A Comparison of the Effects of Short-Term Physical and Combined Multi-Modal Training on Cognitive Functions
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Pretests and Posttests
- Perceptual processing (M3): Selective attention/perceptual processing was assessed with the M3 test. During this computer-based letter focus task, participants were instructed to press one of two response keys when the upper- or lowercase target letter “M” surrounded by three dots appeared onscreen. When the upper- or lowercase letter “W” or the upper- or lowercase letter “M” surrounded by less than three dots were presented, participants had to press the other response key. The participants performed the task under time pressure (90 s) and had to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible. The mean response accuracy (percentage of correct responses) and mean response time (RT) were analyzed.
- Alertness (0-back): Participants performed a 0-back-task on a sequence of letters successively presented on the computer screen. If the currently shown letter was an “X”, a response key had to be pressed. A total of 102 letters were presented at intervals of 1500 ms, including 23 targets. The participants had to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible. The mean response accuracy and mean RT were analyzed.
- Working Memory Updating (2-back): Participants performed a 2-back-task on a sequence of letters successively presented on the computer screen. They had to press the response key if the current letter matched the letter presented two positions back in the sequence. A total of 156 letters were presented at intervals of 1500 ms, including 31 targets. The subjects had to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible. The mean response accuracy and mean RT were analyzed.
- Short-Term Memory (Immediate and Delayed Recall): Participants were instructed to memorize a list of 10 words that was presented for one minute. They then had to recall as many of the words as possible immediately after presentation (immediate recall) and 30 min later (delayed recall). The participants were given a 1.5 min time window during which they had to type in the words they recalled from the list via a computer keyboard. The immediate recall score was defined as the sum of correctly recalled words immediately after the presentation of the list. The delayed recall score was defined as the sum of correctly recalled words 30 min later. Two different versions of the test featuring different word lists were used for pretest and posttest measurement. The mean recall scores were analyzed separately for immediate and delayed recall.
- Inhibition (Stroop): A computer-based Stroop task [37] with three subtests was used to assess inhibition. First, the participants were presented with 20 trials, each featuring one of four color words (red, green, yellow, or blue) in white font. Their task was to indicate the meaning of the words by pressing an assigned key on the keyboard. Second, the participants were shown 20 trials, each featuring either a red, green, yellow, or blue rectangle, and had to indicate its color by pressing the assigned key. Third, 120 trials followed, each featuring one of the four color words printed in colored font. Participants were asked to indicate the font color by pressing the respective key. Font color was congruent to the word meaning on half of the trials (e.g., the word “green” presented in green font) and incongruent to it on the other half (e.g., the word “blue” presented in yellow font). The response time was limited to 2500 ms per trial. The mean Stroop effects in accuracy and RT (performance differences between congruent and incongruent trials) were calculated and analyzed.
- Perceptual Speed (TMT): A computerized version of the Trail Making Test (TMT) A and B [38] was used to assess perceptual speed. In a first subtest (TMT-A), the participants were presented with a display featuring the numbers from 1 to 26 at random positions. They were instructed to click on these numbers in ascending order as quickly as possible. In a second subtest (TMT-B), the display contained the numbers from 1 to 13 and the letters from A to M. The participants had to alternate between clicking on the numbers and letters in ascending order as quickly as possible. The mean response accuracy and mean overall completion time were analyzed.
- Mental rotation (Rotate Mirror): In this test, the participants viewed 26 trials, each featuring a row of five identical complex geometric shapes rotated into distinct positions along the horizontal axis. One of the five shapes was additionally mirror-reversed. The participants had to click on this target shape as quickly as possible. The maximum response time window was 30 s per trial. The mean response accuracy and mean RT were analyzed.
2.3. Training Interventions
2.4. Procedure
2.5. Data Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Pretest (Baseline) Performance
3.2. Agility Board-Related Training Gains and Transfer Effects
3.2.1. Reaction Duo
3.2.2. Forward Sequence Memory
3.2.3. Backward Sequence Memory
3.2.4. Math Duo
3.2.5. Rotation
3.2.6. Forward Sequence Memory Duo
3.2.7. Summary—Agility Board-Related Training Gains and Transfer Effects
3.3. Cognitive Transfer Effects
3.3.1. Perceptual Processing (M3)
3.3.2. Alertness (0-Back)
3.3.3. Working Memory Updating (2-Back)
3.3.4. Short-Term Memory (Immediate and Delayed Recall)
3.3.5. Inhibition (Stroop)
3.3.6. Perceptual Speed (TMT)
3.3.7. Mental Rotation (Rotate Mirror)
3.3.8. Summary—Cognitive Transfer Effects
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Colcombe, S.; Kramer, A.F. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 14, 125–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bridenbaugh, S.A. Kognition and Mobilität. Ther. Umsch. 2015, 72, 219–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marchetti, R.; Forte, R.; Borzacchini, M.; Vazou, S.; Tomporowski, P.D.; Pesce, C. Physical and motor fitness, sport skills and executive function in adolescents: A moderated prediction model. Psychology 2015, 6, 1915–1929. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Colcombe, S.J.; Kramer, A.F.; Erickson, K.I.; Scalf, P.; McAuley, E.; Cohen, N.J.; Webb, A.; Jerome, G.J.; Marquez, D.X.; Elavsky, S. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2004, 101, 3316–3321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Erickson, K.I.; Voss, M.W.; Prakash, R.S.; Basak, C.; Szabo, A.; Chaddock, L.; Kim, J.S.; Heo, S.; Alves, H.; White, S.M.; et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 3017–3022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Liu-Ambrose, T.; Nagamatsu, L.S.; Graf, P.; Beattie, B.; Ashe, M.C.; Handy, T.C. Resistance Training and Executive Functions: A 12-Month Randomized Controlled Trial. Arch. Intern. Med. 2010, 170, 170–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Heisz, J.J.; Clark, I.B.; Bonin, K.; Paolucci, E.M.; Michalski, B.; Becker, S.; Fahnestock, M. The effects of physical exercise and cognitive training on memory and neurotrophic factors. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 29, 1895–1907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guadagni, V.; Drogos, L.L.; Tyndall, A.V.; Davenport, M.H.; Anderson, T.J.; Eskes, G.A.; Longman, R.S.; Hill, M.D.; Hogan, D.B.; Poulin, M.J. Aerobic exercise improves cognition and cerebrovascular regulation in older adults. Neurology 2020, 94, e2245–e2257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chang, Y.; Pan, C.Y.; Chen, F.T.; Tsai, C.L.; Huang, C.C. Effect of resistance-exercise training on cognitive function in healthy older adults: A Review. J. Aging Phys. Act. 2012, 20, 497–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Liu-Ambrose, T.; Nagamatsu, L.S.; Voss, M.W.; Khan, K.M.; Handy, T.C. Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: A 12-month randomized controlled trial. Neurobiol. Aging 2012, 33, 1690–1698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herold, F.; Törpel, A.; Schega, L.; Müller, N.G. Functional and/or structural brain changes in response to resistance exercises and resistance training lead to cognitive improvements—A systematic review. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 2019, 16, 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Young, J.; Angevaren, M.; Rusted, J.; Tabet, N. Aerobic exercise to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2015, 4, CD005381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lauenroth, A.; Ioannidis, A.E.; Teichmann, B. Influence of combined physical and cognitive training on cognition: A systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2016, 16, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Zhu, X.; Yin, S.; Lang, M.; He, R.; Li, J. The more the better? A meta-analysis on effects of combined cognitive and physical intervention on cognition in healthy older adults. Ageing Res. Rev. 2016, 31, 67–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, A.K.; Liu-Ambrose, T.; Tate, R.; Lord, S.R. The effect of group-based exercise on cognitive performance and mood in seniors residing in intermediate care and self-care retirement facilities: A randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Sports Med. 2009, 43, 608–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, J.C.; Bryan, S.; Marra, C.A.; Sharma, D.; Chan, A.; Beattie, B.L.; Graf, P.; Liu-Ambrose, T. An Economic Evaluation of Resistance Training and Aerobic Training versus Balance and Toning Exercises in Older Adults with Mild Cognitive Impairment. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e63031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eggenberger, P.; Schumacher, V.; Angst, M.; Theill, N.; de Bruin, E.D. Does multicomponent physical exercise with simultaneous cognitive training boost cognitive performance in older adults? A 6-month randomized controlled trial with a 1-year follow-up. Clin. Interv. Aging 2015, 10, 1335–1349. [Google Scholar]
- Jansen, P.; Titze, C.; Heil, M. The influence of juggling on mental rotation performance. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 2009, 40, 351–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kattenstroth, J.C.; Kolankowska, I.; Kalisch, T.; Dinse, H.R. Superior sensory, motor, and cognitive performance in elderly individuals with multi-year dancing activities. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2010, 2, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kattenstroth, J.C.; Kalisch, T.; Holt, S.; Tegenthoff, M.; Dinse, H.R. Six months of dance intervention enhances postural, sensorimotor, and cognitive performance in elderly without affecting cardio-respiratory functions. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2013, 5, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Li, J.X.; Xu, D.Q.; Hong, Y. Effects of 16-week Tai Chi intervention on postural stability and proprioception of knee and ankle in older people. Age Ageing 2008, 37, 575–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Theill, N.; Schumacher, V.; Adelsberger, R.; Martin, M.; Jäncke, L. Effects of simultaneously performed cognitive and physical training in older adults. BMC Neurosci. 2013, 14, 103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kelly, M.E.; Loughrey, D.; Lawlor, B.A.; Robertson, I.H.; Walsh, C.; Brennan, S. The impact of exercise on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 2014, 16, 12–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Joubert, C.; Chainay, H. Aging brain: The effect of combined cognitive and physical training on cognition as compared to cognitive and physical training alone–a systematic review. Clin. Interv. Aging 2018, 13, 1267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lövdén, M.; Bäckman, L.; Lindenberger, U.; Schaefer, S.; Schmiedek, F. A theoretical framework for the study of adult cognitive plasticity. Psychol. Bull. 2010, 136, 659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jaeggi, S.M.; Buschkuehl, M.; Jonides, J.; Perrig, W.J. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 6829–6833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Karbach, J.; Kray, J. How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 978–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beck, F.; Beckmann, J. Die Bedeutung striataler Plastizitätsvorgänge und unerwarteten Bewegungserfolgs für sportmotorisches Lernen. Sportwissenschaft 2009, 40, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raichlen, D.A.; Alexander, G.E. Adaptive capacity: An evolutionary neuroscience model linking exercise, cognition, and brain health. Trends Neurosci. 2017, 40, 408–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erickson, K.I.; Hillman, C.; Stillman, C.M.; Ballard, R.M.; Bloodgood, B.; Conroy, D.E.; Macko, R.; Marquez, D.X.; Petruzzello, S.J.; Powell, K.E.; et al. Physical activity, cognition, and brain outcomes: A review of the 2018 physical activity guidelines. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2019, 51, 1242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Voelcker-Rehage, C.; Godde, B.; Staudinger, U.M. Cardiovascular and coordination training differentially improve cognitive performance and neural processing in older adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Nagamatsu, L.S.; Handy, T.C.; Hsu, C.L.; Voss, M.; Liu-Ambrose, T. Resistance training promotes cognitive and functional brain plasticity in seniors with probable mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Intern. Med. 2012, 172, 666–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nouchi, R.; Taki, Y.; Takeuchi, H.; Hashizume, H.; Nozawa, T.; Sekiguchi, A.; Nouchi, H.; Kawashima, R. Beneficial effects of short-term combination exercise training on diverse cognitive functions in healthy older people: Study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 2012, 13, 200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nouchi, R.; Taki, Y.; Takeuchi, H.; Sekiguchi, A.; Hashizume, H.; Nozawa, T.; Nouchi, H.; Kawashima, R. Four weeks of combination exercise training improved executive functions, episodic memory, and processing speed in healthy elderly people: Evidence from a randomized controlled trial. Age 2014, 36, 787–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Frey, I.; Berg, A.; Gratwohl, D.; Keul, J. Freiburger Fragebogen zur körperlichen Aktivität. Soz. Präventivmed. 1999, 44, 55–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lehrl, S. Mehrfach-Wortwahl-Test (MWT); Medizinische Verlagsgesellschaft: Erlangen, Germany, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Stroop, J.R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reitan, R.M. Trail Making Test: Manual for Administration, Scoring and Interpretation; Indiana University: Bloomington, IN, USA, 1956; p. 134. [Google Scholar]
- Strack, A.; Eifler, C. The individual lifting performance method (ILP)—A practical method for fitness-and recreational strength training. In Current Results of Strength Training Research; Cuvillier: Göttingen, Germany, 2005; pp. 153–163. [Google Scholar]
- Berger, R.A. Determination of the resistance load for 1-RM and 10-RM. J. Assoc. Phys. Ment. Rehabil. 1961, 15, 132–138. [Google Scholar]
- Owen, N.; Healy, G.N.; Matthews, C.E.; Dunstan, D.W. Too much sitting: The population-health science of sedentary behavior. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 2010, 38, 105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berryman, N.; Bherer, L.; Nadeau, S.; Lauzière, S.; Lehr, L.; Bobeuf, F.; Lussier, M.; Kergoat, M.J.; Vu, T.T.M.; Bosquet, L. Multiple roads lead to Rome: Combined high-intensity aerobic and strength training vs. gross motor activities leads to equivalent improvement in executive functions in a cohort of healthy older adults. Age 2014, 36, 9710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Northey, J.M.; Cherbuin, N.; Pumpa, K.L.; Smee, D.J.; Rattray, B. Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 2018, 52, 154–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stojan, R.; Voelcker-Rehage, C. A systematic review on the cognitive benefits and neurophysiological correlates of exergaming in healthy older adults. J. Clin. Med. 2019, 8, 734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Basak, C.; Boot, W.R.; Voss, M.W.; Kramer, A.F. Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychol. Aging 2008, 23, 765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Dahlin, E.; Nyberg, L.; Bäckman, L.; Neely, A.S. Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: Immediate training gains, transfer, and long-term maintenance. Psychol. Aging 2008, 23, 720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Papp, K.V.; Walsh, S.J.; Snyder, P.J. Immediate and delayed effects of cognitive interventions in healthy elderly: A review of current literature and future directions. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2009, 5, 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zehnder, F.; Martin, M.; Altgassen, M.; Clare, L. Memory training effects in old age as markers of plasticity: A meta-analysis. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2009, 27, 507–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Von Bastian, C.C.; Oberauer, K. Distinct transfer effects of training different facets of working memory capacity. J. Mem. Lang. 2013, 69, 36–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salminen, T.; Strobach, T.; Schubert, T. On the impacts of working memory training on executive functioning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brehmer, Y.; Westerberg, H.; Bäckman, L. Working-memory training in younger and older adults: Training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hertzog, C.; Kramer, A.F.; Wilson, R.S.; Lindenberger, U. Enrichment Effects on Adult Cognitive Development: Can the Functional Capacity of Older Adults Be Preserved and Enhanced? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 2008, 9, 1–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jaeggi, S.M.; Studer-Luethi, B.; Buschkuehl, M.; Su, Y.F.; Jonides, J.; Perrig, W.J. The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning—implications for training and transfer. Intelligence 2010, 38, 625–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strobach, T.; Salminen, T.; Karbach, J.; Schubert, T. Practice-related optimization and transfer of executive functions: A general review and a specific realization of their mechanisms in dual tasks. Psychol. Res. 2014, 78, 836–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Klingberg, T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010, 14, 317–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Küper, K.; Karbach, J. Increased training complexity reduces the effectiveness of brief working memory training: Evidence from short-term single and dual n-back training interventions. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2016, 28, 199–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marquié, J.C.; Duarte, L.R.; Bessières, P.; Dalm, C.; Gentil, C.; Ruidavets, J.B. Higher mental stimulation at work is associated with improved cognitive functioning in both young and older workers. Ergonomics 2010, 53, 1287–1301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gajewski, P.D.; Wild-Wall, N.; Schapkin, S.A.; Erdmann, U.; Freude, G.; Falkenstein, M. Effects of aging and job demands on cognitive flexibility assessed by task switching. Biol. Psychol. 2010, 85, 187–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oltmanns, J.; Richter, G.; Godde, B.; Staudinger, U.M. Healthy Aging at Work. In Healthy at Work; Wiencke, M., Cacace, M., Fischer, S., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 69–84. [Google Scholar]
- Gajewski, P.D.; Falkenstein, M. Neurocognition of aging in working environments. Z. Arb. 2011, 44, 307–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Physical Training | Combined Training | Active Control | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | 33 | 35 | 34 | ||||
Sex Distribution | Males (N) | 28 | 30 | 32 | |||
Females (N) | 5 | 5 | 2 | ||||
Highest Educational Degree (Years of Education) | 12 Years (N) | 2 | 2 | 4 | |||
11 Years (N) | 7 | 9 | 7 | ||||
10 Years (N) | 14 | 13 | 11 | ||||
9 Years (N) | 10 | 10 | 12 | ||||
Other | 0 | 1 | 0 | ||||
Age Range (Years) | 20–61 | 20–52 | 21–60 | ||||
M | (SE) | M | (SE) | M | (SE) | ||
Age (Years) | 36.1 | (1.8) | 37.1 | (1.5) | 37.3 | (1.5) | |
fFkA Overall Activity Score | 48.5 | (7) | 41.5 | (6.7) | 30.5 | (4.1) | |
MWT score | 28 | (0.8) | 29 | (0.5) | 27.4 | (0.8) |
Test | Target Dimension | Task Description | Dependent Variables |
---|---|---|---|
Agility Board 1: Reaction Duo | Reaction and co-ordination | Two randomly selected response fields are highlighted on the tablet screen. The participants’ task is to jump onto these two fields with both feet simultaneously as quickly as possible. (Task duration: 60 s) | Accuracy and overall number of responses completed within time limit |
Agility Board 2: Forward Sequence Memory | Working memory | Onscreen, the response fields are highlighted sequentially. The participants’ task is to memorize the sequence and tap the respective fields on the board with one foot in the correct (forward) order. The task’s difficulty is adaptable 1. (Task duration: 180 s) | Accuracy and set size level achieved |
Agility Board 3: Backward Sequence Memory | Working memory | Onscreen, the response fields are highlighted sequentially. The participants’ task is to memorize the sequence and tap the respective fields on the board with one foot in reverse order. The task’s difficulty is adaptable 1. (Task duration: 180 s) | Accuracy and set size level achieved |
Agility Board 4: Math Duo | Visual search and processing speed | Onscreen, an addition term is presented showing only the sum, but leaving the summands blank (e.g., _ + _ = 8). Below the addition term, the response fields of the board are depicted superimposed with numbers, which could potentially be the summands. The participants’ task is to jump onto the two response fields assigned to the numbers that yield the displayed sum. (Task duration: 60 s) | Accuracy and overall number of responses completed within time limit |
Agility Board 5: Rotation | Mental rotation, processing speed, and inhibition | Onscreen, a target response field is highlighted, along with a direction arrow. The participants’ task is to mentally rotate this display in the direction indicated by the arrow and then tap the field corresponding to the target field on the board. (Task duration: 60 s) | Accuracy and overall number of responses completed within time limit |
Agility Board 6: Forward Sequence Memory Duo | Working memory | Onscreen, pairs of response fields are highlighted sequentially. The participants’ task is to memorize the sequence and jump on the respective field pairs on the board with both feet in the correct (forward) order. The task’s difficulty is adaptable 1. (Task duration: 180 s) | Accuracy and set size level achieved |
Physical Training | Combined Training | Active Control | Contrast Analysis | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pretest | Posttest | Pretest | Posttest | Pretest | Posttest | Time | Group | IA | PT > AC | CT > AC | CT > PT | ||
Agility Board Tasks | |||||||||||||
Reaction Duo | Acc | 87.3 (1.2) | 90.2 (1.4) | 88.2 (1.1) | 91.4 (1.3) | 87.3 (1.2) | 90.3 (1.4) | 0.002 | |||||
Resp | 56.9 (1.5) | 57.7 (2) | 55.1 (1.5) | 71.1 (2) | 55.2 (1.6) | 56.2 (2.1) | <0.001 | 0.003 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | ||
Rotation | Acc | 89.2 (1.8) | 91.5 (1.2) | 90.1 (1.7) | 96.1 (1.1) | 86.1 (1.8) | 91 (1.2) | <0.001 | 0.033 | ||||
Resp | 42.1 (1.5) | 48.3 (1.6) | 43.2 (1.4) | 68 (1.6) | 41.2 (1.5) | 44.6 (1.7) | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | ||
Math Duo | Acc | 86.7 (1.8) | 88.3 (1.9) | 87.4 (1.7) | 93.8 (1.8) | 84 (1.8) | 84.1 (2) | 0.030 | 0.010 | 0.096 | |||
Resp | 16.4 (0.7) | 17.2 (0.8) | 16.7 (0.6) | 23.3 (0.7) | 16.2 (0.7) | 16.5 (0.8) | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | ||
Forward Sequence Memory | Acc | 76.8 (1.4) | 78.8 (1.3) | 77.2 (1.3) | 85.8 (1.2) | 74.9 (1.4) | 75.7 (1.3) | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 0.002 | |
Level | 4.7 (0.1) | 5.2 (0.1) | 4.9 (0.1) | 5.9 (0.1) | 4.8 (0.1) | 4.8 (0.1) | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 0.035 | <0.001 | 0.011 | |
Backward Sequence Memory | Acc | 75.2 (1.2) | 77.5 (1.4) | 75 (1.1) | 82.9 (1.3) | 73.5 (1.2) | 73.9 (1.4) | <0.001 | 0.004 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 0.004 | |
Level | 4.7 (0.1) | 4.9 (0.2) | 4.7 (0.1) | 5.6 (0.1) | 4.5 (0.2) | 4.6 (0.2) | <0.001 | 0.004 | <0.001 | <0.001 | 0.001 | ||
Forward Sequence Memory Duo | Acc | 65.9 (1.2) | 66.3 (1.1) | 65.6 (1.2) | 70.1 (1.1) | 62.5 (1.3) | 63.8 (1.2) | 0.011 | 0.003 | 0.081 | |||
Level | 3.4 (0.1) | 3.6 (0.1) | 3.5 (0.1) | 4.0 (0.1) | 3.2 (0.1) | 3.3 (0.1) | 0.002 | 0.013 | 0.063 | 0.087 | 0.051 | ||
Cognitive Tasks | |||||||||||||
M3: Perceptual Processing | Acc | 97.5 (1.8) | 95.9 (1.1) | 94.2 (1.8) | 95.1 (1) | 94.9 (1.8) | 94.4 (1.8) | ||||||
RT | 660.5 (23.2) | 617.9 (34.3) | 652.9 (22.5) | 577.4 (33.4) | 668.7 (22.9) | 660.9 (33.8) | 0.021 | ||||||
Mental Rotation | Acc | 77.6 (3.4) | 77.4 (3.4) | 79.8 (3.3) | 84.7 (3.3) | 74.9 (3.3) | 78.5 (3.3) | 0.060 | |||||
RT | 8641.6 (546.6) | 7570.3 (493) | 7940.7 (530.8) | 7613.3 (478.7) | 8888.9 (538.5) | 8090.6 (485.6) | <0.001 | ||||||
Stroop | Acc-D | 7.3 (1.9) | 2.7 (1.1) | 5.7 (1.8) | 3.5 (1.1) | 11.2 (1.9) | 4.7 (1.1) | <0.001 | |||||
RT-D | 158.5 (15.7) | 133.2 (16.6) | 155.1 (15.3) | 107.7 (16.1) | 161.7 (15.5) | 136.3 (16.4) | <0.001 | ||||||
2-back: Working Memory Updating | Acc | 79.8 (3) | 85.1 (3.7) | 82 (2.9) | 86.5 (3.6) | 81.1 (3) | 77.8 (3.6) | 0.038 | 0.032 | 0.052 | |||
RT | 462.9 (18.5) | 424.1 (16.6) | 455.1 (18) | 414 (16.19 | 461.3 (18.2) | 428.9 (16.3) | <0.001 | ||||||
0-back: Alertness | Acc | 96.8 (2.3) | 100 (0.1) | 97.1 (2.2) | 99.9 (0.1) | 98.2 (2.2) | 99.6 (0.1) | 0.062 | |||||
RT | 299.7 (10.7) | 297.2 (7.8) | 310 (10.4) | 299.7 (7.6) | 306.3 (10.5) | 309.7 (7.7) | |||||||
TMT-A | Acc | 97.2 (0.8) | 97.1 (1) | 96.6 (0.8) | 96.1 (0.9) | 94.9 (0.8) | 96.8 (0.9) | ||||||
Time | 44,927.9 (2043.2) | 43,797.9 (2197.9) | 46,592.7 (1984) | 41,613.3 (2134.2) | 50,835.2 (2012.9) | 46,244 (2165.4) | 0.006 | ||||||
TMT-B | Acc | 95.2 (2) | 95 (1.4) | 91.7 (1.9) | 93.5 (1.3) | 92.9 (1.9) | 94.4 (1.4) | ||||||
Time | 54,005.8 (4760.2) | 56,449.1 (4203.2) | 50,686.1 (4622.2) | 55,600.7 (4081.3) | 42,115.4 (4689.7) | 45,767.5 (4140.9) | 0.055 | ||||||
Immediate Recall | Acc | 74.5 (3.2) | 84.8 (3) | 80.6 (3.1) | 80 (2.9) | 76.2 (3.1) | 74.7 (3) | 0.099 | 0.007 | 0.007 | 0.013 | ||
Delayed Recall | Acc | 67.7 (3.6) | 77 (3.4) | 69.7 (3.5) | 76.3 (3.3) | 66.8 (3.6) | 69.4 (3.3) | 0.002 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kardys, C.; Küper, K.; Getzmann, S.; Falkenstein, M.; Voelcker-Rehage, C. A Comparison of the Effects of Short-Term Physical and Combined Multi-Modal Training on Cognitive Functions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 7506. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127506
Kardys C, Küper K, Getzmann S, Falkenstein M, Voelcker-Rehage C. A Comparison of the Effects of Short-Term Physical and Combined Multi-Modal Training on Cognitive Functions. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(12):7506. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127506
Chicago/Turabian StyleKardys, Claudia, Kristina Küper, Stephan Getzmann, Michael Falkenstein, and Claudia Voelcker-Rehage. 2022. "A Comparison of the Effects of Short-Term Physical and Combined Multi-Modal Training on Cognitive Functions" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 12: 7506. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127506