Next Article in Journal
Water-Use Characteristics and Physiological Response of Moso Bamboo to Flash Droughts
Next Article in Special Issue
A Systematic Review of Polyvictimization among Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity or Autism Spectrum Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Effects of Pb(II) and Cr(VI) Stress on Phosphate-Solubilizing Bacteria (Bacillus sp. Strain MRP-3): Oxidative Stress and Bioaccumulation Potential
Previous Article in Special Issue
Examining the Impact of Victimization on Girls’ Delinquency: A Study of Direct and Indirect Effects
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cyberbullying in Gifted Students: Prevalence and Psychological Well-Being in a Spanish Sample

by
Joaquín González-Cabrera
1,
Javier Tourón
1,
Juan Manuel Machimbarrena
2,*,
Mónica Gutiérrez-Ortega
1,
Aitor Álvarez-Bardón
1 and
Maite Garaigordobil
2
1
Faculty of Education, Universidad Internacional de La Rioja (UNIR), Avenida de la Paz, 137, 26006 Logroño, La Rioja, Spain
2
Faculty of Psychology, University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU), Avenida de Tolosa, 70, 20018 Donostia, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16(12), 2173; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122173
Submission received: 9 May 2019 / Revised: 17 June 2019 / Accepted: 18 June 2019 / Published: 19 June 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Child Victimisation)

Abstract

:
The differential characteristics of gifted students can make them vulnerable to cyberbullying. There is very little empirical evidence about cyberbullying and giftedness. In the Spanish context, it is unexplored. The main goal of this work is to determine the prevalence of cyberbullying, its distribution in the different roles, and its relationship with other psychological variables. A cross-sectional study was performed with 255 gifted students (M = 11.88 years, SD = 2.28 years) in Spain (155 males, 60.8%). We used the cyberbullying test and the Spanish versions of the DASS-21, ISEL, KIDSCREEN-10, and the SWLS. The results indicate that 25.1% of the students are pure-cybervictims, 3.9% pure-cyberbullies, and 6.6% cyberbully-victims. Pure-cybervictims and cyberbully-victims present worse scores (p < 0.001) in health-related quality of life, depression, life satisfaction and stress than the uninvolved individuals. The results suggest that the gifted sample presents more cybervictimization and less cyberbullying than observed in other studies of the general population.

1. Introduction

1.1. Cyberbullying: Definition, Prevalence, Consequences and Parental Control

Increased access to the internet has been accompanied by an increase of online bullying or cyberbullying. Cyberbullying is defined as conscious and repetitive harm inflicted through the use of computers, mobile phones, or other electronic devices [1]. Together with this definition, there is also the possibility of performing these actions at any time and from any place, as well as the amplifying effect of any action through the social networks [2]. Regarding its prevalence, a Spanish study carried out with a representative sample triangulating the three roles involved in cyberbullying (cybervictim, cyberaggressor, and cyberbystanding) found a 28.2% prevalence of cybervictims, but only 3% of them were pure-cybervictims [3]. In addition, in a survey involving 24 European and Latin American countries, the World Health Organization [4] declared that Spain is one of the countries with the highest rate of cyberbullying. Overall, several studies have concluded that between 20% and 72% of young people have experienced at least one episode of cyberbullying, either as victims or as bullies [2,5,6]. Specifically, a systematic review of Spanish cyberbullying studies [7] found a prevalence of up to 78% for cybervictimization and up to 56% for cyberbullying.
Cyberbullying is associated with emotional and psychosocial problems and can lead to suicidal ideation [8,9,10]. It is also associated with internalizing and externalizing problems [11], as well as depression [12], stress and anxiety [13,14], and alterations in the cortisol release pattern [15]. Cyberbullying also has an impact on general aspects of subjective well-being such as satisfaction with life or happiness [16,17]. More specifically, there is evidence that the roles associated with victimization and cybervictimization present a significant loss of health-related quality of life (HRQoL) [18,19,20]. Other studies have linked low social support to victimization [21], although few studies have examined this variable in cyberbullying [11]. Moreover, the consequences of cyberbullying increase when these behaviors are maintained over long periods of time [22].
In relation to parental control, studies carried out to date suggest that behavioral control of children constitutes a protective factor against various internet risks [23,24]. Low and Espelage [25] found that parental supervision was associated with less involvement in cyberbullying. Other authors have shown that supervising smartphone usage is associated with a significant reduction in cyberbullying behavior [3]. Similar results have been found regarding various risks related to online grooming of minors (see, for example, Whittle and colleagues [26]). This is in line with the findings that cyberaggression-cybervictimization behaviors are related to family conflicts and poor parent-child communication [27].

1.2. Giftedness

Pfeiffer [28], according to his tripartite model, proposes an operational definition of intellectually gifted students in these terms: “(…) compared to others of their age, experience, and opportunities, gifted children are more likely to attain outstanding accomplishments in one or more of the culturally valued domains" (p. 23). Referring specifically to the most academically capable students, he states: "academically gifted students demonstrate outstanding performance, or evidence of potential for outstanding academic performance, in comparison with other students of the same age, experience, and opportunities (...) and a thirst for excelling in one or more areas of academic competition. (...)” [28] (p. 24). Consistent with a developmental approach, we note the growing importance of nonintellectual variables that contribute decisively to establish gifted youngsters’ successful trajectories. In general, gifted students not only show quantitative differences with their age peers, they also show qualitative differences that make them more or less different, such as unusual sensitivity, perfectionism, increased awareness of ethical and moral issues, intense bonds, or early development of internal locus of control, motivation, passion for learning, outstanding academic performance, etc. [29,30]. This sometimes entails becoming a vulnerable and “easier” target for other students who are not different from others. In this sense, some studies have shown that minority groups (for cultural, ethnic, religious or any other reason) are more vulnerable [7,31], drawing on theories like that of social identity [32] and considering that individuals tend to favor their own group (ingroup) while discriminating against others (outgroup). Such distancing from the outgroup has even been found in four- and five-year-olds [33] and could serve as a justification for intimidation simply because a classmate is not a member of the ingroup [34], even encouraging aggression [35]. Therefore, the peculiarities and characteristics of potentially gifted students could increase the odds of them being seen as different, with an outstanding eagerness to learn, exceptional performance, skills that allow them to ask and answer complex questions and excel in schoolwork, etc. This turns them into the target of aggression and bullying, particularly when their strengths do not coincide with the fashionable stereotypes of their age group. This can also occur with above-average students who are not officially identified as gifted, but are recognized by their peers as such [36].

1.3. Cyberbullying and Giftedness

Currently, there are very few studies on bullying and giftedness in the international setting (especially in the United States), and the data are contradictory [37]. This was shown by Peterson and Ray [38,39] who found that a high percentage (67%) of gifted students have experienced bullying. On another hand, several studies suggest that this group is similar to the general population in prevalence of bullying [40,41,42] or even presents lower levels of involvement [43,44]. However, there are no current data for Spain or other Spanish-speaking countries.
Studies of gifted student populations are almost non-existent in the field of cyberbullying, as we only found one study of the prevalence of cyberbullying in gifted students. Mitchell [45] found 5% of cybervictims, 4% of cyberbullies, and 17% of cyberbully-victims among gifted students from fifth to eight grade.
Drawing on this, the main goal of this work is to determine the prevalence of school cyberbullying in a sample of Spanish gifted students and its distribution according to the different roles. Secondary objectives are: (1) to analyze possible differences in relation to the variables sex and age in cyberbullying and the other study variables; (2) to analyze the impact on perceived HRQoL, life satisfaction, depression, and stress of students depending on the cyberbullying role performed; (3) to relate cyberbullying to anxiety, depression, stress, perceived social support, HRQoL, and life satisfaction; and (4) to compare the scores on cybervictimization and cyberbullying depending on general use of the smartphone and parental control over it.
The hypotheses of this study are: (1) the most prevalent role in the sample of gifted students will be that of cybervictim, similar to the general student population [5,7,46]; (2) the percentage of gifted students in the role of cybervictim will be greater than that found in other studies of the general student population [38,39]; (3) differences as a function of sex and age will be similar to those in general student population [3,5,7,47]; (4) students involved in cyberbullying will suffer more psychological impact in the studied variables than students who are uninvolved [8,10,14]; and (5) students who report parental control will obtain lower scores in cybervictimization and cyberbullying than students without such parental control [3].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The total number of participants was initially 323 pre-adolescents and adolescents, which, after the initial screening of the database, included 255 adolescents diagnosed as gifted and officially recognized by public institutions (155 males, 60.8%, and 100 females, 39.2%). The mean and standard deviation of age was 11.88 ± 2.28 years with a range of 9–18 years. Concerning educational level, 107 students were in primary education (42.8%), 112 were in compulsory secondary education (44.8%), and 12 were in high school (12.4%).
As in many other countries, the population of gifted students in Spain presents some peculiarities that should be highlighted. Among them, their geographic and school dispersion, the diversity of identification criteria, the lack of systematic detection processes leading to infra-identification, among others [48]. According to the Ministry of Education of Spain, there were 27,133 gifted students in 2017 (last available year). It must therefore be kept in mind that, with reference to the situation of identification in Spain, it is impossible to ensure that the identified students are in any way a representative sample of all the potentially gifted Spanish students. However, the number of students who responded is reasonably large enough to attain a first approximation to the prevalence of giftedness in Spain for this population. If the number of participants who responded to this study had been selected in a strictly random way, the margin of error with a 95% confidence level would be 6.1% for the estimation of proportions.
The sampling was non-probabilistic and incidental. Initially, the researchers contacted 40 associations related to gifted students throughout all the regions of Spain. Of them, 22 agreed to participate, disseminating the study among their affiliates (legal guardians of the surveyed children). The participating associations are scattered around 17 Spanish autonomous communities. We estimate that the documentation was sent to more than 3000 families. In a second phase, we sent the relevant documentation to the legal managers of the associations to send to their members (legal guardians of gifted children) to be able to participate in the study.

2.2. Instruments

First, the gifted students responded to questions on sociodemographic variables such as sex, age, grade, association to which their families belonged, and province in which they lived. Other socio-family questions focused on whether their parents limited their use of the smartphone or other devices with internet connection, and whether their parents supervised their actions on these devices. Additionally, we asked about the weekday (Monday to Friday) and weekend usage of the mobile phone.
We used the psychological assessment tools described below:
The Cyberbullying Screening of Peer Harassment (technological scale) [5,49].
This tool allows one to triangulate each subject for his/her role in three profiles related to cyberbullying: cybervictim, cyberbully, and cyberbystander. This self-report tool consists of a total of 45 items (15 for each profile) and collects the most significant behaviors associated with cyberbullying. It complies with the recommendations of Berne et al.’s assessment tools review [50], such as providing appropriate indicators of validity (e.g., exploratory or confirmatory factor analyses) and reliability (e.g., dimensional reliability indicators). It uses a 4-point Likert scale (0 = never, 1 = sometimes, 2 = fairly often, and 3 = always), and we coded those who suffered, performed, or observed any cyberbullying behavior "sometimes" as occasional cybervictims, cyberaggressors or cyberbystanders. We considered cyberbullying as severe when the participant suffered, performed, or observed cyberbullying behavior from "fairly often" to "always.” In the study sample, the subscales of cybervictim, cyberbully, and cyberbystander had the following alpha values: α = 0.82, α = 0.72, and α = 0.89, respectively. The Omega coefficient values were, respectively, 0.82, 0.72, and 0.90. Additionally, we added a question addressed to the cyberbystanders, based on the participant role scale (PRS) [51], which allows categorizing the type of cyberbystander as: (a) helps the cyberbully (never starts the bullying, but sometimes participates, supporting the bully; (b) reinforces the cyberbully (sympathizes with the bully, but never directly participates with him/her); (c) uninvolved (is neutral when an aggression occurs); (d) pro-cybervictim (is in favor of the victim, but does nothing to prevent the bullying); and (e) defender (usually actively defends the victim and helps him/her as much as possible).
The Spanish version of Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) [52]. This 5-item scale evaluates, on a single dimension, the individual’s global judgment of satisfaction with life. It presents adequate indices of reliability and validity. In addition, it is appropriately related to other variables of satisfaction in the school setting, feelings of happiness, and feelings of loneliness. The Cronbach alpha and Omega coefficients were 0.89 and 0.90, respectively, for this sample.
The Spanish version of the Depression, Anxiety, Stress Scales-21 [53]. This self-report assesses negative moods during the past week. It consists of 21 statements that are rated on a 4-point Likert scale, distributed in three subscales with seven items each: depression, anxiety, and stress. In this study, we used the adapted and validated Spanish version by Daza et al. [54]. It has shown evidence of convergent and divergent validity with other self-reports of depression, anxiety, and psychopathology, as well as a three-dimensional solution that empirically supports the proposed subscales [54]. For the subscales of depression, and anxiety, and stress, it presented alpha values of α = 0.90, α = 0.86, and α = 0.91, respectively, in this sample. The Omega coefficient values were, respectively, 0.91, 0.87, and 0.91.
The Spanish version of the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (ISEL) [55]. This questionnaire assesses respondents’ perceived availability of potential social resources. It consists of four subscales, but we only used the subscales of informative support and sense of belonging. The version used in this research was adapted to Spanish population by Trujillo-Mendoza et al. [56] and it has adequate indices of reliability and validity. The Cronbach alphas for the two dimensions were 0.74 and 0.76, respectively, in this sample. The Omega coefficient values were, respectively, 0.86 and 0.82.
The Spanish version of the KIDSCREEN-10 [57]. This questionnaire assesses HRQoL in children and adolescents from eight to 18 years, from the global perspective of the World Health Organization (WHO), by means of an inclusive (bio-psycho-social) viewpoint. This version presents a global dimension of perceived quality of life. It has suitable indices of reliability and internal validity and is also standardized to Spanish population. The Cronbach alpha and Omega coefficient values were 0.80 and 0.88, respectively, for this sample.

2.3. Procedure

This analytical and cross-sectional study was performed between June and October 2017. The measuring instruments were applied to the students online under the supervision of the respective families. The communication process, described in the previous section, was channeled through a specific website created for this study [hidden for reasons of blind review]. We informed the legal guardians about the research team carrying out the project, the purpose of the study, assessment tools, time frame, etc. Before accessing the online form, the legal guardians had to acknowledge that they had been informed of all the above-mentioned points and agree to the participation of their children in the study. The online platform we used was Survey Monkey®. The average time needed to complete the questionnaires was 25 min. The collaboration of the legal guardians and minors was voluntary, anonymous, and disinterested. The project was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Principality of Asturias (Ref.41/17).
The inclusion criteria for the study were as follows: (a) to have an official diagnosis of giftedness; (b) to belong to one of the associations of giftedness officially registered in Spain, (c) to be of school age and enrolled between fifth grade of primary education and second grade of high school; (d) to have access to the battery of questionnaires prior authorization of the legal guardian. There were no exclusion criteria. As it was an online procedure, and we had no real proof of the exact number of people who had received or read the invitation emails, we cannot cite a specific number of rejections.

2.4. Data Analysis

The following analyses were conducted: (1) confirmation of the assumption of normality of the variables involved in the study (Shapiro-Wilks statistic) as well as the variance homogeneity for group comparisons (Levene test); (2) analysis of frequencies and measures of central tendency and dispersion of the instrument; (3) calculation of standardized scores for variables that established relations; (4) chi-square statistic (χ2) for the contrast of proportions, and Student’s t-test for independent samples for the contrast of mean differences. In cases where statistically significant differences were found, we calculated the Cohen’s d to provide an estimate of the effect size of the difference; (5) partial correlations controlling for age; and (6) analysis of variance with Games-Howel post-hoc comparisons; the value of p < 0.05 was considered significant (we calculated the Eta-square (η2) to provide an estimate of the effect size of the difference). Statistical analyses were carried out using the program Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 23 (IBM®).

3. Results

3.1. Differences as a Function of Sex and Group

There were no significant differences in the total scores of any cyberbullying dimension as a function of the variable sex: cybervictimization (t = 0.893, p = 0.373), cyberbullying (t = 0.312, p = 0.755), and cyberbystanding (t = 0.156, p = 0.876). Regarding the dimensions of the DASS-21, no differences were obtained according to the variable sex: depression (t = 0.258, p = 0.797), anxiety (t = 0.068, p = 0.946), and stress (t = 0.521, p = 0.603). The same trend applied to the rest of the dimensions: HRQoL (t = 0.253, p = 0.818), informative support (t = 0.419, p = 0.832), sense of belonging (t = 0.999, p = 0.319), and life satisfaction (t = 0.510, p = 0.978).
There were significant age differences in some dimensions: cybervictimization (F9,243 = 7.282, p < 0.001; η = 0.212), cyberbullying (F9,243 = 3.837, p < 0.001; η2 = 0.124), cyberbystanding (F9,243 = 4.476, p < 0.001; η2 = 0.142), HRQoL (F9,243 = 3.729, p < 0.001; η2 = 0.121), life satisfaction (F9,243 = 3.070, p = 0.002; η2 = 0.102), and depression (F9,243 = 2.270, p = 0.019; η2 = 0.078). Post-hoc Games-Howel comparisons indicated differences in the prior constructs between the scores of the group of 9-10-year-olds and the 17-18-year-olds (p < 0.001), with the latter group obtaining higher mean scores.

3.2. Cyberbullying Profiles and Relationships Between Variables

Next, we calculated the number of pupils involved in the different cyberbullying roles. As can be seen in Table 1, the most prevalent role was that of the cyberbystander (40.8%). On the other hand, 31.8% had suffered cyberbullying behavior, whereas only 10.6% had perpetrated it. We found no significant differences as a function of sex.
In addition, for analytical purposes, we created the following four mutually exclusive categories on the basis of prior categorization: pure-cybervictims (have been occasional or severe cybervictims, but have not perpetrated cyberbullying), pure-cyberbullies (have performed either occasional or severe acts of aggressive behavior, but have never been a victim), cyberbully-victims (have been cybervictims and cyberbullies occasionally or severely), and uninvolved (have not participated either as cybervictim or as cyberbully). The prevalence of these categories was 25.1% (n = 64) for pure-cybervictims, 3.9% (n = 10) for pure-cyberbullies, 6.6% (n = 17) for cyberbully-victims, and 64.3% (n = 164) of the sample was uninvolved.
Table 2 shows the various subroles related to the role of the cyberbystander based on the categorization proposed by Salmivalli et al. [51]. There were no significant differences in the distributions of the subroles a function of sex (χ2 = 4.349, p = 0.226).
Table 3 shows the partial correlations (controlling for age) of all dimensions of the study. As can be seen, in general, cybervictimization correlated significantly and negatively with HRQoL (r = −0.242, p < 0.001) and life satisfaction (r = −0.279, p < 0.001), and positively with cyberbullying (r = 0.432, p < 0.001), depression (r = 0.325, p < 0.001) or stress (r = 0.361, p < 0.001), among others.

3.3. Differences in Depression, Anxiety, and Stress, Quality of Life, and Social Support as a Function of the Role Performed

Comparisons of the role performed for each of the constructs of study can be seen in Table 4. There were differences in all the constructs (p < 0.001) as a function of the four roles. In general, the “uninvolved” role systematically presented the highest scores (high scores in quality of life, sense of belonging, and life satisfaction) and low scores in depression, anxiety, and stress. Cybervictims, and especially cyberbully-victims, had significantly lower scores in quality of life, life satisfaction, and social support, as well as higher scores in depression and stress. It should be noted that, despite there being significant differences in anxiety only between cybervictims and uninvolved, cyberbullies obtained the highest total score.

3.4. Use of the Smartphone and Differences in Relation to Parental Control

The results regarding the possession and use of the mobile phone and scores in cybervictimization and cyberaggression are presented in Table 5. It can be seen that children who used the smartphone for five hours per day or more presented significantly higher scores in cybervictimization. These differences were also observed in the case of cyberaggression, although without reaching significance.
Table 6 shows the results of scores in cybervictimization and cyberaggression as a function of parental control measures (supervision and time limitation). Although there were no significant differences in the socio-family variables, there was a trend in the reduction of cybervictimization and cyberbullying behaviors when parental control was reported. Additionally, we calculated partial correlations (controlling for age) between cybervictimization behaviors and the number of weekday and week-end hours of mobile phone use (r = 0.149, p = 0.024 and r = 0.161, p = 0.015, respectively).

4. Discussion

The main objective of this study was to analyze the prevalence of cyberbullying in a sample of gifted students. The results showed that 31.5% of the sample is related to the cybervictim profile and 10.6% to the cyberbully profile. This confirms the first hypothesis, that the role of cybervictim is the most prevalent in cyberbullying. These results point in the same direction as the study of Dalosto and Alencar [46], where it was found that gifted students suffered more bullying and performed it less frequently. In addition, when dividing the gifted students into three mutually exclusive categories of involvement, it was observed that almost two thirds of the students who had committed some aggression belonged to the cyberbully-victim category. This suggests that much of the aggression committed may be motivated by prior victimization. Regarding prevalence, the results of our study coincide with other international works like that carried out by Connel et al. [58], who found a prevalence of 25% for cybervictims and of 13.8% for cyberbullies. However, other studies in the general population suggest that the roles of cyberbully-victim [59,60] or of cybervictim-cyberbully-cyberbystander [3] are the most prevalent.
In order to compare the results of this study with those of the general student population, we analyzed various works that used the same tool (The Cyberbullying Test). Garaigordobil [61], in a sample of 3026 participants aged 12 to 18, found that 30.2% of the participants had been cybervictims one or more times in the last year, 15.5% had been cyberbullies, and 65.1% had been cyberbystanders. On the other hand, González-Cabrera et al. [15] found 34.5% of cybervictims, 23.5% of cyberbullies, and 23.5% of cyberbystanders in a sample of students with a mean age of 14.89 years. These data are very similar to those reported in Table 1 in the area of cybervictimization, although they are lower for cyberbullying. However, it is important to note the difference in age between this study (11.88 years mean age) and the afore-mentioned ones (>14 years mean age) because cyberbullying behaviors increase with age, reaching a peak from 14 to 16 years, approximately [62,63]. The most approximate study in age is that of Machimbarrena and Garaigordobil [63], with a mean age of 10.68 years, which reported globally (one or more times in the last year) 13.4% of pure-cybervictims, 0.7% of pure-cyberbullies, and 3.1% of cyberbully-victims. Direct comparison with this study seems to indicate that gifted students present slightly higher prevalence rates in cybervictimization than general student population and also a smaller number of cyberbullies. All this suggests the confirmation of the second hypothesis (the percentage of gifted students in the role of cybervictim will be greater than that found in other studies with general population).
When comparing the results of this study with other studies carried out with different assessment tools in general population, the results of cybervictimization are slightly higher for the gifted group [3,60,64,65]. Studies with representative samples also report lower percentages of cybervictimization, such as the works of Save the Children [66] and González-Cabrera et al. [67]. These data support the second hypothesis of the study (gifted students will present a higher percentage in the role of cybervictim than those found in other studies with general student population).
With regard to the single article on cyberbullying and giftedness that we found, the data contrast with those obtained by Mitchell [45], who suggests that gifted students are less likely to be cybervictims or cyberbullies. The differences between the two works could be due to the fact that Mitchell classified the students based on self-reports (without an external diagnosis by a specialist team) and the time bracket between 2011 and 2018 is very broad for issues related to the internet, as these years have brought about great changes in the possession and use of smartphones, as well as in time spent and use made of the Internet by young people.
Regarding sex differences, there were no differences in any of the dimensions of cyberbullying. This evidence is shared by several other studies [21,68,69]. However, there were significant differences consistent with findings showing that cyberbullying increases with age [63,69,70]. All this seems to confirm the third hypothesis, which suggested that differences as a function of sex and age would be similar to those of other studies with general student population.
Comparisons among the three involved roles and the uninvolved participants reveal that cybervictims and cyberbully-victims present significantly worse scores than the uninvolved participants in depression, anxiety, stress, sense of belonging and informative support, and HRQoL. All this confirms the fourth hypothesis concerning the greater psychological impact of the roles associated with cybervictimization. These findings are consistent with several studies that found a relation between victimization and depression in the general population [12], cyberbullying and stress and anxiety [13,15], poor quality of life in children suffering from bullying [18,19,20], and low social support [21]. These data are of particular concern for the gifted group of students, because the high prevalence of victimization and its psychological impact occur at an early age. It is important for public collectives and policy makers to take this situation into account, because cybervictimization can generate harmful long-term consequences [71,72].
The results do not yield significant differences in cybervictimization and cyberbullying as a function of parental control, so we cannot confirm the last hypothesis (students who report parental control will obtain lower scores of cybervictimization and cyberbullying than students without such parental control). However, there does seem to be a tendency to present lower mean scores in students who report parents’ limitation of the use of the smartphone and parental control of their actions (see Table 5 and Table 6). Moreover, other studies along these lines suggest that parental control is a protective factor against cyberaggression behaviors [3]. Other authors such as Low and Espelage [25] found that parental supervision was associated with lower involvement in cyberbullying. On the other hand, Ang et al. [23] found that parents’ knowledge of their children’s activities on the internet was associated with less problematic internet use.
This study has several practical implications: (a) the prevalence found suggests the need to reinforce the strategies of coexistence with students who present an educational singularity like, in this case, giftedness; (b) those responsible for schools that have gifted students enrolled should also ensure that they are adequately integrated and be alert to specific aggressions or bullying, because gifted students are often reluctant to report these experiences [73]; (c) in general, cyberbullying situations may be added to other problems, if any, due to the condition of giftedness, which should be taken into account by the educational teams and the families; (d) reduction of cyberbullying should be included in current existing programs (e.g., Cyberprogram 2.0 [74], "Asegúrate" [Make sure] [75] or Prev@cib [76] in Spain, or modules of specific activities for singular groups such as gifted students. These actions could also be extended to students who excel intellectually, even if they are not officially recognized as gifted [36].
This work also has some limitations. Firstly, the use of self-reports, with the entailed bias of social desirability. This could be improved in the future with complementary measures (reports by parents, teachers, and peers) with which to triangulate a more complete view. A second limitation is the use of cross-sectional methodology and incidental sampling. However, these initiatives in developing sciences as well as the recruitment of difficult-to-access population subgroups through adaptation of sampling strategies are common [74]. In addition, the sampling carried out provides an advantage, as it is a homogeneous convenience sample, which allows for an estimation of the results with clearer generalizability towards the gifted collective [77]. In general, this procedure was the only way to gain access to a largely dispersed population whose localization is complex. Therefore, an equivalent control group would have been extremely complex, if not unfeasible, but this was overcome by employing assessment tools that are widely used in the general population, and that are also standardized. In addition, extrapolation of these results should be done cautiously, as they are to be considered a first approximation to the reality of cyberbullying and giftedness in Spain. Data have been obtained from students who are members of associations, but access to many others who are not members is extremely difficult. Future studies should replicate these findings with additional samples in Spain and in other countries. Students who stand out intellectually over the average but who have no official recognition as gifted should also be incorporated, as a possible line of future research.

5. Conclusions

This work provides empirical evidence of a prevalence of 31.6% of cybervictimization in a group of gifted students, even though 38.4% do not own a mobile phone, and the mean age is lower than the habitual age in studies of cyberbullying (focusing on adolescents). These prevalence rates, coupled with an important psychological impact, reveal a situation of vulnerability in this group. Therefore, special attention is required of teachers, who must take this reality into account with regard to support that should be provided to students with specific educational needs. In addition, it is important for institutions and associations linked to these groups to generate plans for cyberbullying prevention and specific actions to improve the situation of this population and the rest of the school population in general.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.G.-C., J.T., J.M.M. and M.G.; formal analysis, J.G.-C. and J.M.M.; funding acquisition, J.G.-C. and J.T.; investigation, J.G.-C., J.T., J.M.M., M.M.-G. and A.A.-B.; methodology, J.G.-C.; project administration, J.G.-C. and J.T.; supervision, J.G.-C., J.T. and M.G.; writing—original draft, J.G.-C., J.T. and J.M.M.; writing—review & editing, J.G.-C., J.T., J.M.M., M.M.-G., A.A.-B. and M.G.

Funding

This research was funded by Spanish Ministry of Economy, Industry and Competitiveness, RTI2018-094212-B-I00: Bullying and cyberbullying among gifted students: from prevalence to intervention (CIBER-AACC). This research was funded by Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, Project “Cyberpsychology (Bienio 2017-19)”.

Acknowledgments

To thank the participants as well as the legal guardians of the associations of gifted students. Also thank the representatives of the participating associations.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Hinduja, S.; Patchin, J.W. Bullying Beyond the Schoolyard: Preventing and Responding to Cyberbullying; SAGE Publications Sage (Corwin Press): Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  2. Tokunaga, R.S. Following You Home from School: A Critical Review and Synthesis of Research on Cyberbullying Victimization. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2010, 26, 277–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. González-Cabrera, J.M.; León-Mejía, A.; Machimbarrena, J.M.; Balea, A.; Calvete, E. Psychometric properties of the cyberbullying triangulation questionnaire: A prevalence analysis through seven roles. Scand. J. Psychol. 2019, 60, 160–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Organization, W.H. Growing Up Unequal: Gender and Socioeconomic Differences in Young People’s Health and Well-Being; WHO Regional Office for Europe: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  5. Garaigordobil, M. Cyberbullying. Screening de Acoso Entre Iguales [Cyberbullying. Screening of Peer Harassment]; TEA: Madrid, Spain, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  6. Modecki, K.L.; Minchin, J.; Harbaugh, A.G.; Guerra, N.G.; Runions, K.C. Bullying prevalence across contexts: A meta-analysis measuring cyber and traditional bullying. J. Adolesc. Health 2014, 55, 602–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Zych, I.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Marin-Lopez, I. Cyberbullying: A Systematic Review of Research, its Prevalence, and Assessment Ossues in Spanish Studies. Psicol. Educ. 2016, 22, 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Gámez-Guadix, M.; Orue, I.; Smith, P.K.; Calvete, E. Longitudinal and reciprocal relations of cyberbullying with depression, substance use, and problematic internet use among adolescents. J. Adolesc. Health 2013, 53, 446–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Kowalski, R.M.; Giumetti, G.W.; Schroeder, A.N.; Lattanner, M.R. Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth. Psychol. Bull. 2014, 140, 1073–1137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Iranzo, B.; Buelga, S.; Cava, M.J.; Ortega-Barón, J. Cyberbullying, Psychosocial Adjustment, and Suicidal Ideation in Adolescence. Psychosoc. Interv. 2019, 28, 75–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Fisher, B.W.; Gardella, J.H.; Teurbe-Tolon, A.R. Peer Cybervictimization Among Adolescents and the Associated Internalizing and Externalizing Problems: A Meta-Analysis. J. Youth Adolesc. 2016, 45, 1727–1743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Tsitsika, A.; Janikian, M.; Wójcik, S.; Makaruk, K.; Tzavela, E.; Tzavara, C.; Greydanus, D.; Merrick, J.; Richardson, C. Cyberbullying victimization prevalence and associations with internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents in six European countries. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 51, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Aoyama, I.; Barnard-Brak, L.; Talbert, T. Cyberbullying Among High School Students: Cluster Analysis of Sex and Age Differences and the Level of Parental Monitoring. Int. J. Cyber Behav. Psychol. Learn. 2011, 1, 25–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Garaigordobil, M.; Machimbarrena, J.M. Victimization and Perpetration of Bullying/Cyberbullying: Connections with Emotional and Behavioral Problems and Childhood Stress. Psychosoc. Interv. 2019, 28, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. González-Cabrera, J.; Calvete, E.; León-Mejía, A.; Pérez-Sancho, C.; Peinado, J.M. Relationship between cyberbullying roles, cortisol secretion and psychological stress. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 70, 153–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Moore, P.M.; Huebner, E.S.; Hills, K.J. Electronic Bullying and Victimization and Life Satisfaction in Middle School Students. Soc. Indic. Res. 2012, 107, 429–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Navarro, R.; Ruiz-Oliva, R.; Larrañaga, E.; Yubero, S. The Impact of Cyberbullying and Social Bullying on Optimism, Global and School-Related Happiness and Life Satisfaction Among 10–12-year-old Schoolchildren. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 2013, 10, 15–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Chester, K.L.; Spencer, N.H.; Whiting, L.; Brooks, F.M. Association Between Experiencing Relational Bullying and Adolescent Health-Related Quality of Life. J. Sch. Health 2017, 87, 865–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. González-Cabrera, J.; León-Mejía, A.; Beranuy, M.; Gutiérrez-Ortega, M.; Alvarez-Bardón, A.; Machimbarrena, J.M. Relationship between cyberbullying and health-related quality of life in a sample of children and adolescents. Qual. Life Res. 2018, 27, 2609–2618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Hidalgo-Rasmussen, C.; Molina, T.; Molina, R.; Sepúlveda, R.; Martínez, V.; Montaño, R.; González, E.; George, M. Influence of bullying on the quality of life perception of Chilean students. Rev. Med. Chile 2015, 143, 716–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Williams, K.R.; Guerra, N.G. Prevalence and Predictors of Internet Bullying. J. Adolesc. Health 2007, 41, 14–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Evans, C.B.R.; Smokowski, P.R.; Cotter, K.L. Cumulative bullying victimization: An investigation of the dose-response relationship between victimization and the associated mental health outcomes, social supports, and school experiences of rural adolescents. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2014, 44, 256–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ang, R.P.; Chong, W.H.; Chye, S.; Huan, V.S. Loneliness and generalized problematic Internet use: Parents’ perceived knowledge of adolescents’ online activities as a moderator. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2012, 28, 1342–1347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Goh, W.W.L.; Bay, S.; Chen, V.H.H. Young school children’s use of digital devices and parental rules. Telemat. Inform. 2015, 32, 787–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Low, S.; Espelage, D. Differentiating cyber bullying perpetration from non-physical bullying: Commonalities across race, individual, and family predictors. Psychol. Violence 2013, 3, 39–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Whittle, H.; Hamilton-Giachritsis, C.; Beech, A.; Collings, G. A review of online grooming: Characteristics and concerns. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2013, 18, 62–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Buelga, S.; Iranzo, B.; Cava, M.J.; Torralba, E. Perfil psicosocial de adolescentes agresores de cyberbullying. Rev. Psicol. Soc. 2015, 30, 382–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Pfeiffer, S.I. Identificación y Evaluación del Alumnado con Altas Capacidades [Identification and Assessment of the Gifted Students]; UNIR Editorial: Madrid, Spain, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  29. Peterson, J.S. Myth 17: Gifted and Talented Individuals Do Not Have Unique Social and Emotional Needs. Gift. Child Q. 2009, 53, 280–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Silverman, L.K. Asynchronous development. In The Social and Emotional Development of Gifted Children: What Do We Know; Neihart, M., Reis, S.M., Robinson, N.M., Moon, S.M., Eds.; Prufrock Press: Waco, TX, USA, 2002; pp. 31–37. [Google Scholar]
  31. Spanish UNICEF Comitee. Los Niños y Niñas de la Brecha Digital en España [Children of the Digital Gap in Spain]; UNICEF Comité Español: Madrid, Spain, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  32. Haslam, S.A.; Ellemers, N.; Reicher, S.D.; Reynolds, K.J.; Schmitt, M.T. The social identity perspective today: The impact of its defining ideas. In Rediscovering Social Identity: Core Sources; Postmes, T., Branscombe, N., Eds.; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  33. Oostenbroek, J.; Over, H. Young children contrast their behavior to that of out-group members. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2015, 139, 234–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Hymel, S.; McClure, R.; Miller, M.; Shumka, E.; Trach, J. Addressing school bullying: Insights from theories of group processes. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 37, 16–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Reijntjes, A.; Thomaes, S.; Kamphuis, J.H.; Bushman, B.J.; Reitz, E.; Telch, M.J. Youths’ displaced aggression against in- and out-group peers: An experimental examination. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2013, 115, 180–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Margolin, L. Gifted Education and the Matthew Effect. In Begabung und Gesellschaft; Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2018; pp. 165–182. [Google Scholar]
  37. Smith, B.W.; Dempsey, A.G.; Jackson, S.E.; Olenchak, F.R.; Gaa, J. Cyberbullying among gifted children. Gift. Educ. Int. 2012, 28, 112–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Peterson, J.S.; Ray, K.E. Gifted Child Quarterly Bullying among the Gifted: The Subjective Experience. Gift. Child Q. 2006, 50, 252–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Peterson, J.S.; Ray, K.E. Bullying and the gifted: Victims, perpetrators, prevalence, and effects. Gift. Child Q. 2006, 50, 148–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Oliveira, J.C.; Barbosa, A.J.G. Bullying entre Estudantes com e sem Características de Dotação e Talento. [Bullying among students with and without characteristics of giftedness]. Psicol. Reflexão e Crítica 2012, 25, 747–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Peters, M.P.; Bain, S.K. Bullying and Victimization Rates among Gifted and High-Achieving Students. J. Educ. Gift. 2011, 34, 624–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ryoo, J.H.; Wang, C.; Swearer, S.M.; Park, S. Investigation of transitions in bullying/victimization statuses of gifted and general education students. Except. Child. 2017, 83, 396–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Erwin, J. Prevalence and Impact of Peer Victimization Among Gifted Adolescents. Ph.D. Thesis, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  44. Estell, D.B.; Farmer, T.W.; Irvin, M.J.; Crowther, A.; Akos, P.; Boudah, D.J. Students with exceptionalities and the peer group context of bullying and victimization in late elementary school. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2009, 18, 136–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Mitchell, M.S. Cyberbullying and Academic Achievement: Research into the Rates of Incidence, Knowledge of Consequences, and Behavioral Patterns of Cyberbullying; University of Connecticut: Storrs, CT, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  46. Dalosto, M.M.; de Alencar, E.M.L.S. Manifestações e prevalência de bullyng entre alunos com altas habilidades/superdotação [Bullying manifestations and prevalence among gifted students]. Rev. Bras. Educ. Espec. 2013, 19, 363–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Brochado, S.; Soares, S.; Fraga, S. A Scoping Review on Studies of Cyberbullying Prevalence Among Adolescents. Trauma Violence Abus. 2017, 18, 523–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Tourón, J.; Tourón, M. Identification of Verbal and Mathematical Talent: The Relevance of “Out of Level” Measurement. An. Psicol. 2016, 32, 638–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Garaigordobil, M. Psychometric Properties of the Cyberbullying Test, a Screening Instrument to Measure Cybervictimization, Cyberaggression, and Cyberobservation. J. Interpers. Violence 2017, 32, 3556–3576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Berne, S.; Frisén, A.; Schultze-Krumbholz, A.; Scheithauer, H.; Naruskov, K.; Luik, P.; Katzer, C.; Erentaite, R.; Zukauskiene, R. Cyberbullying assessment instruments: A systematic review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2013, 18, 320–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Salmivalli, C.; Lagerspetz, K.; Björkqvist, K.; Österman, K.; Kaukiainen, A. Bullying as a group process: Participant roles and their relations to social status within the group. Aggress. Behav. 1996, 22, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Atienza, F.L.; Pons, D.; Balaguer, I.; García-Merita, M. Propiedades Psicométricas de la Escala de Satisfacción con la Vida en Adolescentes [Psychometric properties of the satisfaction with life scale in adolescents]. Psicothema 2000, 12, 314–319. [Google Scholar]
  53. Lovibond, S.H.; Lovibond, P.F. Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales, 2nd ed.; Psychology Foundation: Sydney, Australia, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  54. Daza, P.; Novy, D.M.; Stanley, M.A.; Averill, P. The depression anxiety stress scale-21: Spanish translation and validation with a Hispanic sample. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2002, 24, 195–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Cohen, S.; Wills, T.A. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol. Bull. 1985, 98, 310–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Trujillo-Mendoza, H.; Martos-Perales, F.J.; González-Cabrera, J. Adaptación al español del cuestionario Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (ISEL) [The Spanish Adaptation of Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (ISEL)]. Univ. Psychol. 2012, 11, 969–978. [Google Scholar]
  57. The KIDSCREEN Group Europe. The Kidscreen Questionnaires—Handbook; Pabst Science Publishers: Lengerich, Germany, 2006; ISBN 9783899673340. [Google Scholar]
  58. Connell, N.M.; Schell-Busey, N.M.; Pearce, A.N.; Negro, P. Badgrlz? Exploring Sex Differences in Cyberbullying Behaviors. Youth Violence Juv. Justice 2014, 12, 209–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Festl, R.; Vogelgesang, J.; Scharkow, M.; Quandt, T. Longitudinal patterns of involvement in cyberbullying: Results from a Latent Transition Analysis. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 66, 7–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Gámez-Guadix, M.; Gini, G.; Calvete, E. Stability of cyberbullying victimization among adolescents: Prevalence and association with bully-victim status and psychosocial adjustment. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 53, 140–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Garaigordobil, M. Cyberbullying in adolescents and youth in the Basque Country: Prevalence of cybervictims, cyberaggressors, and cyberobservers. J. Youth Stud. 2015, 18, 569–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Garaigordobil, M.; Martínez-Valderrey, V. Efectos del Cyberprogram 2.0 en el bullying “cara-a-cara”, el cyberbullying y la empatia. Psicothema 2015, 27, 45–51. [Google Scholar]
  63. Machimbarrena, J.M.; Garaigordobil, M. Acoso y ciberacoso en educación primaria. Behav. Psychol. Psicol. Conduct. 2018, 26, 263–280. [Google Scholar]
  64. García-Fernández, C.M.; Romera-Félix, E.M.; Ortega-Ruiz, R. Relaciones entre el bullying y el cyberbullying: Prevalencia y co-ocurrencia. Pensam. Psicol. 2016, 14, 86–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Navarro, R.; Yubero, S.; Larrañaga, E. Psychosocial Risk Factors for Involvement in Bullying Behaviors: Empirical Comparison Between Cyberbullying and Social Bullying Victims and Bullies. Sch. Ment. Health 2015, 7, 235–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Save the Children. Yo a Eso no Juego. Bullying y Ciberbullying en la Infancia [I Won’t Play That Game. Bullying and Cyberbullying]; Save the Children: London, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  67. González-Cabrera, J.; Balea-Vazquel, A.; Vallina-Paco, M.; Moya, A.; Laviana-Corte, F. Informe Ejecutivo del Proyecto Ciberastur [CYBERASTUR Executive Project Report]; Universidad Internacional de la Rioja (UNIR) y Consejería de Educación y Cultura del Principado de Asturias: Oviedo, Spain, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  68. Herrera-López, M.; Romera, E.; Ortega-Ruiz, R. Bullying y cyberbullying en Colombia; coocurrencia en adolescentes escolarizados [Bullying and cyberbullying in Colombia; Co-occurrence in adolescent students]. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 2017, 49, 163–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Mishna, F.; Khoury-Kassabri, M.; Gadalla, T.; Daciuk, J. Risk factors for involvement in cyber bullying: Victims, bullies and bully-victims. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2012, 34, 63–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Garaigordobil, M. Cyberbullying in adolescents and youth in the Basque Country: Changes with age. An. Psicol. 2015, 31, 1069–1076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Bannink, R.; Broeren, S.; Van De Looij-Jansen, P.M.; De Waart, F.G.; Raat, H. Cyber and traditional bullying victimization as a risk factor for mental health problems and suicidal ideation in adolescents. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, 11–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Reijntjes, A.; Kamphuis, J.H.; Prinzie, P.; Telch, M.J. Peer victimization and internalizing problems in children: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Child Abus. Negl. 2010, 34, 244–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Connolly, J.P. Exploring the Factors Influencing Gifted Adolescents’ Resistance to Report Experiences of Cyberbullying Behavior: Toward an Improved Understanding. J. Educ. Gift. 2018, 41, 136–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Garaigordobil, M.; Martínez-Valderrey, V. Impact of cyberprogram 2.0 on different types of school violence and aggressiveness. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Del Rey, R.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Casas, J.A. Asegúrate: An intervention program against cyberbullying based on teachers’ commitment and on design of its instructional materials. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Ortega-Barón, J.; Buelga, S.; Ayllón, E.; Martínez-Ferrer, B.; Cava, M.-J. Effects of Intervention Program Prev@cib on Traditional Bullying and Cyberbullying. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Jager, J.; Putnick, D.L.; Bornstein, M.H. More Than Just Convenient: The Scientific Merits of Homogeneous Convenience Samples. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2017, 82, 13–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Prevalence of cybervictim, cyberbully, cyberbully-victim and cyberbystander profiles according to the categories of no-problem, occasional, and severe problem (n = 255).
Table 1. Prevalence of cybervictim, cyberbully, cyberbully-victim and cyberbystander profiles according to the categories of no-problem, occasional, and severe problem (n = 255).
Total
n = 255
f (%)
Males
N = 155
f (%)
Females n = 100
f (%)
χ2p
CybervictimizationNo problem174 (68.24)112 (72.26)62 (62.00)2.960.227
Occasional 70 (27.45)37 (23.87)33 (33.00)
Severe11 (4.31)6 (3.87)5 (5.00)
CyberbullyingNo problem228 (89.42)138 (89.03)90 (90.00)0.390.822
Occasional 23 (9.01)15 (9.68)8 (8.00)
Severe4 (1.57)2 (1.29)2 (2.00)
Cyberbully-victimNo problem238 (93.33)145 (93.55)93 (93.00)1.570.456
Occasional 16 (6.27)10 (6.45)6 (6.00)
Severe1 (0.39)0 (0.0)1 (1.00)
CyberbystandingNo problem151 (59.21)95 (61.29)56 (56.00)1.480.477
Occasional 80 (31.47)48 (30.98)32 (32.00)
Severe24 (9.42)12 (7.74)12 (12.00)
Note: Cyberbullying: Screening of Peer Harassment [49] can simultaneously assign an adolescent to one or more roles.
Table 2. Distribution of the bystander subroles for the total sample and for each sex.
Table 2. Distribution of the bystander subroles for the total sample and for each sex.
Total
n = 228
f (%)
Males
n = 139
f (%)
Females
n = 89
f (%)
Helping the bully 0 (0.00)0 (0.00)0 (0.00)
Reinforcing the bully 1 (0.44)0 (0.00)1 (1.12)
Uninvolved 36 (15.79)26 (18.71)10 (11.24)
Provictim 53 (23.24)29 (20.86)24 (26.97)
Defender 138 (60.52)84 (60.43)54 (60.67)
Table 3. Means and standard deviations and partial correlations controlling for age between the dimensions of study (n = 255).
Table 3. Means and standard deviations and partial correlations controlling for age between the dimensions of study (n = 255).
12345678910
1. Cybervictimization -
2. Cyberbullying0.43 **-
3. Cyberbystanding0.55 **0.32 **-
4. Depression0.33 **0.13 *0.17 *-
5. Anxiety0.37 **0.13 *0.20 **0.78 **-
6. Stress0.36 **0.16 *0.24 **0.71 **0.75 **-
7. Informative Support−0.11−0.12 *−0.11−0.21 **−0.37 **−0.24 **-
8. Sense of Belonging−0.24 **−0.17 *−0.16 *−0.53 **−0.50 **−0.40 **−0.47-
9. Perceived Quality of Life−0.24 **−0.18 **−0.12 **−0.57 **−0.57 **−0.47 **0.48 **0.57 **-
10. Life Satisfaction−0.28 **−0.20 *−0.10−0.55 **−0.68 **−0.52 **0.42 **0.46 **0.71 **-
Mean0.890.161.592.962.645.1038.5920.5649.9527.47
SD2.100.543.134.213.644.874.943.6410.956.77
Range0–200–50–200–210–210–2122–4610–2818.5–837–35
Note: * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Comparisons of the total scores in the depression, anxiety, and stress scale, informative support and a sense of belonging, HRQoL, and life satisfaction based on cyberbullying roles performed by gifted students (n = 255).
Table 4. Comparisons of the total scores in the depression, anxiety, and stress scale, informative support and a sense of belonging, HRQoL, and life satisfaction based on cyberbullying roles performed by gifted students (n = 255).
RolesDepressionStressAnxietyInf-Supp Sense-Bel HRQoLLife-Sat
MSDMSDMSDMSDMSDMSDMSD
UnInvol a2.123.213.914.021.772.7439.304.6221.203.4452.3610.4729.225.83
Pure-CV b4.395.597.085.584.234.6637.975.2019.593.7346.4810.9724.897.38
Pure-CB c3.804.247.106.054.404.9737.704.1419.303.5346.589.7725.706.43
CB-CV d5.244.667.945.193.943.6734.595.4818.713.8941.768.6221.416.74
F6.880 ***10.321 ***9.543 ***5.574 ***5.339 ***9.068 ***13.149 ***
Post-hocb,d > ab,d > ab > aa > da > bb,d < ab,d < a
Note: UnInvol = uninvolved (n = 164); Pure-CV = pure-cybervictims (n = 64); Pure-CB = pure-cyberbullies (n = 10); CB-CV= cyberbully-victims (n = 17); Inf-Supp = informative support; Sense-Bel = sense of belonging; HRQoL = health-related quality of life; Life-sat = life satisfaction; M = mean; SD = standard deviation; F = Welch’s F; Post-hoc = Games-Howell; *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Comparison of scores on cybervictimization and cyberaggression as a function of frequency of smartphone use, weekday and weekend hours of use and possession of mobile phone.
Table 5. Comparison of scores on cybervictimization and cyberaggression as a function of frequency of smartphone use, weekday and weekend hours of use and possession of mobile phone.
CybervictimizationCyberaggression
M (SD)t (p)
Cohen’s d
M (SD)t (p)
Cohen’s d
Owns mobile phone with internet
Yes (n = 156)1.28 (2.47)4.45 (0.000)
d = 0.52
0.22 (0.66)3.03 (0.003)
d = 0.34
No (n = 98)0.29 (1.02)0.05 (0.22)
CybervictimizationCyberaggression
M (SD)F (p) η2
Post-hoc
M (SD)F (p) η2
Post-hoc
Frequency of smartphone use
Daily (n = 143) a1.39 (2.61)8.05 (0.000)
η2 = 0.064
c < a
0.20 (0.65)1.03 (0.360)
-
Only on weekends (n = 28) b0.46 (1.04)0.18 (0.47)
Rarely (n = 69) c0.23 (0.69)0.09 (0.33)
Frequency of mobile use on weekdays
Less than 1 hour/day (n = 131) a0.43 (1.12)10.38 (0.000)
η2 = 0.120
a,b,c < d
a < b < d
0.11 (0.36)2.53 (0.058)
-
Between 1 and 2 h/day (n = 73) b1.66 (3.00)0.19 (0.70)
Between 3 and 4 h/day (n = 24) c1.21 (2.09)0.29 (0.75)
More than 5 ho/day (n = 4) d4.75 (4.57)0.75 (0.96)
Frequency of mobile use on weekends
Less than 1 h/day (n = 81) a0.36 (0.90)6.63 (0.000)
η2 = 0.079
a < c,d
0.09 (0.36)3.08 (0.028)
η2 = 0.038
a < d
Between 1 and 2 h/day (n = 67) b0.72 (1.45)0.15 (0.44)
Between 3 and 4 h/day (n = 61) c1.66 (3.17)0.18 (0.72)
More than 5 h/day (n = 26) d1.92 (2.86)0.46 (0.81)
Note: M = mean; SD = standard deviation; F = fisher’s F; p = significance, η2 = eta squared; t = Student’s t.
Table 6. Comparison of scores on cybervictimization and cyberaggression as a function of frequency of parental control (supervision and time limitation).
Table 6. Comparison of scores on cybervictimization and cyberaggression as a function of frequency of parental control (supervision and time limitation).
Limited Time of Smartphone UseCybervictimizationCyberaggression
M (SD)t (p)M (SD)t (p)
Yes (n = 156)0.77 (1.63)1.60 (0.114)0.15 (0.42)0.67 (0.504)
No (n = 82)1.32 (2.88)0.21 (0.75)
Parents supervise smartphone use
Yes (n = 149) 0.87 (2.19)0.90 (0.367)0.15 (0.57)0.71 (0.482)
No (n = 85)1.13 (2.12)0.20 (0.53)
Note: M = mean; SD = standard deviation; p = significance, t = Student’s t.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

González-Cabrera, J.; Tourón, J.; Machimbarrena, J.M.; Gutiérrez-Ortega, M.; Álvarez-Bardón, A.; Garaigordobil, M. Cyberbullying in Gifted Students: Prevalence and Psychological Well-Being in a Spanish Sample. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2173. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122173

AMA Style

González-Cabrera J, Tourón J, Machimbarrena JM, Gutiérrez-Ortega M, Álvarez-Bardón A, Garaigordobil M. Cyberbullying in Gifted Students: Prevalence and Psychological Well-Being in a Spanish Sample. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019; 16(12):2173. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122173

Chicago/Turabian Style

González-Cabrera, Joaquín, Javier Tourón, Juan Manuel Machimbarrena, Mónica Gutiérrez-Ortega, Aitor Álvarez-Bardón, and Maite Garaigordobil. 2019. "Cyberbullying in Gifted Students: Prevalence and Psychological Well-Being in a Spanish Sample" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16, no. 12: 2173. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16122173

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop