient model fit for configural invariance, the analysis proceeds to metric invariance evaluation.
In the metric invariance model, the factor loadings are assumed to be equal across groups, but the intercepts are allowed to vary between gender-groups. To test metric invariance, a comparison of the configural model against the metric model is made using a chi-square difference (Δχ2) test. The p-value is statistically significant, thus there is a lack of metric invariance. Even though, due to the non-invariance evidence, the procedure could stop here, all next tests were realized and presented for demonstration purposes. The scalar invariance model test is significant, thus, factor loadings and/or intercepts are different in males and females [46]. The Strict invariance model test suggests that the residual variances are fixed across groups. Summarizing, in the overall analysis (Table 4), given that the metric and scalar invariance tests are statistically significant, measurement invariance does not hold for gender.
Subsequently, CFA was applied to data separately for the two sexes. The fit indexes of the four-dimensional model for men is {χ2 = 200.293, df = 183, p > 0.05; CFI = 0.992; TLI = 0.990; RMSEA = 0.022; 90% CI of RMSEA = [0.000; 0.039]; SRMR = 0.065; NNFI = 0.990; GFI = 0.995}, while the fit indexes of the four-dimensional model for women is {χ2 = 493.57, df = 183, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.961; TLI = 0.955; RMSEA = 0.052; 90% CI of RMSEA = [0.049; 0.057]; SRMR = 0.058; NNFI = 0.955; GFI = 0.996}. Table 5 and Table 6 show the CFA measurement models with factors, estimates of factor loadings, standards errors, and statistical significance, for men and women, respectively.

4. Discussion

Behavioral sciences have a strong interest in peoples’ thoughts, attitudes, and beliefs, among which, not surprisingly, irrational beliefs or dysfunctional myths have a great impact on human actions, and they are omnipresent in all societies and cultures. Within educational context, research has shown that irrational beliefs can hinder teachers’ performance, being associated with negative emotions. Teachers possessing those dysfunctional thoughts usually face difficulties understanding students’ feelings and furthermore responding to their delinquent behaviors, while in general, they confront classroom management troubles. Understanding these problems, theoreticians and researchers need to diagnose irrational beliefs, so they can help practitioners to reduce their effects and enhance performance and work interpersonal relationships.
Schools are complex environments, where relationships, co-actions and synergies require a consensus for what is needed to be accomplished and a common understanding about the educational processes. The network of interactions between individual characteristics, which involve the influence of rational and irrational beliefs, is vital behavioral determinants [46], and the investigation of the latter has been a special interest not only for psychiatry, but also for school psychology, rapidly extending to the educational field [2,47,48]. Irrationality becomes a core issue in the contemporary ever-changing society which undergoes rapid changes. Teachers as professionals must support new innovative and demanding situations, going through an essential adaptation, but also via bureaucratic procedures which might be stressful and ineffective. In such sudden changes, given emotional states and the bounded rationality [49], dysfunctional myths are more likely to operate as a defense mechanism against the ensuing uncertainty. TIBS appears a useful tool in research for identifying, understanding, preventing and/or treating those irrational beliefs.
The present study evaluated the psychometric properties of the Greek version of Teachers’ Irrational Belief Scale (TIBS-G). Exploratory and confirmatory procedures revealed the four-factor structure and demonstrated its validity, showing that Self-downing (SD), Authoritarianism (A), Demands for Justice (DJ), and Low Frustration Tolerance (LT) were the four dimensions as initially it has theoretically proposed [4]. In addition, analysis of measurement invariance showed that there are differences between genders and this finding is useful in both measurement procedures and the theoretical consideration. This paper contributes to the measurement issues in the literature of irrational beliefs, by exploring the factorial structure of the TIBS-G, and, thus, further supporting the theoretical framework.
It is imperative to mention at this point that the irrational beliefs or dysfunctional myths are not at all times “dysfunctional”, in the sense that they do not always have negative consequences. As cognitive resources, they are involved in decision making processes and under bounded rationality [49] and play a determinant role. Research has shown that certain types of dysfunctional myths help some people to reach decision easier, while others staying in constant indecisiveness. This unanticipated and peculiar role was explained within complexity theory framework [3,50], where the self-regulation in decision-making is viewed as a nonlinear dynamical process and the ensued taking on as emergent, self-transcending constructions [51], the study of which might illuminate the pathways to rational and irrational human adoptions.
There are of course some limitations of the study which originate from the opportunity sampling procedure. Moreover, measurement invariance, which here is restricted to gender, should be expanded to other categories such as the level of education or teaching specialties. Given that this is the first endeavor appealing to the Greek population, replications of the study should follow to establish firm conclusions.
Conclusively, the present research can support new investigations on human behavior by covering the fundamental requirement in research methodology, that is, the validation of the TIBS-G,. Future inquiries are planned to explore the role of IB within the school context, related to a number of teachers’ individual differences, such as self-efficacy, burnout constructs, creativity, and innovative behaviors.

Author Contributions

The three authors have equally contributed to the present research, sharing the work in each step and the corresponding responsibility. Conceptualization, A.G., J.V. and D.S.; methodology, A.G., J.V. and D.S.; software, A.G., J.V. and D.S.; validation, X A.G., J.V. and D.S.; formal analysis, A.G., J.V. and D.S.; investigation, A.G., J.V. and D.S.; resources, A.G., J.V. and D.S.; data curation, A.G., J.V. and D.S.; writing—original draft preparation, A.G., J.V. and D.S.; visualization, A.G., J.V. and D.S.; supervision, D.S.; project administration, D.S.; funding acquisition, A.G., J.V. and D.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, following the protocol of the Ethics and deontology Committee of Aristotle University.

Informed Consent Statement

The research involved adults participating anonymously in an online survey. Before completion, an accompanying cover letter explained the confidentiality and purpose of the study, the potential objectives, and the voluntary participation. No financial incentives were provided for participating in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

The TIBS-G Instrument
SD1. To make mistakes or perform poorly as a teacher is for me one of the worst things in the world.
SD3. I think I’m really hopeless when I haven’t gotten all my work done on time.
SD5. I really should be able to solve all of my students’ problems perfectly.
SD6. I can’t stand it when I put all my energy in a student or class and get no results.
SD8. I can’t stand being criticized or thought badly of when I haven’t finished something or done it properly.
SD10. I think I’m really inadequate when I don’t get approval or respect for what I do.
A4. Before I can really change how I feel about a student or class, they must change.
A7. I can’t stand it when students misbehave.
A11. Students should always be respectful, considerate and behave well.
A12. As a teacher, I must have the power to make my students do what I want.
A16. Students who constantly misbehave are horrible and should be severely punished.
A17. Students can really upset me when they do the wrong things or misbehave.
DJ13. I can’t stand it when I am not consulted about a decision, which affects my teaching.
DJ14. Without good administrator-teacher communication and support, schools are the very worst and terrible places to work.
DJ18. One thing I find totally bad is the lack of communication between teachers and central administration.
DJ21. Teachers should be consulted about decisions, which affect their teaching, and it’s really unfair when they are not.
DJ25. Schools really should attend more to teachers’ problems, and it is totally unfair when they don’t.
LT9. I find it too hard to balance my work and home demands.
LT15. School are really lousy places to work because they give teachers too much work and not enough time to do it.
LT20. I shouldn’t have to work so hard.
LT24. It’s really bad to have to put so many hours both inside and outside the classroom.

References

  1. Bermejo-Toro, L.; Prieto-Ursúa, M. Teachers’ irrational beliefs and their relationship to distress in the profession. Psychol. Spain 2006, 10, 88–96. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bernard, M.E. Teacher Beliefs and Stress. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 2016, 34, 209–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Stamovlasis, D.; Vaiopoulou, J. The role of dysfunctional myths in a decision-making process under bounded rationality: A complex dynamical systems perspective. Nonlinear Dyn. Psychol. Life Sci. 2017, 21, 267–288. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bernard, M.E. Teacher irrationality and teacher stress. In Proceedings of the 24th International Congress of Psychology, Sydney, Australia, 28 August–2 September 1988. [Google Scholar]
  5. Ellis, A. Rational Psychotherapy. TACD J. 1958, 59, 35–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. DiGiuseppe, R.; Doyle, K.A.; Dryden, W.; Backx, W. A Practitioner’s Guide to Rational-Emotive Behavior Therapy, 3rd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  7. Montgomery, G.H.; David, D.; Dilorenzo, T.A.; Schnur, J.B. Response expectancies and irrational beliefs predict exam-related distress. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 2007, 25, 17–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. David, D. Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT): The view of a cognitive psychologist. In Theoretical Developments in REBT.; Dryden, W., Ed.; Brunner/Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 130–159. [Google Scholar]
  9. Vîslǎ, A.; Cristea, I.A.; Szentagotai, A.; David, D. Core beliefs, automatic thoughts and response expectancies in predicting public speaking anxiety. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2013, 55, 856–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. David, D.; DiGiuseppe, R. Social and Cultural Aspects of Rational and Irrational Beliefs: A Brief Reconceptualization. In Rational and Irrational Beliefs: Research, Theory, and Clinical Practice; David, D., Lynn, S.J., Ellis, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010; pp. 49–61. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bernard, M.E.; Pires, D. Emotional Resilience in Children and Adolescence: Implications for Rational-Emotive Behavior Therapy. In Rational Emotive Behavioral Approaches to the Problems of Childhood; Ellis, A., Bernard, M.E., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 156–174. [Google Scholar]
  12. Turner, M.; Bennett, R. Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy in Sport and Exercise; Routledge: Abingdon-on-Thames, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  13. Szentagotai, A.; Jones, J. The Behavioral Consequences of Irrational Beliefs. In Rational and Irrational Beliefs: Research, Theory, and Clinical Practice; David, D., Lynn, S.J., Ellis, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010; pp. 75–97. [Google Scholar]
  14. Ellis, A. Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy; Lyle Stuart: New York, NY, USA, 1962. [Google Scholar]
  15. David, D.; Montgomery, G.H.; Macavei, B.; Bovbjerg, D.H. An Empirical Investigation of Albert Ellis’s Binary Model of Distress. J. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 61, 499–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. David, D.; Schnur, J.B.; Belloiu, A. Another search for the “hot” cognitions: Appraisal, irrational beliefs, attributions, and their relation to emotion. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 2002, 20, 93–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. MacInnes, D. The theories underpinning rational emotive behaviour therapy: Where’s the supportive evidence? Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 2004, 41, 685–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. DiGiuseppe, R. The nature of irrational and rational beliefs: Progress in rational emotive behavior theory. J. Ration. Cogn. Ther. 1996, 14, 5–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Martin, R.C.; Dahlen, E.R. Irrational beliefs and the experience and expression of anger. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 2004, 22, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dryden, W. “The cream cake made me eat it”: An introduction to the ABC theory of REBT. In Rational Emotive Behaviour Therapy: Theoretical Developments; Dryden, W., Ed.; Brunner Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2003; p. 121. [Google Scholar]
  21. Dryden, W. Fundamentals of Rational Emotive Behaviour Therapy; Whurr Publishers Ltd.: London, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  22. Watson, P.J.; Sherbak, J.; Morris, R.J. Irrational beliefs, individualism-collectivism, and adjustment. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1998, 24, 173–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Jones, J.; Trower, P. Irrational and evaluative beliefs in individuals with anger disorders. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 2004, 22, 153–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Ellis, A.; Knauss, W.J. Overcoming Procrastination; Institute for Rational Living: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  25. Eisenberger, R. Learned industriousness. Psychol. Rev. 1992, 99, 248–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. David, D.; Cramer, D. Rational and Irrational Beliefs in Human Feelings and Psychophysiology. In Rational and Irrational Beliefs: Research, Theory, and Clinical Practice; David, D., Lynn, S.J., Ellis, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010; pp. 99–112. [Google Scholar]
  27. Ellis, A. The Myth of Self-Esteem: How Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy Can Change Your Life Forever; Prometheus Books: Amherst, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  28. Ellis, A. The revised ABC’s of rational-emotive therapy (RET). J. Ration. Cogn. Ther. 1991, 9, 139–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ellis, A.; David, D.; Lynn, S.J. Rational and irrational beliefs: A historical and conceptual perspective. In Rational and Irrational Beliefs: Research, Theory, and Clinical Practice; David, D., Lynn, S.J., Ellis, A., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010; pp. 3–22. ISBN 9780195182231. [Google Scholar]
  30. Mitchell, L.K.; Krumboltz, J.D. Social learning approach to career decision making: Krumboltz’s theory. In Career Choice and Development; Brown, D., Brooks, L., Eds.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1990; pp. 145–196. [Google Scholar]
  31. Bernard, M.E.; Joyce, M.R.; Rosewarne, P. Helping teachers cope with stress: A rational-emotive approach. In Rational-Emotive Approaches to the Problems of Childhood; Ellis, A., Bernard, M.E., Eds.; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  32. Bullock, D. Learner self-assessment: An investigation into teachers’ beliefs. ELT J. 2011, 65, 114–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Warren, J.M.; Hale, R.W. The Influence of Efficacy Beliefs on Teacher Performance and Student Success: Implications for Student Support Services. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 2016, 34, 187–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Klassen, R.M.; Chiu, M.M. Effects on Teachers’ Self-Efficacy and Job Satisfaction: Teacher Gender, Years of Experience, and Job Stress. J. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 102, 741–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Khaledian, M.; Babaee, H.; Amani, M. The relationship of psychological hardiness with irrational beliefs, emotional intelligence and work holism. World Sci. News 2016, 28, 86–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Huk, O.; Terjesen, M.D.; Cherkasova, L. Predicting teacher burnout as a function of school characteristics and irrational beliefs. Psychol. Sch. 2019, 56, 792–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Bora, C.; Bernard, M.E.; Trip, S.; Decsei-Radu, A.; Chereji, S. Teacher irrational belief scale—Preliminary norms for Romanian population. J. Cogn. Behav. Psychother. 2009, 9, 211–220. [Google Scholar]
  38. Popov, S.; Popov, B.; Damjanović, R. The role of stressors at work and irrational beliefs in the prediction of teachers’ stress. Primenj. Psihol. 2015, 8, 5–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Douglas, S.; Craig, S.S. Collaborative and Iterative Translation: An Alternative Approach to Instrument Translation. SSRN 2006, 15, 30–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Crayen, C.; Geiser, C.; Scheithauer, H.; Eid, M. Evaluating Interventions with multimethod data: A structural equation modeling approach. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2011, 18, 497–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Chen, F.F. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2007, 14, 464–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hair, J.F.; Anderson, R.E.; Tatham, R.L.; Black, W.C. Multivariate Data Analysis; Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 1998; ISBN 9780139305870/0139305874. [Google Scholar]
  43. Cheung, G.W.; Rensvold, R.B. Structural Equation Modeling: A Evaluating Goodness-of—Fit Indexes for Testing Measurement Invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2002, 9, 233–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Arbuckle, J.L. Amos 7.0 Users’ Guide; Amos Development Corporation: Spring House, PA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hu, L.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Van de Schoot, R.; Lugtig, P.; Hox, J. A checklist for testing measurement invariance. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2012, 9, 486–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Stein, D.; Grant, A.M. Disentangling the relationships among self-reflection, insight, and subjective well-being: The role of dysfunctional attitudes and core self-evaluations. J. Psychol. Interdiscip. Appl. 2014, 148, 505–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Pham, M.T. Emotion and Rationality: A Critical Review and Interpretation of Empirical Evidence. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2007, 11, 155–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Simon, H.A. Reason in Human Affairs; Stanford University Press: Stanford, CA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  50. Vaiopoulou, J.; Papavassiliou-Alexiou, I.; Stamovlasis, D. Career decision-making difficulties and decision statuses among Greek student teachers. Hell. J. Psychol. 2019, 16, 74–94. [Google Scholar]
  51. Goldstein, J. Emergence, self-transcendence, and education. In Complex Dynamical Systems in Education: Concepts, Methods and Applications; Koopmans, M., Stamovlasis, D., Eds.; Springer Academic Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 30–57. ISBN 9783319275772. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Scree plot with parallel analysis demonstrating the dimensionality of four factors.
Figure 1. Scree plot with parallel analysis demonstrating the dimensionality of four factors.
Behavsci 11 00160 g001
Table 1. Factor loadings of four-dimensional structure.
Table 1. Factor loadings of four-dimensional structure.
Factor 1Factor 2Factor 3Factor 4
Self-Downing AuthoritarianismDemands for JusticeLow Frustration ToleranceUniqueness
SD10.568 0.735
SD30.692 0.570
SD50.492 0.757
SD60.507 0.514
SD80.701 0.477
SD100.576 0.542
A4 0.490 0.670
A7 0.519 0.483
A11 0.577 0.677
A12 0.577 0.647
A16 0.760 0.492
A17 0.493 0.532
DJ13 0.512 0.608
DJ14 0.601 0.599
DJ18 0.711 0.528
DJ21 0.669 0.575
DJ25 0.514 0.675
LT9 0.4020.699
LT15 0.5620.610
LT20 0.6680.546
LT24 0.6970.510
Note. Applied rotation method is promax. Self-downing (SD), Authoritarianism (A), Demands for Justice (DJ), and Low Frustration Tolerance (LT).
Table 2. CFA measurement model (4-factor): Factors, estimates of factor loadings, standards errors, and statistical significance.
Table 2. CFA measurement model (4-factor): Factors, estimates of factor loadings, standards errors, and statistical significance.
95% Confidence Interval
FactorIndicatorSymbolEstimateStd. Errorz-Value pLowerUpperStd. Est. (All)
SDSD1λ110.6900.03122.33<0.0010.6290.7500.421
SD3λ120.8620.03029.12<0.0010.8040.9200.571
SD5λ130.6920.03122.65<0.0010.6320.7520.426
SD6λ141.2440.03536.01<0.0011.1761.3110.756
SD8λ151.2020.03534.69<0.0011.1341.2700.706
SD10λ161.2270.03634.28<0.0011.1571.2970.702
AA4λ210.5810.02225.98<0.0010.5370.6240.524
A7λ221.2460.03436.56<0.0011.1791.3120.775
A11λ230.6950.03023.53<0.0010.6370.7520.440
A12λ240.9090.03228.29<0.0010.8460.9720.554
A16λ250.9110.03129.19<0.0010.8500.9720.580
A17λ261.1050.03135.76<0.0011.0441.1660.740
DJDJ13λ311.2480.04428.68<0.0011.1631.3340.736
DJ14λ320.9420.03626.37<0.0010.8721.0120.648
DJ18λ330.7950.03324.09<0.0010.7310.8600.576
DJ21λ340.6540.03022.06<0.0010.5960.7130.529
DJ25λ350.7370.03322.25<0.0010.6720.8020.536
LTLT9λ410.8370.03623.26<0.0010.7670.9080.502
LT15λ421.0330.03926.63<0.0010.9571.1090.629
LT20λ431.0700.04026.78<0.0010.9921.1490.632
LT24λ441.0930.04027.43<0.0011.0151.1710.660
Note: Self-downing (SD), Authoritarianism (A), Demands for Justice (DJ), and Low Frustration Tolerance (LT).
Table 3. Factor correlation matrix, means, standard deviations, and internal consistency measures (Alpha, α and Omega, ω).
Table 3. Factor correlation matrix, means, standard deviations, and internal consistency measures (Alpha, α and Omega, ω).
SDADJLT
Self-downing (SD)1.000
Authoririanism (A)0.638 ***1.000
Demands for Justice (DJ)0.352 ***0.396 ***1.000
Low Frustration
Tolerance (LT)
0.535 ***0.533 ***0.496 ***1.000
Mean
Standard Deviation
3.60
(1.13)
3.27
(1.04)
5.45
(1.01)
3.10
(1.20)
Alpha, α0.7770.7790.7540.695
Omega, ω0.7800.7820.7510.704
Note: *** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Measurement Invariance for Gender.
Table 4. Measurement Invariance for Gender.
Invariance Modelχ2dfCFITLTRMSEASRMR Δχ2 Δdfp-Value
00
Configural693,873620.9670.9610.0470.06693,9362
Metric750,8833790.9630.9590.0480.06257,0117<0.001
scalar785,4914000.9610.9590.0480.06434,6121<0.05
Strict815,8084210.9600.9610.0470.06530,3221<0.10
Table 5. CFA measurement model for men: Factors, estimates of factor loadings, standards errors, and statistical significance.
Table 5. CFA measurement model for men: Factors, estimates of factor loadings, standards errors, and statistical significance.
Factor Loadings
95% Confidence Interval
FactorIndicatorSymbolEstimateStd. Errorz-ValuepLowerUpper
SDSD1λ110.5620.0638.884<0.0010.4380.686
SD3λ120.8230.05614.619<0.0010.7130.933
SD5λ130.5050.0578.837<0.0010.3930.617
SD6λ141.1760.06916.923<0.0011.0401.312
SD8λ151.0010.06615.165<0.0010.8721.131
SD10λ161.0840.06815.839<0.0010.9501.219
AA4λ210.7870.05813.468<0.0010.6720.901
A7λ221.1670.06817.080<0.0011.0331.300
A11λ230.6190.05610.998<0.0010.5090.729
A12λ240.9970.07413.530<0.0010.8531.141
A16λ250.9640.06913.987<0.0010.8291.099
A17λ260.9910.06315.678<0.0010.8671.115
DJDJ13λ311.4460.10014.415<0.0011.2501.643
DJ14λ320.8490.07012.177<0.0010.7130.986
DJ18λ330.6970.06410.864<0.0010.5710.823
DJ21λ340.6050.0639.624<0.0010.4820.729
DJ25λ350.6820.06610.396<0.0010.5540.811
LTLT9λ410.9870.07513.138<0.0010.8401.135
LT15λ420.8580.07511.469<0.0010.7121.005
LT20λ431.1180.08612.978<0.0010.9491.287
LT24λ441.1350.08613.222<0.0010.9671.303
Note: Self-downing (SD), Authoritarianism (A), Demands for Justice (DJ), and Low Frustration Tolerance (LT).
Table 6. CFA measurement model for women: Factors, estimates of factor loadings, standards errors, and statistical significance.
Table 6. CFA measurement model for women: Factors, estimates of factor loadings, standards errors, and statistical significance.
Factor Loadings
95% Confidence Interval
FactorIndicatorSymbolEstimateStd. Errorz-ValuepLowerUpper
SDSD1λ110.7310.03520.989<0.0010.6630.800
SD3λ120.8700.03425.505<0.0010.8030.937
SD5λ130.7510.03521.314<0.0010.6820.820
SD6λ141.2570.03932.098<0.0011.1811.334
SD8λ151.2630.04031.588<0.0011.1851.341
SD10λ161.2820.04131.163<0.0011.2011.362
AA4λ210.5130.02222.848<0.0010.4690.556
A7λ221.2570.03832.656<0.0011.1811.332
A11λ230.7100.03421.101<0.0010.6440.776
A12λ240.8750.03525.207<0.0010.8070.943
A16λ250.8830.03426.205<0.0010.8170.949
A17λ261.1240.03532.386<0.0011.0561.192
DJDJ13λ311.1940.04825.002<0.0011.1011.288
DJ14λ320.9730.04123.605<0.0010.8921.054
DJ18λ330.8260.03821.683<0.0010.7510.901
DJ21λ340.6590.03319.877<0.0010.5940.724
DJ25λ350.7550.03819.910<0.0010.6810.829
LTLT9λ410.7960.04119.630<0.0010.7160.875
LT15λ421.0720.04524.017<0.0010.9841.159
LT20λ431.0570.04523.752<0.0010.9701.145
LT24λ441.0790.04424.295<0.0010.9921.166
Note: Self-downing (SD), Authoritarianism (A), Demands for Justice (DJ), and Low Frustration Tolerance (LT).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gkontelos, A.; Vaiopoulou, J.; Stamovlasis, D. Teachers’ Irrational Belief Scale: Psychometric Properties of the Greek Version and Measurement Invariance across Genders. Behav. Sci. 2021, 11, 160. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11110160

AMA Style

Gkontelos A, Vaiopoulou J, Stamovlasis D. Teachers’ Irrational Belief Scale: Psychometric Properties of the Greek Version and Measurement Invariance across Genders. Behavioral Sciences. 2021; 11(11):160. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11110160

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gkontelos, Angelos, Julie Vaiopoulou, and Dimitrios Stamovlasis. 2021. "Teachers’ Irrational Belief Scale: Psychometric Properties of the Greek Version and Measurement Invariance across Genders" Behavioral Sciences 11, no. 11: 160. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs11110160

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop