Next Article in Journal
Ethical and Animal Welfare Considerations in Relation to Species Selection for Animal Experimentation
Previous Article in Journal
Sex Differences in Physiological Acclimatization after Transfer in Wistar Rats
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Transfer from Breeding to Research Facility on the Welfare of Rats

1
Department of Animals in Science & Society, Division of Animal Welfare & Laboratory Animal Science, Veterinary Faculty, Utrecht University, Utrecht, 3584 CM, The Netherlands
2
Harlan Laboratories B.V., P.O. Box 553, Venray, 5800 AN, The Netherlands
3
Department of Human and Animal Physiology, P.O. Box 338, Wageningen University and Research Centre, Wageningen, 6700 AH, The Netherlands
4
Department of Occupational Health, Safety and Environment, Free University, Amsterdam, 1081 HV, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2014, 4(4), 712-728; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4040712
Submission received: 27 June 2014 / Revised: 18 November 2014 / Accepted: 19 November 2014 / Published: 3 December 2014

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

This study measured effects of transfer on body temperature, stress hormone levels, body weight, behavior and water and food intake in rats. Environmental temperature strongly affected body temperature of rats and needs to be controlled. Male rats need to habituate for at least one week, females for two weeks after transfer.

Abstract

Transfer from the breeding facility to a research facility is a stressful event for laboratory animals. Heat stress has been reported to constitute one of the major concerns during transport of animals. This study measured ambient and body temperature, corticosterone and glucose levels, body weight, behavior and water and food intake before, during and after transfer in Wistar rats. Decreased body weight, water and food intake were observed on the day of transfer in rats. Environmental temperature strongly affected body temperature of rats and needs to be controlled. Male rats need to habituate for at least one week, females for two weeks after transfer.

1. Introduction

The transfer of laboratory animals from the breeding facility to research facilities embraces a variety of challenges for the animals, such as physiological and social stress due to handling, with new sounds and smells, fluctuations in temperature and humidity, disruption of light-dark cycle, separation from cage mates, and new caretakers [1,2]. Such stressors lead to activation of the hypophysal-pituitary-adrenal (HPA)-axis, promoting secretion of corticosterone and production of glucose (for review see [3]). Several studies on transportation of rodents report an increase in plasma corticosterone [2,4,5,6,7] and plasma glucose levels [8], as well as disrupted behavior [2,6,9], though occasionally a decrease in corticosterone and glucose levels in transported rats have been found [10]. Also, decreased heart rate, blood pressure, and body weight have been related to the process of transportation in rodents [2,9,11] and a decrease in glucose levels was detected in rabbits transported to the slaughter house [12].
Heat stress has been reported to constitute the most frequently occurring cause of death in transported animals and thus might be considered as one of the major stressors during transfer [13]. A previous study on transport in rats showed highly variable temperatures in transportation boxes [14]. During a pilot study with mice at Harlan Laboratories BV (The Netherlands), the peak temperature inside the transportation box was found to exceed 30 °C [15]. Syversen et al. (2008) for example found fluctuations in temperature of 11 °C during air transfer in more than 60% of shipments [16]. It is known that stress-induced-hyperthermia and ambient temperature (Ta) affect body temperature (Tb) [17,18,19,20,21,22]. Results of studies measuring Tb before and after transfer, however, are inconsistent: Capdevilla et al. (2007) [11] found no effect of transfer of rats on Tb, while Dallman et al. (2006) [23] observed an increase in Tb of rats when moving a cage within the same room or to another room, respectively. Stemkens-Stevens et al. (2009) transported guinea pigs for a shorter or longer period, respectively, which resulted in a decreased Tb in both conditions [9]. Changes in Tb have been shown to affect physiological parameters [24,25,26], metabolism and behavior [19], and plasma corticosterone and glucose [27].
In laboratory animal vivaria either a constant temperature within the rat’s thermal neutral zone is used or suitable cage enrichment is supplied in order to give the animals the opportunity of self-regulating their environmental temperature. Neither a constant temperature, nor cage enrichment is present during transfer. It is to be expected that fluctuations in Ta during transfer will modulate Tb in laboratory animals during and after transfer, and that transfer stress in combination with ambient temperature fluctuations may result in either hypo- or hyperthermia in transported animals. Both short termed modulation in ambient temperature and potential hyperthermia may constitute a serious challenge for the adaptive capacities for animals that are being transferred from a breeder to a research facility. Before transfer, animals are housed in stable environmental conditions, which are optimized for temperature, humidity, and air composition. During transfer, animals can experience temperatures that fluctuate and deviate from the temperatures they are used to, and after transfer animals need to adapt to new standard conditions. In this study, we measured Ta and Tb and their interaction before, during, and after transfer to elucidate the animals’ responses to such temperature challenges, which no other study has measured so far. How temperature affects the animal during and after transfer was measured biochemically, by plasma corticosterone and glucose.
The transfer of research animals withholds a multitude of procedures, which results in a multifactorial stressor that can hardly be disentangled. Therefore, the transfer procedure as referred to in our studies embraces weighing and packing of the animals, multiple movements of the transportation boxes between buildings/areas (barrier-holding area-logistic center–research facility-in house) and includes the change of environment and caretakers.
The present study extends on previous findings on the effect of transfer procedures in male and female rats by now relating effects to the animals’ core temperature, to body weight, body temperature, food and water intake, plasma-corticosterone and -glucose (indicating hormonal stress response) and home cage behavior [14].

2. Materials and Methods

Experimental procedures were approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of Utrecht University.

2.1. Animals and Housing

Immediately after weaning 48 male (aged 3 weeks, weighing 38.5 ± 2.5 g) and 48 female (aged 3 weeks, weighing 37.8 ± 2.5 g) Wistar rats (HsdCpb:WU, Harlan Laboratories BV, The Netherlands) were moved in house from the animal breeding room to the animal room of the Surgical Unit of Harlan NL. All rats were color coded on their backs with spray for individual recognition, which was repeated weekly after weighing. Rats were housed in groups of three in translucent type IV Makrolon cages (Floor area 1815 cm2) with bedding (wood shavings), an opaque black polyethylene tube, and tissues. The rats had free access to processed water (acidified (pH 5.8–6.4), chlorinated (6–8 ppm), softened and filtered (0.02 microns)) and food (Harlan Teklad 2018S, irradiated Global 18% protein rodent diet, Harlan, Madison, WI, USA). The rats were kept under a 12 h:12 h light regime, lights on at 6.00 am, in a temperature (22–24.5 °C) and humidity (50%) controlled vivarium. Group compositions of the rats remained constant during housing and during and after transfer.

2.2. Experimental Setup

Three days after weaning, a temperature logger was implanted in one rat per cage to record core body temperature according to a randomized schedule. After a recovery period of 14 days, baseline measurements started. Four weeks after surgery, 48 rats (24 male and 24 female, eight cages per sex) were randomly selected and transferred to a new facility at the Utrecht University, the remaining 48 rats stayed at the breeding facility as a non-transferred control group. Housing conditions (temperature and humidity settings) were similar at both facilities (Harlan Laboratories BV and the Central Laboratory Animal Research Facility (CLARF) of the Utrecht University and Medical Centre).
The first week after surgery and the first week after transfer, body weight (BW) was registered daily in all animals. The remaining time all animals were weighed three times a week. Every week a blood sample was taken by tail vein incision as described in Paragraph 2.6.

2.3. Implantation Temperature Logger

Before implantation, temperature loggers (Thermochron iButton, DS1921L-F50, Dallas Semiconductor, Sunnyvale, CA, USA: Ø 17 mm, height 6 mm, weight 4 g) were programmed to start measuring simultaneously two weeks after implantation and to record and store Tb every 30 min. The iButtons were coated with a thin layer of silicon (Elastostil E41, Wacker) for water protection and applicability for abdominal use, and equipped with non-absorbable ligatures (Silkam 5-0, B.Braun, Melsungen, Germany) for fixation to the abdominal wall. The abdomen of fully anesthetized rats (Isoflurane 2.5%, O2:NO2 (1:2)) was shaved and disinfected. An incision was made to open the skin and abdominal wall. iButtons were attached to the ventrolateral part of the abdominal wall and the abdomen and skin were closed with absorbable sutures (Vicryl 5-0, Ethicon, San Angelo, TX, USA). Ten minutes before and one day after surgery, meloxicam (0.55 mg/kg, Metacam 5 mg/mL, Boehringer Ingelheim Vetmedica, St. Joseph, IN, USA) was administered subcutaneously (sc) to provide analgesia. Also, the rats received an antibiotic (5.55 mg/kg, Enrofloxacin 50 mg/mL, Bayer AG, Barmen, Germany) 10 min before surgery.

2.4. Transfer

The day before transfer (DBT1), the rats in the selected cages were packed with three animals per standard transportation boxes (62 × 44 × 15 cm), 1 cage per box. Besides bedding and sufficient food pellets from the home cage, a water source (HydroGel, Clear H2O, Portland, ME, USA) was included in the transportation box. The remaining cages of male and female rats stayed in the original animal room at the breeding facility.
In 13 transportation boxes, a temperature logger (Keytag, Askey Dataloggers B.V., Leiderdorp, The Netherlands) protected by wire mesh was attached to the wall of the box. To the outside of one box an iButton was attached to measure outside-box temperature. Immediately after packing, transportation boxes were moved to a temperature controlled holding area at the breeding site for approximately 100 min at a temperature of 22–25 °C. At the end of the afternoon, approximately 5 h after packing, rats were transferred by truck to a climate controlled logistic center (Boxmeer, The Netherlands), approximately 20 min and 27 km away, where the rats stayed overnight for 13 h at a temperature of Ta 17 °C, until transfer early the next morning (DAT0). Transfer from Harlan NL to CLARF Utrecht took six hours, with several stops for unloading, in a climate controlled van preset at 15 °C. All transfer procedures were performed according to work instructions of the breeding facility, which are based on the ILAR guidelines by Baldwin et al. (2006) [13]. In the new animal facility rats were exposed to unfamiliar caretakers, sounds, new cages, and a humidity higher by about 15%. Room temperature, water, food, and cage enrichment were equal to control situation.

2.5. Water and Food Intake

Food and water intake were weighed daily per cage in transported and control groups during and four days after transfer.

2.6. Blood Collection and Analysis

Blood was collected by tail incision [28] (approximately 200 μL/sample) a week before, directly after and weekly after transfer (five times total), between 3 h and 4 h after lights were switched on. Sampling of blood was the day’s first procedure. Blood samples were taken from two rats per cage (one rat with and one without a temperature logger), according to a randomized schedule with equal distribution over the animals. After sampling, animals were rewarded with a small amount of birdseed. Plasma-corticosterone (CORT) levels were determined in duplicate with radio immunoassay (RIA) specified for rats and mice (ImmuChemTM double antibody 125I RIA kit, MP Biomedicals, LLC, OH, USA) according to the protocol of the supplier. Plasma-glucose (GLUC) was measured with the Glucose Assay Kit (BioChain, Hayward, WI, USA) following the o-Toluidine method [29] and read out using a microplate reader (DTX 880 Multimode Detector, Beckman Coulter Inc., Brea, CA, USA).

2.7. Behavioral Observations

All behavioral observations were scored live in the home cage in randomized cage order, directly after cage cleaning. Cleaning disturbs the animals and increases the level of activity in rats, which offers an opportunity to observe active behavior during the daylight period [30,31]. Observations were performed by continuous focal sampling for 5 min, 3–5 h after the start of the light period. One animal per cage was scored, according to a cage randomization schedule, with randomized equal distribution between animals with and without an iButton. Eighteen behavioral parameters were scored using the software The Observer (Noldus, Wageningen, The Netherlands) see table in Appendix. For analysis, the following three behavioral categories, each consisting of different behavioral elements, were used:
(1)
Activity/Locomotion (LOC): consisting of the behaviors explore, rear, walk, shake, scratch, scan, and hop/jump.
(2)
Social interactions (SI): consisting of social exploration, social groom, follow/chase, push and pin.
(3)
Self grooming (GRO), consisting of six behavioral elements which together compose the cephalo-caudal-sequence that animals perform in an undisturbed situation: fore-paw licking, face/nose wash, head wash, body wash/fur licking, hind leg licking, tail/genitals licking [32].

2.8. Statistical Analysis

In the present study the cage was the experimental and therefore statistical unit [33]. If applicable, parameters were averaged per cage. Body weight data was corrected for weight of the iButton. Data, residuals, and variance were tested for Gaussianity using a One-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Having a temperature logger was found not to have a significant effect and was therefore left out as factor. To detect overall effects and differences between before and after transfer for all parameters apart from behavior, a Repeated Measures ANOVA was executed. Data was considered significant when p < 0.050. An Independent-Samples t-test was used to compare transfer vs. control cages and male vs. female with each other per day for Tb, BW, food and water intake, CORT and GLUC. Alpha was corrected for multiple factors using the Dunn-Šidák correction and data was considered significant when p < 0.025. Pearson’s correlation coefficient was calculated for the correlation between Ta and Tb.
Behavior was analyzed using a Linear Mixed Model, obtaining the best fit using a model with a random intercept. Data was analyzed both split and un-split for treatment, with factors treatment, time/observation, sex and their interactions, all non-significant interaction terms were removed. Dependent variables were the duration percentages of total grooming (=sum of all self-grooming behaviors), total social interaction behavior and total active and locomotor behavior. The Q-Q plots of the residuals indicated that all behavioral variables were normally distributed.
Some plasma samples had CORT values below the detection limit (7.7 ng/mL). These data values were replaced by the value pre-half of the detection limit [34].
Calculations were performed using Microsoft Excel 2003. SPSS 16 (IBM SPSS Statistics, New York, NY, USA) was used for statistical analysis. All data is displayed as mean ± SD, both in tables and figures.

3. Results

3.1. Body Weight

There was a sex effect on bodyweight (M > F, F(1,28) = 540.0, p < 0.001) (Figure 1). Transfer resulted in a decrease in BW of transferred animals compared to the control animals. This difference was significant at day of transfer (male: t(14) = −2.63, p = 0.020, female: t(14) = −2.68, p = 0.018). One day after transfer this decrease was no longer significant. In the week before transfer, males increased in weight 7.1 ± 1.5 g/day. On the day of transfer they lost on average 6.2 ± 3.8 g (=3.2%), followed by an increased bodyweight gain of 11.8 ± 4.7 g the day after transfer. Females increased in weight the week before transfer 4.3 ± 1.4 g/day. On the day of transfer they lost on average 5.8 ± 2.6 g (=3.8%), followed by an increased bodyweight gain of 8.7 ± 3.8 g the day after transfer.
Figure 1. Body Weight (gram ± standard deviation (SD)) in male (squares) and female (circles), transferred (black symbols-solid line) and control (white symbols-dotted line) rats, 7 days before (DBT) until 23 days after (DAT) transfer (n = 8). * indicates significant different growth between transferred (TF) and control (CO) individuals.
Figure 1. Body Weight (gram ± standard deviation (SD)) in male (squares) and female (circles), transferred (black symbols-solid line) and control (white symbols-dotted line) rats, 7 days before (DBT) until 23 days after (DAT) transfer (n = 8). * indicates significant different growth between transferred (TF) and control (CO) individuals.
Animals 04 00712 g001

3.2. Water and Food Intake

Food and water intake are shown in Table 1. Over the total measured period there was a treatment effect (Transfer < Control: F(1,28) = 36.63, p < 0.001), a sex effect (M > F; F(1,28) = 69.59, p ≤ 0.001) and a time × treatment interaction (F(3,85) = 73.36, p ≤ 0.001) found for water intake. During transfer, a water source was added to the transportation box. Less of this water source was consumed by the transferred animals, both male and female, compared to water consumed by male and female control rats (male: t(14) = −9.47, p < 0.001, female: t(14) = −12.30, p < 0.001) during the same period. Compared to male transferred rats, male control rats showed a higher water consumption at day 3 (t(14) = −2.98), p = 0,01) and Day 4 after transfer (t(14) = −7.94, p < 0,001). Water intake of female transferred rats was significantly higher at day 1 (t(14) = 3.80, p ≤ 0.002) and in female control rats on Day 4 (t(14) = −4.03, p = 0,001). Over the total measured period, there was a time × sex interaction (F(4,106) = 2.50, p = 0.050), a time × treatment interaction (F(4,106) = 172.02, p < 0.001) and a time × sex × treatment interaction (F(4,106) = 3.61, p = 0.010) found for food intake. Food intake was reduced during transfer in both male and female transferred rats (male: t(14) = −11.83, p < 0.001, female Z = −3.37, p < 0.001). Food intake was increased in transferred female rats for three days after transfer and in transferred male rats for two days after transfer (p ≤ 0.010), compared to control animals.
Table 1. Water and food intake (mean ± SD grams) per cage of transferred and control rats (M = male and F = female). Day 0 is day of transfer. * Significantly different from control per t-test (p < 0.025) between transfer and control group, n = 8 cages of three animals.
Table 1. Water and food intake (mean ± SD grams) per cage of transferred and control rats (M = male and F = female). Day 0 is day of transfer. * Significantly different from control per t-test (p < 0.025) between transfer and control group, n = 8 cages of three animals.
Water (g)Day 0Day 1Day 2Day 3Day 4
M Transfer42.13 ± 11.10 *80.75 ± 18.4366.38 ± 10.1871.50 ± 10.54 *71.38 ± 5.07 *
M Control86.63 ± 7.3173.63 ± 6.6176.13 ± 7.6285.63 ± 8.4891.38 ± 5.01
F Transfer26.63 ± 6.07 *66.75 ± 5.63 *50.13 ± 3.8352.38 ± 4.6052.38 ± 5.45 *
F Control64.13 ± 6.1355.13 ± 6.5856.63 ± 6.8061.63 ± 10.4968.38 ± 9.81
Food (g)Day 0Day 1Day 2Day 3Day 4
M Transfer40.63 ± 6.02 *65.25 ± 7.57 *60.63 ± 5.29 *61.88 ± 6.3161.00 ± 3.96
M Control69.50 ± 3.3844.13 ± 2.5954.00 ± 3.4256.75 ± 2.7660.50 ± 2.88
F Transfer31.25 ± 3.28 *53.50 ± 3.16 *45.75 ± 3.49 *45.75 ± 3.54 *44.63 ± 3.70
F Control50.75 ± 4.8333.13 ± 3.6439.75 ± 4.0639.13 ± 4.0947.00 ± 3.30

3.3. Body Temperature

Post mortem examination after the completion of the study showed that most of the iButtons were attached to the abdominal wall with one suture. Two iButtons were encapsulated, one of which was detached and found back at the opposite side of the abdomen. However, the two concerning rats had shown no clinical signs. After data analysis of the loggers it was determined that it was not necessary to remove them from the study.
No sex effect was found for body temperature Tb, therefore data was pooled for analysis and the results are shown in Figure 2. During dark periods, Tb was increased for all groups compared to light periods, shown as a light period effect (F(1,52) = 160.50, p < 0.001), except on the day before transfer (DBT1) for transferred animals (Figure 2), when transferred rats were packed in transportation boxes and were transferred to the logistic center awaiting final transfer to the CLARF. During the dark period there was a before-after × treatment interaction (F(1,26) = 9.82, p = 0.004), which was not found during the light period. During the dark period of transfer a decreased Tb was found in both sexes (t(13) = −4.77, p = 0.001).
Figure 2. Core body temperature Tb (°C ± SD) in transferred (TF: triangles, solid line) and control (CO: diamonds, dotted line) rats during dark (black symbols) and light (white symbols) period, 7 days before (DBT) until 22 days after (DAT) transfer (n = 16), sexes pooled. * indicates significant difference between TF and CO.
Figure 2. Core body temperature Tb (°C ± SD) in transferred (TF: triangles, solid line) and control (CO: diamonds, dotted line) rats during dark (black symbols) and light (white symbols) period, 7 days before (DBT) until 22 days after (DAT) transfer (n = 16), sexes pooled. * indicates significant difference between TF and CO.
Animals 04 00712 g002

3.4. Ambient Temperature

The highest Ta’s inside the transportation boxes were measured when transportation boxes were collected at the holding area of Harlan NL (male: 28.50 °C, female: 27.40 °C, Figure 3). Environmental temperature varied between 22 °C and 25 °C at the holding area. During transfer to CLARF Utrecht with the climate controlled van (15 °C), Ta inside transportation boxes with male rats decreased to 17.30 °C and 17.10 °C for female rats. During the period of transfer Ta at the housing facility of control groups varied between 24.1 °C and 25.1 °C.
A significant correlation was detected between Ta (solid line) and Tb (solid line with diamonds) during transfer of rats (Figure 3, r > 0.7, p < 0.001). In the control situation Ta (dotted line) and Tb (grey line with circles) were not correlated (r = −0.061, p > 0.050).
Figure 3. Core body temperature Tb of transferred (triangles) and control (diamonds) rats and ambient temperature Ta inside transportation boxes (solid line) (°C ± SD) during transfer and in control area (dotted line) (top x-axis: time since packing, bottom x-axis: actual time). The black-white bar at bottom indicates light (white) and dark (black) periods. Different stages during transfer are included. n = 16, sexes pooled.
Figure 3. Core body temperature Tb of transferred (triangles) and control (diamonds) rats and ambient temperature Ta inside transportation boxes (solid line) (°C ± SD) during transfer and in control area (dotted line) (top x-axis: time since packing, bottom x-axis: actual time). The black-white bar at bottom indicates light (white) and dark (black) periods. Different stages during transfer are included. n = 16, sexes pooled.
Animals 04 00712 g003

3.5. Blood Parameters

In CORT levels a time × sex interaction (F(4,112) = 21.14, p < 0.001) and a time × treatment interaction (F(4,112) = 3.93, p = 0.005) were found (Figure 4). Female rats had higher CORT levels that also increased more over time than male rats (F(1,28) = 80.96, p < 0.001).
GLUC levels of female rats were lower than GLUC levels in male rats (F(1,27) = 23.19, p < 0.001). A time × sex (F(4,108) = 4.71, p = 0.002) effect was found, but no transfer effect (Figure 5).

3.6. Behavior

Analysis of the behavioral data showed a time × sex interaction on all parameters (Table 2). Further analysis of the behavioral data (Table 3) showed that in female rats transfer decreased social behavior and increased grooming behaviors. Activity was increased 1, 2 and 3 weeks after transfer in all groups. Social behavior was decreased after transfer in all groups, except in female control rats 1 week after transfer. Grooming was increased in female transferred rats directly after unpacking, but differed no longer from baseline after one week of acclimatization. After 3 weeks of acclimatization all behavioral elements in female rats seemed to have stabilized. In male rats differences in behavioral elements were found comparing after-transfer observations with baseline, but no significant differences were found between observations after transfer.
Figure 4. Plasma corticosterone levels (nmol/L) in male (squares) and female (circles), transferred (black symbols-solid line) and control (white symbols-dotted line) rats, one week before, on day of transfer and one, two and three weeks after transfer (n = 8). * indicates significant difference between TF and CO.
Figure 4. Plasma corticosterone levels (nmol/L) in male (squares) and female (circles), transferred (black symbols-solid line) and control (white symbols-dotted line) rats, one week before, on day of transfer and one, two and three weeks after transfer (n = 8). * indicates significant difference between TF and CO.
Animals 04 00712 g004
Figure 5. Plasma glucose levels (mg/dL) in male (squares) and female (circles), transferred (black symbols-solid line) and control (white symbols-dotted line) rats, one week before, on day of transfer and one, two and three weeks after transfer (n = 8).
Figure 5. Plasma glucose levels (mg/dL) in male (squares) and female (circles), transferred (black symbols-solid line) and control (white symbols-dotted line) rats, one week before, on day of transfer and one, two and three weeks after transfer (n = 8).
Animals 04 00712 g005
Table 2. Behavioral observations; Effects (bold p-values) and trends (italic p-values) of transfer on the behavioral categories Locomotor activity (ACT), Social behavior (SOC) and Grooming (GRO) n = eight cages.
Table 2. Behavioral observations; Effects (bold p-values) and trends (italic p-values) of transfer on the behavioral categories Locomotor activity (ACT), Social behavior (SOC) and Grooming (GRO) n = eight cages.
EffectACTSOCGROtot
SexF(32,32) = 5.73, p = 0.023NSNS
TreatmentNSNSNS
Observation (time)F(88,32) = 12.29, p < 0.001F(110,30) = 9.00, p < 0.001F(103,104) = 4.49, p = 0.005
Sex x TreatmentNSNSNS
Sex x timeF(88,32) = 4.00, p = 0.005F(110,30) = 3.08, p = 0.019F(94,104) = 4.70, p = 0.002
Treatment x timeNSNSNS
Sex x Treatment x timeF(90,32) = 2.35, p = 0.077F(110,30) = 2.46, p = 0.066NS
Table 3. Behavioral observations; Comparison between observations, −1: 1 week before transfer; 0: directly after transfer; 1: 1 week after transfer; 2: 2 weeks after transfer; 3: 3 weeks after transfer (significant: bold p-value, trend: italic p-value), Split for sex. ↑: increase or ↓: decrease of behavior during obseravtion on vertical axis (left) compared to behavior during observation on horizontal axis (top) n = eight cages.
Table 3. Behavioral observations; Comparison between observations, −1: 1 week before transfer; 0: directly after transfer; 1: 1 week after transfer; 2: 2 weeks after transfer; 3: 3 weeks after transfer (significant: bold p-value, trend: italic p-value), Split for sex. ↑: increase or ↓: decrease of behavior during obseravtion on vertical axis (left) compared to behavior during observation on horizontal axis (top) n = eight cages.
Obs−1012
sexACTSOCGROACTSOCGROACTSOCGROACTSOCGRO
0FNS0.003↑<0.001
M0.0430.052NS
1FNSNSNSNS0.007↓<0.001
M↑<0.0010.0020.017NSNSNS
2F0.0020.011NS0.004NS↓<0.0010.0030.024NS
M↑<0.0010.0030.013NSNSNSNSNSNS
3F↑<0.001↓<0.001NS↑<0.001NS↓<0.001↑<0.0010.001NSNSNSNS
M↑<0.0010.0010.004NSNS0.056NSNSNSNSNSNS
(Dunn-Šidák correction for multiple comparison(per sex): 1−((0,95)^(1/n)), n = 10 (number of comparisons) α = 0, 005).

4. Discussion

The present study investigated whether changes in core temperature of Wistar rats may be an underlying cause for findings in previous studies in which we found that transfer affected rats at both the physiological and behavioral level [2,14]. Further, in the current study we identified sex specific changes, which showed allostatic stabilization within 2 weeks, though inter-individual variability proved to be high [14]. Though hyperthermia was expected due to a combination of stress and elevated ambient temperature, this effect was not observed during or after transfer. This seems noteworthy, especially since the day before transfer, when animals were packed, was a warm summer day and temperature inside transportation boxes reached up to 28 °C.
Body temperature was recorded every 30 min in the present study. From the literature it is known that maximal rise of body temperature induced by disturbance was detected after 10 min in male NMRI mice [35], followed by a gradual decrease during the following 30–60 min [23,27]. While we did not find any significant increase of body temperature in rats, ambient and body temperature appeared to be significantly correlated during transfer, but not during the control phase. From these results we conclude that, at least under transfer conditions as used in our study, core temperature is unlikely to be the primary cause for physiological or behavioral transfer effects.
As body temperature is correlated with the animal’s overall activity [36], changes in both parameters are determined by circadian rhythm [37,38]. During transfer, however, body temperature appeared to be lower during the dark period in transferred rats when compared to the animals in the control situation, which may be explained by lower activity of the animals in the transportation box.
Further, after transfer, body temperature in transferred rats was lower during the light period when compared to control animals, which may be a response to differences in the management regime between facilities (e.g., fewer disturbances by personnel).
Transfer is a stressful event for rats, as shown in previous studies resulting in elevated levels of CORT [2,4,5,6,7]. Here, CORT levels in male rats of both experimental groups remained stable over time while all females revealed increasing CORT levels over the same time period. No significant transfer effect on CORT levels occurred, which contrasted previous findings. However, in extrapolating results from a study in rabbits where it has been shown that rewarding and handling at an early age led to more tame animals that also did not struggle during a tonic immobility test [39], we provided rats in the present study with a reward after blood sampling and weighing, which may have counteracted the stress caused by blood sampling, thus, resulting in less increase in plasma corticosterone levels compared to other studies. Except at baseline and directly after transfer, CORT levels of female rats were higher compared to male rats. It is known that the HPA-axis activity is sex-dependent, resulting in higher baseline CORT levels [39] and a more pronounced increase in CORT levels after exposure to a stressor in female rats [40]. Further, rats used in the present study were adolescent, implying the development of estrous in females, which also is known to increase CORT levels [41,42,43,44]. Similar to findings in the literature, decreased CORT levels in transferred female rats directly after transfer had been found in our previous study [14] and by Van Ruiven et al. (1998) [10]. Unexpectedly however, an increased level of CORT was measured in male control animals at the day of transfer, an effect that may be attributed to external conditions (here: in the replacement of a technician who collected the blood samples).
As result of a stress-response, the production of glucose (GLUC) has been reported to increase [45,46]. However, in the present study GLUC levels were within a normal range 134–219 mg/dL [47].

5. Conclusions

Bodyweight and food and water intake recovered within a few days after transfer. Behavior in this study shows time effects, most clearly in female rats, but no treatment effects, indicating that before-after transfer differences may be a matter of aging rather than of transfer-stress. Daily practice shows that most laboratory animals are transferred at an age at which they are still (physically) developing.
This study showed that it took one day for body weight and temperature, two days for water intake, and four days for food intake to return to control levels. Levels of CORT and GLUC were not affected by transfer in this study. Behavioral parameters indicate that female rats seemed to stabilize after two weeks of acclimatization, male rats within one week. Although core temperature in this study was not affected by transfer, it is recommended to keep environmental temperature and temperature inside transportation boxes as constant as possible, and within the physiologically neutral range of the animals, as external temperature strongly correlates with core body temperature in rats. We conclude from these findings that it is advisable to use an acclimatization period of at least one week in male and two weeks in female Wistar rats.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a research grant from Harlan Laboratories B.V., The Netherlands. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The assistance of the Surgical Team at the Harlan Laboratories B.V. breeding facility and the useful comments on the manuscript of habil F.J. van der Staay, Baumans, and three anonymous referees are gratefully acknowledged.

Author Contributions

Johanna W. M. Arts was the primary investigator. Frauke Ohl and Klaas Kramer assisted with experimental design and interpretation of data. Arts ran the preliminary statistics and wrote the original draft. Nynke R. Oosterhuis assisted in primary investigation, analyses, and writing of the draft. Frauke Ohl and Klaas Kramer reviewed and revised the final manuscript for submission.

Appendix

Table A1. Details of ethogram used.
Table A1. Details of ethogram used.
BehaviorDescription
ExplorationAnimal is sniffing air, the sawdust or cage wall. Collection of behaviors like mobile exploration, sniffing, root/dig and gnaw/nibble, manipulating shelter.state
Rearing during exploration is scored as rearing
WalkObvious forward movement of the animal, with as goal to bridge a distance. Is scored when more than two steps are made with the hind legsstate
Rest (lie&sit)Resting behavior, lying, and sitting without sniffing or looking around in an alert state.state
EatTaking in foodstate
ShakeShaking of the head or shaking the whole bodyevent
ScanHorizontal rocking movement of the head, with the rest of the body still. If performed in a rearing posture this is scored as scanstate
RearAnimal is exploring (sniffing, watching) with its fore paws lifted off the ground. Only standing on its hind legs, or supported by putting the forepaws on the cage wall. If an animal sits sniffing and lifts its forepaws off the ground, this is a rear (also if the animal does not stretch its body)state
Hop/JumpHop: horizontal movement of the animal by little jumpsstate
Jump: the animal jumps on the spotevent
FreezeAnimal freezes and stops all movements state
In shelterAnimal is (partially) in the shelter. If not visible in the shelter, this is still scored as shelterstate
Social explorationAnimal is sniffing/exploring cage matestate
Follow/ChaseAnimal is following/chasing a cage matestate
Animal is being followed by a cage matestate
PushAnimal is pushing a cage mate with its nose or pawevent
Animal is being pushed by a cage mateevent
PinAnimal is pinning a cage mate on its back or sidestate
Animal is being pinned by a cage mate
Often followed by social groomstate
Social groomAnimal is licking a cage mate or nibbling the fur of a cage matestate
Animal is being licked-nibbled by a cage matestate

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Obernier, J.A.; Baldwin, R.L. Establishing an appropriate period of acclimatization following transportation of laboratory animals. ILAR J. 2006, 47, 364–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Arts, J.W.M.; Kramer, K.; Arndt, S.S.; Ohl, F. The impact of transportation on physiological and behavioral parameters in Wistar rats—Implications for acclimatization periods. ILAR J. 2012, 53, E82–E98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. McEwen, B.S. The neurobiology of stress: From serendipity to clinical relevance. Brain Res. 2000, 886, 172–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Olfe, J.; Domanska, G.; Schuett, C.; Kiank, C. Different stress-related phenotypes of BALB/c mice from in-house or vendor: Alterations of the sympathetic and HPA axis responsiveness. BMC Physiol. 2010, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Conour, L.A.; Murray, K.A.; Brown, M.J. Preparation of animals for research—Issues to consider for rodents and rabbits. ILAR J. 2006, 47, 283–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Tuli, J.S.; Smith, J.A.; Morton, D.B. Stress measurements in mice after transportation. Lab. Anim. 1995, 29, 132–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Shim, S.; Lee, S.; Kim, C.; Kim, B.; Jee, S.; Lee, S.; Hwang, D. Effects of air transportation cause physiological and biochemical changes indicative of stress leading to regulation of chaperone expression levels and corticosterone concentration. Exp. Anim. Jpn. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2009, 58, 11–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Tabata, H.; Kitamura, T.; Nagamatsu, N. Comparison of effects of restraint, cage transportation, anaesthesia and repeated bleeding on plasma glucose levels between mice and rats. Lab. Anim. 1998, 32, 143–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Stemkens-Sevens, S.; Berkel, K.V.; de Greeuw, I.; Snoeijer, B.; Kramer, K. The use of radiotelemetry to assess the time needed to acclimatize guinea pigs following several hours of ground transport. Lab. Anim. 2009, 43, 78–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Ruiven, R.V.; Meijer, G.W.; Wiersma, A.; Baumans, V.; van Zutphen, L.F.M.; Ritskes-Hoitinga, J. The influence of transportation stress on selected nutritional parameters to establish the necessary minimum period for adaptation in rat feeding studies. Lab. Anim. 1998, 32, 446–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Capdevila, S.; Giral, M.; Ruiz de la Torre, J.L.; Russell, R.J.; Kramer, K. Acclimatization of rats after ground transportation to a new animal facility. Lab. Anim. 2007, 41, 255–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Liste, M.G.; María, G.A.; García-Belenguer, S.; Chacón, G.; Gazzola, P.; Villarroel, M. The effect of transport time, season and position on the truck on stress response in rabbits. World Rabbit Sci. 2008, 16, 229–235. [Google Scholar]
  13. Baldwin, R.L.; Bhat, C.R.; Bouyer, D.H.; Dhabhar, F.S.; Leary, S.L.; McGlone, J.J.; Raemdonck, E.; Smith, J.L.; Swanson, J.C. Guidelines for the Humane Transportation of Research Animals; National Academies Press (US): Washington, DC, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  14. Arts, J.W.M.; Kramer, K.; Arndt, S.S.; Ohl, F. Sex differences in physiological acclimatization after transfer in Wistar rats. Animals 2014, 4, 693–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Arts, J.W.M. Temperature Measurements during Transfer of BALB/c Mice; Internal Report; Harlan Laboratories B.V.: Venray, The Netherlands, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  16. Syversen, E.; Pineda, F.J.; Watson, J. Temperature variations recorded during interinstitutional air shipment of laboratory mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2008, 47, 31–36. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  17. Doi, K.; Kuroshima, A. Sexual difference in thermoregulatory ability of rats exposed to cold and heat. J. Therm. Biol. 1982, 7, 99–105. [Google Scholar]
  18. Gautier, H. Body temperature regulation in the rat. J. Therm. Biol. 2000, 25, 273–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Gordon, C.J. Thermal biology of the laboratory rat. Physiol. Behav. 1990, 47, 963–991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Poole, S.; Stephenson, J.D. Body temperature regulation and thermoneutrality in rats. Q. J. Exp. Physiol. 1977, 62, 143–149. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  21. McGivern, R.F.; Zuloaga, D.G.; Handra, R.J. Sex differences in stress-induced hyperthermia in rats: Restraint versus confinement. Physiol. Behav. 2009, 98, 416–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Kramer, K.; van de Weerd, H.; Mulder, A.; Heijningen, C.V.; Baumans, V.; Remie, R.; van Zutphen, L.F.M. Effect of conditioning on the increase of heart rate and body temperature provoked by handling in the mouse. ATLA Altern. Lab. Anim. 2004, 32, S177–S181. [Google Scholar]
  23. Dallmann, R.; Steinlechner, S.; Horsten, S.V.; Karl, T. Stress-induced hyperthermia in the rat: Comparison of classical and novel recording methods. Lab. Anim. 2006, 40, 186–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Nakamori, T.; Morimoto, A.; Murakami, N. Effect of a central CRF antagonist on cardiovascular and thermoregulatory responses induced by stress or IL-1 beta. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 1993, 265, R834–R839. [Google Scholar]
  25. Guilhamou, R.; Boulamery, A.; DeLuca, B.; deTurmeny, E.; Bruguerolle, B. Effects of induced hyperthermia on pharmacokinetics of ropivacaine in rats. Fundam. Clin. Pharmacol. 2010, 24, 463–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Crandall, C.G.; González-Alonso, J. Cardiovascular function in the heat-stressed human. Acta Physiol. 2010, 199, 407–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Groenink, L.; van der Gugten, J.; Zethof, T.; van der Heyden, J.; Olivier, B. Stress-induced hyperthermia in mice: Hormonal correlates. Physiol. Behav. 1994, 56, 747–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Fluttert, M.; Dalm, S.; Oitzl, M.S. A refined method for sequential blood sampling by tail incision in rats. Lab. Anim. 2000, 34, 372–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Dubowski, K.M. An o-Toluidine method for body-fluid glucose determination. Clin. Chem. 1962, 8, 215–235. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  30. Burn, C.C.; Peters, A.G.; Mason, G.J. Acute effects of cage cleaning at different frequencies on laboratory rat behaviour and welfare. Anim. Welf. 2006, 15, 161–171. [Google Scholar]
  31. Saibaba, P.; Sales, G.D.; Stodulski, G.; Hau, J. Behaviour of rats in their home cages: Daytime variations and effects of routine husbandry procedures analysed by time sampling techniques. Lab. Anim. 1996, 30, 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Kalueff, A.V.; Tuohimaa, P. The grooming analysis algorithm discriminates between different levels of anxiety in rats: Potential utility for neurobehavioural stress research. J. Neurosci. Methods 2005, 143, 169–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Festing, M.F.W.; Altman, G.D. Guidelines for the design and statistical analysis of experiments using laboratory animals. ILAR J. 2002, 43, 244–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Fekedulegn, D.B.; Andrew, M.E.; Burchfiel, C.M.; Violanti, J.M.; Hartley, T.A.; Charles, L.E.; Miller, D.B. Area under the curve and other summary indicators of repeated waking cortisol measurements. Psychosom. Med. 2007, 69, 651–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Van der Heyden, J.A.M.; Zethof, T.J.J.; Olivier, B. Stress-induced hyperthermia in singly housed mice. Physiol. Behav. 1997, 62, 463–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Buttner, D.; Wollnik, F. Spontaneous short-term fluctuations in the daily pattern of heart rate, body temperature and locomotor activity in the laboratory rat. Lab. Anim. 1982, 16, 319–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Benstaali, C.; Mailloux, A.; Bogdan, A.; Auzeby, A.; Touitou, Y. Circadian rhythms of body temperature and motor activity in rodents—Their relationships with the light-dark cycle. Life Sci. 2001, 68, 2645–2656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Refinetti, R.; Menaker, M. The circadian rhythm of body temperature. Physiol. Behav. 1992, 51, 613–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Verwer, C.M.; van Amerongen, G.; van den Bos, R.; Hendriksen, C.F.M. Handling effects on body weight and behaviour of group-housed male rabbits in a laboratory setting. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 117, 93–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Mashoodh, R.; Wright, L.D.; Hébert, K.; Perrot-Sinal, T.S. Investigation of sex differences in behavioural, endocrine, and neural measures following repeated psychological stressor exposure. Behav. Brain Res. 2008, 188, 368–379. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  41. Goel, N.; Bale, T.L. Sex differences in the serotonergic influence on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal stress axis. Endocrinology 2010, 151, 1784–1794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Atkinson, H.; Waddell, B. Circadian variation in basal plasma corticosterone and adrenocorticotropin in the rat: Sexual dimorphism and changes across the estrous cycle. Endocrinology 1997, 138, 3842–3848. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  43. Windle, R.J.; Wood, S.A.; Shanks, N.; Lightman, S.L.; Ingram, C.D. Ultradian rhythm of basal corticosterone release in the female rat: Dynamic interaction with the response to acute stress. Endocrinology 1998, 139, 443–450. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  44. Figueiredo, H.; Dolgas, C.; Herman, J. Stress activation of cortex and hippocampus is modulated by sex and stage of estrus. Endocrinology 2002, 143, 2534–2540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Sherwood, L.; Klandorf, H.; Yancey, P.H. Animal Physiology: From Genes to Organisms, Brooks/Cole; Cengage Learning: South Melbourne, Australia, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  46. De Boer, S.F.; Koopmans, S.J.; Slangen, J.L.; Gugten, J.V.D. Plasma catecholamine, corticosterone and glucose responses to repeated stress in rats: Effect of interstressor interval length. Physiol. Behav. 1990, 47, 1117–1124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Van Zutphen, L.F.; Baumans, V.; Beynen, A.C. Principles of Laboratory Animal Science, revised ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2001. [Google Scholar]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Arts, J.W.M.; Oosterhuis, N.R.; Kramer, K.; Ohl, F. Effects of Transfer from Breeding to Research Facility on the Welfare of Rats. Animals 2014, 4, 712-728. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4040712

AMA Style

Arts JWM, Oosterhuis NR, Kramer K, Ohl F. Effects of Transfer from Breeding to Research Facility on the Welfare of Rats. Animals. 2014; 4(4):712-728. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4040712

Chicago/Turabian Style

Arts, Johanna W. M., Nynke R. Oosterhuis, Klaas Kramer, and Frauke Ohl. 2014. "Effects of Transfer from Breeding to Research Facility on the Welfare of Rats" Animals 4, no. 4: 712-728. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4040712

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop