Next Article in Journal
What If Healthy Aging Is the ‘New Normal’?
Previous Article in Journal
Body Burden of Dichlorodiphenyl Dichloroethene (DDE) and Childhood Pulmonary Function
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Do Effort and Reward at Work Predict Changes in Cognitive Function? First Longitudinal Results from the Representative German Socio-Economic Panel

1
Institute of Occupational, Social, and Environmental Medicine, Centre for Health and Society, Faculty of Medicine, University of Düsseldorf, 40225 Düsseldorf, Germany
2
Department of Social Epidemiology, Institute of Public Health and Nursing Research, University of Bremen, 28359 Bremen, Germany
3
Senior Professorship on Work Stress Research, Life Science Center, Faculty of Medicine, University of Düsseldorf, 40225 Düsseldorf, Germany
4
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Faculty of Medicine, University of Düsseldorf, 40225 Düsseldorf, Germany
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14(11), 1390; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14111390
Submission received: 9 October 2017 / Revised: 9 November 2017 / Accepted: 13 November 2017 / Published: 15 November 2017
(This article belongs to the Section Occupational Safety and Health)

Abstract

:
It has been suggested that work characteristics, such as mental demands, job control, and occupational complexity, are prospectively related to cognitive function. However, current evidence on links between psychosocial working conditions and cognitive change over time is inconsistent. In this study, we applied the effort–reward imbalance model that allows to build on previous research on mental demands and to introduce reward-based learning as a principle with beneficial effect on cognitive function. We aimed to investigate whether high effort, high reward, and low over-commitment in 2006 were associated with positive changes in cognitive function in terms of perceptual speed and word fluency (2006–2012), and whether the co-manifestation of high effort and high reward would yield the strongest association. To this end, we used data on 1031 employees who participated in a large and representative study. Multivariate linear regression analyses supported our main hypotheses (separate and combined effects of effort and reward), particularly on changes in perceptual speed, whereas the effects of over-commitment did not reach the level of statistical significance. Our findings extend available knowledge by examining the course of cognitive function over time. If corroborated by further evidence, organization-based measures in the workplace can enrich efforts towards preventing cognitive decline in ageing workforces.

1. Introduction

With increasing life expectancies, dementia has advanced as a major concern to public health worldwide [1,2,3]. In Western Europe, the prevalence of dementia has been recently estimated at nearly 7% among the population aged over 60 years and described as exponential function of age from this baseline age onwards [1,2,4]. As there is still no aetiology-specific treatment available [3,4], knowledge on amenable risk factors is of utmost importance for effective primary prevention [5]. Besides cardiovascular risk factors related to behavior (e.g., smoking, physical inactivity, and obesity in mid-life) and associated chronic conditions (e.g., hypertension and diabetes), education, and cognitive activities have been identified as promising entry points for primary prevention of dementia across adulthood [3,5,6,7].
Neuro-cognitive approaches such as “differential preservation” [8], “cognitive reserve” [9,10], or “complexity of work environment” [11] have been proposed to exert protective effects on cognitive function among older populations [12]. Biologically, it is plausible to assume that stimulating cognitive activities enhance the plasticity of neuronal circuits enabling the brain to delay age-related cognitive decline and pathological changes [13,14]. This assumption is referred to as the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. One major field associated with cognitive activity in adulthood relates to one’s occupational tasks. Therefore, ageing research in the field of occupational health psychology and epidemiology has primarily focused on the effects of distinct mental task features, such as “complexity,” and “novelty” on cognitive function [15,16]. These psychosocial work characteristics were also framed and operationalized as job demands (time pressure and required concentration) as well as job control (skill discretion, task variety, learning opportunities, decision latitude, and self-directedness) in line with Karasek’s well-known “demand–control” model [17]. Accordingly, higher levels of job demands and job control are assumed to be conducive to employees’ cognitive health. Joint exposure to both high job demands and control constitutes an “active job,” according to the model, fostering learning and strengthening brain capacities through neuro-cognitive stimulation. Findings from most prospective studies on mental work demands in general [11,18,19,20] as well as on job demands and job control in particular [21,22,23,24,25] tentatively or partly support the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. However, in a recent systematic review, the overall evidence on the link of working characteristics and cognitive decline has been rated as “insufficient, conflicting or weak” [15].
In this study, we complemented the work-related neuro-cognitive approaches by a neuro-affective perspective focusing on reward processing and reward-based learning. Distinct processes within the brain reward circuits are involved in cognitive performance [26,27]. For instance, positive learning reward expectancies are linked to dopaminergic neurons [28], the deficiency of which may account for impaired information processing and reduced working memory. Behavioral and motor abnormalities related to these deficiencies were observed in patients with Alzheimer or frontotemporal dementia [26,29]. Conversely, an inclination to positive affectivity in reward-based learning has been conceived as a feature of healthy ageing [26]. In the occupational context, receiving reward strongly builds on social interactions with colleagues and superiors and is usually related to successful accomplishment of tasks, thus eliciting positive emotions of self-esteem and enhancing work-related motivation, with beneficial effects on cognitive attention, stimulation, and performance [30]. Along these lines, we hypothesize that a psychosocial work environment that supports learning processes by offering positive rewards such as promotion prospects, recognition, and esteem may preserve cognitive function of working people from age-related decline, or at least decelerate its initiation. To our knowledge, this hypothesis has not yet been tested in the context of occupational health research, although it may be implicated in the concept of complex work environments [11] and in research on work motivation, learning, and development in an ageing workforce [12].
In order to accomplish a reward-based extension of previous research, we applied Siegrist’s effort–reward imbalance model to this analysis [31]. As a complementary concept to Karasek’s demand–control model mentioned above, this model focuses on the work contract and its core principle of reciprocity between efforts spent and rewards received in return. In addition, it includes an intrinsic component in terms of a coping pattern termed “over-commitment” (inability to withdraw from work at the expense of recovery) [32]. In the present contribution, we aimed to test longitudinal associations of psychosocial work environment measured by this model with cognitive function, using a representative sample of the German working population. Specifically, we expected that high effort, high reward, and an absence of excessive over-commitment were positively associated with cognitive changes. Importantly, a favorable balance of effort and reciprocated rewards is assumed to contribute to cognitive improvement in addition to the single components mentioned.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Population and Study Sample

Our study is based on the German Socio-Economic Panel (GSOEP) that is coordinated by the German Institute for Economic Promotion (Deutsches Institut für Wirtschaftsforschung, DIW), Berlin. This well-established longitudinal study covers a randomly selected and representative sample of the German adult population aged 18+. Since its initiation in 1984, this study has been followed up and replenished annually [33,34]. Participants gave their informed consent prior to data collection. Compliant with national laws as well as evaluated and approved by the German Council of Science and Humanities (Wissenschaftsrat), the GSOEP is ethically sound and explicitly intended for epidemiological analyses [34].
We used data from two waves in 2006 and in 2012. In 2006, the GSOEP included a total of 22,358 participants. Complete information on psychosocial work characteristics including the effort–reward imbalance model and other relevant covariates (see below) was available for 9746 employed participants. Given that cognitive function is assumed to be modifiable throughout adulthood, we did not restrict the age range within the employed population.
Cognitive tests (i.e., perceptual speed and verbal fluency, see below for details) were conducted in random sub-samples in both waves of 2006 and 2012, yielding a sample of 1031 employed participants with repeated measurements of perceptual speed and verbal fluency (see Figure 1).

2.2. Exposure and Outcome Measures, Covariates

The validated short version of the Effort–Reward Imbalance questionnaire was applied in the GSOEP 2006, with three items for “effort” (e.g., “Over the past few years, my job has become more and more demanding”), seven items for “reward” (e.g., “I receive the respect I deserve from superior or a respective relevant person,” “Considering all my efforts and achievements, my job promotion prospects are adequate,” and “My job security is poor”), and six items for “over-commitment” (e.g., “People close to me say I sacrifice too much for my job” and “Work rarely lets me go, it is still on my mind when I go to bed”) (please see [35] for details). Responses to the items of “effort” and “reward” were scored on a 5-point scale where a value of 1 indicated no respective distressful experience, and a value of 5 indicated very high distressful experiences. The items of “over-commitment” were scored on a 4-point scale (1 = full disagreement, 4 = full agreement with statement). Sum scores were computed with higher scores reflecting higher effort, reward, and over-commitment with potential scoring ranges of 3–15, 7–35, and 6–24, respectively. The psychometric properties of this questionnaire were tested in the GSOEP sample, with satisfactory reliability (Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were 0.74, 0.79, and 0.79 for “effort,” “reward,” and “over-commitment,” respectively), as well as good factorial, discriminant, and criterion validity. Median cut-off points were used to compare high levels with low levels of effort, reward, and over-commitment. In line with the theoretical model, the “reward” scale was further divided in its three sub-scales “esteem,” “promotion prospects,” and “job security” [31,35]. In addition, we built a composite variable from the binary variables for effort and reward, with low levels of both effort and reward as reference category.
It is generally not feasible to perform comprehensive neuropsychiatric assessments in large-scale epidemiological studies. Therefore, two short well-established and validated tests of cognitive ability with slight modification were used in the GSOEP by means of Computer Assisted Personal Interviewing (CAPI). These were the Symbol-Digit Test (SDT) and Animal Naming Test (ANT) [36]. These two tests have been widely used in several large epidemiological studies, e.g., [18,21,25,37]. The SDT measures cognitive mechanics that are hard-wired, biologically based capacities for information processing. This test corresponds to a sub-module in the non-verbal section of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) and examines cognitive processing speed, accuracy, processing capacity, coordination, and inhibition of basic cognitive processes, representing perceptual speed, working memory, and the capacity for deductive reasoning. The ANT measures cognitive pragmatics, i.e., training-related competencies. This test corresponds to a sub-module in the verbal section of the WAIS and represents verbal fluency. Recently, the cognitive tests in GSOEP have been successfully applied to study healthy aging [38]. To measure the changes in perceptual speed and word fluency between 2006 and 2012, test scores in 2006 were subtracted from test scores in 2012. Thus, positive score values indicated an improved cognitive function.
In addition, demographic (age, gender, and marital status), socioeconomic (education), lifestyle behaviors (smoking, alcohol consumption, and body mass index), and a short summary measure of health functioning (SF-12 physical and mental health) were also collected in 2006. These variables were included in statistical multivariate analyses as covariates. For instance, behavioral risk factors have been linked to work stress and dementia [3,5,39].

2.3. Statistical Analyses

Multivariate linear regressions were used to assess the associations of effort, reward, and their combinations, as well as over-commitment, with the changes in perceptual speed and verbal fluency between 2006 and 2012, respectively. Sociodemographic variables (age, gender, marital status, and education) and the score of respective cognitive tests at our baseline year in 2006 were regarded as a basic adjustment set (Model I). Robustness of associations was examined in two subsequent models that included behavioral risk factors of dementia (smoking, alcohol drinking, and BMI in 2006) in the first step (Model II) and physical and mental health in the second step (Model III).

3. Results

Characteristics of our study sample are given in Table 1. Both genders were equally distributed and two-thirds were married. A relatively high mean level of formal education (ca. 13 years) may reflect a characteristic of the German system that combines mandatory school education with vocational training. Overweight was the most common behavioral risk factor (52%). Smoking and regular alcohol drinking were reported by 32% and 17% of the participants, respectively. Distributions of physical and mental health scales were fairly similar, with a mean value of about 53 each. Median cut-off points of effort, reward, and over-commitment (7, 31, and 13, respectively) roughly corresponded to the respective mean values shown in Table 1. On average, in this age group under study (mean 44 years old), performance on the two cognitive tests (perceptual speed and verbal fluency) slightly improved from 2006 to 2012.
Results from multivariate linear regressions on changes in perceptual speed and verbal fluency are displayed in Table 2 and Table 3, respectively.
In Table 2, high levels of effort and reward at work in 2006 were each significantly associated with an increase in perceptual speed between 2006 and 2012, with estimated mean effects of effort (b = 1.90) being slightly higher than those of reward (b = 1.73) in Model I. According to the theoretical model, three components of reward have additionally been included in this analysis, and the component “promotion prospects” exerted a significant effect. In addition, in comparison to the reference group characterized by low effort and low reward levels, the remaining combinations of these two extrinsic components of the model were associated with significant improvements in perceptual speed over 6 years, with the relatively most pronounced effect for the combination “high effort and high reward” (b = 3.24). Patterns remained largely unaffected by adjustment for additional covariates in Models II and III. Concerning the model’s intrinsic component over-commitment, we did not observe a significant effect, despite an elevated coefficient in Model III.
In Table 3, the respective results are given for changes in verbal fluency between 2006 and 2012. Different from perceptual speed, high reward, but not high effort, emerged as a significantly positive predictor of improved cognitive functioning in this analysis (b = 2.09, in Model III). Again, the reward subscale of “promotion prospects” yielded a significant effect. Concerning the combination of the two extrinsic components of the model, the co-manifestation of high effort and high reward was the only variable category with a significant effect (in the fully adjusted Model III). The intrinsic component over-commitment was not significantly associated with improved verbal fluency.

4. Discussion

In this study, we tested the hypothesis that components of the model of effort–reward imbalance at work are positively related to improvements of cognitive function over a 6-year observation period in a representative middle-aged working population in Germany. More specifically, high effort and high reward, as well as a lack of excessive over-commitment to work, were expected to yield significant effects on the two measures of cognitive function under study, perceptual speed and verbal fluency. While high effort and high reward, both as separate and combined variables, were positively associated with perceptual speed, high reward and the co-manifestation of high effort and high reward only were related to verbal fluency. In both analyses, the subcomponent “promotion prospect” was the important aspect within the reward construct. Concerning the model’s intrinsic component over-commitment, our hypothesis was not confirmed. In additional sensitivity analyses, using the upper tertile of scale scores as an alternative cut-off point to define high vs. low levels of psychosocial working conditions yielded a very similar pattern of associations between effort–reward imbalance and changes in cognitive function (data are available upon request). Restricting the analyses to participants working at both time points in 2006 and 2012 did not produce different patterns as well (data are available upon request). The results of this study lend some preliminary support to the notion that positive experience of reward in response to effort spent at work may be conducive to cognitive function, given the links between affective and cognitive information processing in the brain reward circuits.
When relating our findings to the current literature, it is easier to discuss the role of effort, given that reward at work, our innovative feature, has not yet been studied previously in this context. The operationalization of “effort” is closely linked to the notion of high mental work demand, a notion that is also underlying the measurement of “demand” in Karasek’s job demand–control model. In our study, a significant association of effort with cognitive functioning was restricted to perceptual speed, while it was unrelated to verbal fluency. However, we need to be cautious in interpreting a single statistically significant effect, in particular in view of the fact that in two longitudinal investigations on associations of job demands with perceptual speed the relationship turned out to be inverse [25,40], at least after adjustment for education and occupation [25]. A further study finding deserves attention. When looking at all the results of Table 2 and Table 3, we observed that associations of psychosocial work characteristics with cognitive improvement were less consistent in cases of the verbal fluency test (Animal Naming Test) compared to the perceptual speed test (Symbol-Digit Test). It is possible to explain this inconsistency by the fact that different cognitive domains or abilities are measured by the two tests. Perceptual speed primarily reflects fluid cognitive abilities, while verbal fluency, as measured by the Animal Naming Test, is conceived as an interplay of fluid and crystallized cognitive abilities [41,42]. Whether these two types of cognitive abilities are related in different ways to beneficial work-related conditions, such as high effort and high reward, deserves more detailed analysis in future research.
Taken together, the associations of reward at work with the two measures of cognitive function were somewhat more consistent than those with effort, and it was evident that reward in terms of promotion prospects contributed to this consistency. This finding lends some preliminary support to the assumption that neuro-affective processes related to the reward system may provide an additional link between recurrent beneficial experiences at work and cognitive functioning. In view of the mean age of the study population (44 ± 10 years at baseline), occupational promotion prospects may matter more than esteem reward, and more than job security, given a low level of unemployment in Germany during the observation period. We have no explanation why no support was found for the over-commitment hypothesis. According to the theoretical argument, excessive striving at work in combination with a high motivation of being in control of one’s proximal environment entails the risk of cognitive misperception (e.g., under-estimation of demands) and cognitive rigidity [31]. Thus, scoring low on over-commitment is expected to contribute to more accurate perceptual and computational functioning. However, given the score distribution with a low mean level of over-commitment in this sample (mean 13.0 ± 3.9), an explanatory role of this variable may be compromised.
The fact that mean scores of cognitive function over time slightly improved rather than declined in this middle-aged cohort deserves an additional comment. One explanation points to a retest bias in repetitive cognitive testing. Yet, as we adjusted for baseline cognitive function and as the time interval between the two tests was 6 years, this explanation is not convincing. The “Flynn effect” may offer an alternative explanation as it captures the observation of an “age gain” of enhanced cognitive function across generations and even between cohorts with relatively small age differences. Along this hypothesis, some evidence was observed in a recent publication based on GSOEP data, comparing cognitive function (Symbol Digit Test) between two different age groups within a sample of elderly people (50–90 years). Here, an “age gain” of five to eight years in test performance has been demonstrated [43].
This study suffers from a number of limitations. First, the analysis is restricted to two waves of data collection. A robust test of changes over time in cognitive function would require additional measurement waves. This limitation also applies to the measurement of work stress. As we have no information on stressful work in 2012, no statement about a potential bi-directionality of effects can be made. Second, the GSOEP study included just two domains of cognitive function that may reflect different degrees of fluid intelligence or facets of crystalline intelligence, perceptual speed and verbal fluency. Other relevant aspects, such as phonemic fluency, episodic memory, inductive reasoning, or executive function were not included [21,23,24,25,40]. Third, we focused this study on one out of several available theoretical concepts of a stressful psychosocial work environment [17,21,25,44]. While the choice of the effort–reward imbalance model [31] has been justified by an innovative research hypothesis, and while respective data were available in 2006, we may nevertheless have bypassed other qualitative aspects of a health-conducive work environment with relevance to cognitive function. Fourth, as no clinical data on mental and physical disorders or their main risk factors were available, their potential confounding impact on cognitive function could not be assessed. Obviously, a self-reported measure of current health status, as available in the GSOEP data of 2006, cannot rule out this limitation. Fifth, we could not preclude selection occurring between 2006 and 2012: Participants who did not participate in cognitive tests in 2012 again were significantly younger than those who underwent the testing a second time and were included in our study (see Figure 1). Given the strong association of cognitive impairment with older age, it is unlikely that this selection has biased the general research results. Finally, the clinical significance of our results must be judged as modest. We observed a slight overall increase in the two tests of cognitive function within a middle-aged working population over a time span of six years. Whether we can expect a potential impact of favorable psychosocial working conditions on the postponement of cognitive decline in older ages remains largely unknown.
There are also several strengths in this study. Different from several previous reports dealing with associations of demand and control at work and cognition [15], in our analysis we were able to take the baseline values of cognitive function into account. Moreover, our study population has been recruited from a nationally representative sample of working people, whereas several earlier reports were based on employees from specific companies or organizations (e.g., [21,25]). Given the documented influence of several sociodemographic and socioeconomic factors on cognitive function, our study adjusted for these factors in stepwise multivariate model testing. Furthermore, this study applied a validated standard measurement of an internationally established theoretical concept of stressful work, effort–reward imbalance, whose success in explaining work-related health outcomes has been widely documented [45].

5. Conclusions

To conclude, our study is one of the first to document a positive association of balanced exchange between high effort and high reward at work with improved cognitive function over a 6-year period among a nationally representative group of middle-aged men and women in Germany, where cognitive function was assessed by perceptual speed and verbal fluency. If corroborated by further evidence, our results can instruct organization-based measures of maintaining and improving a health-conducive work environment that may contribute to the prevention of cognitive decline in ageing workforces.

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by a grant from the Research Commission, Faculty of Medicine, Heinrich-Heine-University of Düsseldorf, Germany (Project No.: 2016-52/9772652). The GSOEP is a public use data set that can be obtained from the German Institute for Economic Promotion (DIW), Berlin.

Author Contributions

J.L. and J.S. developed the study conception; J.L. obtained the funding and performed the design; J.L. and N.R. conducted the statistical analyses; N.R. prepared a first draft of the manuscript; J.S., J.L. and N.W. contributed substantially to the argumentation and revision. A.L. and P.A. helped critically interpret the results and revise the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Prince, M.; Bryce, R.; Albanese, E.; Wimo, A.; Ribeiro, W.; Ferri, C.P. The global prevalence of dementia: A systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimer’s Dement. J. Alzheimer’s Assoc. 2013, 9, 63–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Alzheimer’s Disease International. The Global Impact of Dementia: An Analysis of Prevalence, Incidence, Cost and Trends; Alzheimer’s Disease International: London, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  3. Livingston, G.; Sommerlad, A.; Orgeta, V.; Costafreda, S.G.; Huntley, J.; Ames, D.; Ballard, C.; Banerjee, S.; Burns, A.; Cohen-Mansfield, J.; et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Hugo, J.; Ganguli, M. Dementia and cognitive impairment: Epidemiology, diagnosis, and treatment. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 2014, 30, 421–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Baumgart, M.; Snyder, H.M.; Carrillo, M.C.; Fazio, S.; Kim, H.; Johns, H. Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: A population-based perspective. Alzheimer’s Dement. J. Alzheimer’s Assoc. 2015, 11, 718–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Mistridis, P.; Mata, J.; Neuner-Jehle, S.; Annoni, J.M.; Biedermann, A.; Bopp-Kistler, I.; Brand, D.; Brioschi Guevara, A.; Decrey-Wick, H.; Demonet, J.F.; et al. Use it or lose it! Cognitive activity as a protec-tive factor for cognitive decline associated with Alzheimer’s disease. Swiss Med. Wkly. 2017, 147, w14407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Salthouse, T.A. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol. Aging 2009, 30, 507–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Salthouse, T.A. Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the validity of the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. J. Assoc. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 1, 68–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Stern, Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2002, 8, 448–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 2015–2028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Schooler, C.; Mulatu, M.S.; Oates, G. The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychol. Aging 1999, 14, 483–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Fisher, G.G.; Chaffee, D.S.; Tetrick, L.E.; Davalos, D.B.; Potter, G.G. Cognitive functioning, aging, and work: A review and recommendations for research and practice. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2017, 22, 314–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2012, 11, 1006–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cheng, S.T. Cognitive reserve and the prevention of dementia: The role of physical and cognitive activities. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2016, 18, 85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Nexo, M.A.; Meng, A.; Borg, V. Can psychosocial work conditions protect against age-related cognitive decline? Results from a systematic review. Occup. Environ. Med. 2016, 73, 487–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Then, F.S.; Luck, T.; Luppa, M.; Thinschmidt, M.; Deckert, S.; Nieuwenhuijsen, K.; Seidler, A.; Riedel-Heller, S.G. Systematic review of the effect of the psychosocial working environment on cognition and dementia. Occup. Environ. Med. 2014, 71, 358–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Karasek, R.A. Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental strain: Implications for job redesign. Adm. Sci. Q. 1979, 24, 285–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Bosma, H.; van Boxtel, M.P.; Ponds, R.W.; Houx, P.J.; Burdorf, A.; Jolles, J. Mental work demands protect against cognitive impairment: MAAS prospective cohort study. Exp. Aging Res. 2003, 29, 33–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Fisher, G.G.; Stachowski, A.; Infurna, F.J.; Faul, J.D.; Grosch, J.; Tetrick, L.E. Mental work demands, retirement, and longitudinal trajectories of cognitive functioning. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2014, 19, 231–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Marquie, J.C.; Duarte, L.R.; Bessieres, P.; Dalm, C.; Gentil, C.; Ruidavets, J.B. Higher mental stimulation at work is associated with improved cognitive functioning in both young and older workers. Ergonomics 2010, 53, 1287–1301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Elovainio, M.; Ferrie, J.E.; Singh-Manoux, A.; Gimeno, D.; De Vogli, R.; Shipley, M.J.; Vahtera, J.; Brunner, E.J.; Marmot, M.G.; Kivimaki, M. Cumulative exposure to high-strain and active jobs as predictors of cognitive function: The Whitehall II study. Occup. Environ. Med. 2009, 66, 32–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Andel, R.; Crowe, M.; Kareholt, I.; Wastesson, J.; Parker, M.G. Indicators of job strain at midlife and cognitive functioning in advanced old age. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2011, 66, 287–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Andel, R.; Infurna, F.J.; Hahn Rickenbach, E.A.; Crowe, M.; Marchiondo, L.; Fisher, G.G. Job strain and trajectories of change in episodic memory before and after retirement: Results from the health and retirement study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2015, 69, 442–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Agbenyikey, W.; Karasek, R.; Cifuentes, M.; Wolf, P.A.; Seshadri, S.; Taylor, J.A.; Beiser, A.S.; Au, R. Job strain and cognitive decline: A prospective study of the framingham offspring cohort. Int. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2015, 6, 79–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Sabbath, E.L.; Andel, R.; Zins, M.; Goldberg, M.; Berr, C. Domains of cognitive function in early old age: Which ones are predicted by pre-retirement psychosocial work characteristics? Occup. Environ. Med. 2016, 73, 640–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Perry, D.C.; Kramer, J.H. Reward processing in neurodegenerative disease. Neurocase 2015, 21, 120–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Rowe, J.B.; Eckstein, D.; Braver, T.; Owen, A.M. How does reward expectation influence cognition in the human brain? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008, 20, 1980–1992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Simon, J.R.; Howard, J.H.; Howard, D.V. Adult age differences in learning from positive and negative probabilistic feedback. Neuropsychology 2010, 24, 534–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Hedden, T.; Gabrieli, J.D. Insights into the ageing mind: A view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 5, 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Kensinger, E.A.; Gutchess, A.H. Cognitive aging in a social and affective context: Advances over the past 50 years. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2017, 72, 61–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Siegrist, J. Adverse health effects of high-effort/low-reward conditions. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 1996, 1, 27–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Siegrist, J.; Li, J. Associations of extrinsic and intrinsic components of work stress with health: A systematic review of evidence on the effort–reward imbalance model. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Wagner, G.G.; Frick, J.R.; Schupp, J. The German Socio-Economic Panel study (SOEP)—Scope, evolution and enhancements. Schmollers Jahrb. 2007, 127, 139–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Schupp, J. The socioeconomic panel (SOEP). Bundesgesundh. Gesundh. Gesundh. 2012, 55, 767–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Siegrist, J.; Wege, N.; Puhlhofer, F.; Wahrendorf, M. A short generic measure of work stress in the era of globalization: Effort–reward imbalance. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2009, 82, 1005–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Lang, F.R.; Weiss, D.; Stocker, A.; Rosenbladt, B. Assessing cognitive capacities in computer-assisted survey research: Two ultra-short tests of intellectual ability in the German Socio-Economic Panel. Schmollers Jahrb. 2007, 127, 183–192. [Google Scholar]
  37. Wege, N.; Dlugaj, M.; Siegrist, J.; Dragano, N.; Erbel, R.; Jockel, K.H.; Moebus, S.; Weimar, C. Population-based distribution and psychometric properties of a short cognitive performance measure in the population-based Heinz Nixdorf Recall Study. Neuroepidemiology 2011, 37, 13–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Steiber, N. Population aging at cross-roads: Diverging secular trends in average cognitive functioning and physical health in the older population of Germany. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0136583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Siegrist, J.; Rodel, A. Work stress and health risk behavior. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2006, 32, 473–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Sindi, S.; Kåreholt, I.; Solomon, A.; Hooshmand, B.; Soininen, H.; Kivipelto, M. Midlife work-related stress is associated with late-life cognition. J. Neurol. 2017, 264, 1996–2002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Lindenberger, U.; Baltes, P.B. Intellectual functioning in old and very old age: Cross-sectional results from the berlin aging study. Psychol. Aging 1997, 12, 410–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Craik, F.I.; Bialystok, E. Cognition through the lifespan: Mechanisms of change. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 131–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Bordone, V.; Scherbov, S.; Steiber, N. Smarter every day: The deceleration of population ageing in terms of cognition. Intelligence 2015, 52, 90–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Elovainio, M.; Singh-Manoux, A.; Ferrie, J.E.; Shipley, M.; Gimeno, D.; De Vogli, R.; Vahtera, J.; Virtanen, M.; Jokela, M.; Marmot, M.G.; et al. Organisational justice and cognitive function in middle-aged employees: The Whitehall II study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2012, 66, 552–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Siegrist, J.; Wahrendorf, M. Work Stress and Health in a Globalized Economy: The Model of Effort–Reward Imbalance; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; ISBN 978-3-319-32937-6. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study sample.
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study sample.
Ijerph 14 01390 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of study subjects (n = 1031).
Table 1. Characteristics of study subjects (n = 1031).
Characteristics
Continuous Variables Mean ± SD
Age in 2006Years44.44 ± 10.08
Education in 2006Years12.66 ± 2.72
Physical health (SF-12) in 2006 52.92 ± 8.11
Mental health (SF-12) in 2006 53.29 ± 7.90
Effort in 2006 7.06 ± 3.13
Reward in in 2006 29.55 ± 4.99
Esteem reward in 2006 8.77 ± 1.87
Promotion reward in 2006 11.99 ± 2.57
Security reward in 2006 8.78 ± 2.08
Over-commitment in 2006 13.04 ± 3.94
Perceptual speed in 2006 25.20 ± 13.35
Perceptual speed in 2012 31.44 ± 8.29
Word fluency in 2006 27.87 ± 10.70
Word fluency in 2012 30.37 ± 11.10
Categorical Variables N (%)
Gender in 2006Men513 (49.76%)
Women518 (50.24%)
Marital status in 2006Married697 (67.61%)
Single182 (17.65%)
Separated, divorced, widowed152 (14.74%)
Smoking in 2006No697 (67.60%)
Yes334 (32.40%)
Alcohol drinking in 2006Occasionally, seldom, never854 (82.83%)
Regularly177 (17.17%)
BMI in 2006Normal (<25)495 (48.01%)
Overweight (≥25 and <30)362 (35.11%)
Obese (≥30)174 (16.88%)
Table 2. Associations of ERI in 2006 with changes in Perceptual Speed (Symbol-Digit Test) during 2006–2012 (n = 1031).
Table 2. Associations of ERI in 2006 with changes in Perceptual Speed (Symbol-Digit Test) during 2006–2012 (n = 1031).
Model IModel IIModel III
EffortLow000
High1.90 (0.51, 3.30) **1.94 (0.54, 3.33) **1.88 (0.47, 3.30) **
RewardLow000
High1.73 (0.45, 3.01) **1.57 (0.28, 2.86) *1.64 (0.34, 2.94) *
Esteem rewardLow000
High0.89 (−0.40, 2.18)0.78 (−0.51, 2.08)0.82 (−0.48, 2.13)
Promotion rewardLow000
High1.68 (0.41, 2.94) **1.55 (0.28, 2.82) *1.62 (0.33, 2.91) *
Security rewardLow000
High1.30 (−0.03, 2.64)1.20 (−0.13, 2.54)1.22 (−0.13, 2.57)
Over-commitmentHigh000
Low0.75 (−0.60, 2.10)0.78 (−0.57, 2.12)0.87 (−0.51, 2.24)
E-R CombinationsLow Effort + Low Reward000
High Effort + Low Reward2.69 (0.94, 4.43) **2.77 (1.03, 4.52) **2.72 (0.95, 4.49) **
Low Effort + High Reward2.50 (0.86, 4.15) **2.39 (0.74, 4.04) **2.44 (0.78, 4.10) **
High Effort + High Reward3.24 (1.08, 5.41) **3.10 (0.93, 5.26) **3.11 (0.94, 5.28) **
Multivariate linear regression, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01. Model I: adjusted for age, gender, marital status, education, and perceptual speed in 2006. Model II: Model I + additionally adjusted for smoking, alcohol drinking, and BMI in 2006. Model III: Model II + additionally adjusted for physical health and mental health in 2006.
Table 3. Associations of ERI in 2006 with changes in verbal fluency (Animal Naming Test) during 2006–2012 (n = 1031).
Table 3. Associations of ERI in 2006 with changes in verbal fluency (Animal Naming Test) during 2006–2012 (n = 1031).
Model IModel IIModel III
EffortLow000
High1.60 (−0.53, 3.73)1.61 (−0.52, 3.74)1.69 (−0.22, 3.61)
RewardLow000
High1.82 (−0.13, 3.76)1.83 (−0.13, 3.79)2.09 (0.34, 3.84) *
Esteem rewardLow000
High0.49 (−1.47, 2.45)0.55 (−1.42, 2.52)0.85 (−1.11, 2.81)
Promotion rewardLow000
High1.73 (−0.18, 3.63)1.79 (−0.13, 3.72)2.01 (0.08, 3.94) *
Security rewardLow000
High0.42 (−1.61, 2.44)0.41 (−1.62, 2.44)0.72 (−1.31, 2.76)
Over-commitmentHigh000
Low0.64 (−1.42, 2.71)0.73 (−1.34, 2.81)1.65 (−0.24, 3.54)
E-R CombinationsLow Effort + Low Reward000
High Effort + Low Reward0.65 (−2.02, 3.32)0.62 (−2.07, 3.30)1.42 (−1.00, 3.85)
Low Effort + High Reward0.86 (−1.67, 3.39)0.83 (−1.73, 3.39)1.82 (−0.46, 4.11)
High Effort + High Reward3.82 (0.50, 7.14) *3.84 (0.51, 7.17) *3.88 (0.93, 6.84) *
Multivariate linear regression, * p < 0.05. Model I: adjusted for age, gender, marital status, education, and perceptual speed in 2006. Model II: Model I + additionally adjusted for smoking, alcohol drinking, and BMI in 2006. Model III: Model II + additionally adjusted for physical health and mental health in 2006.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Riedel, N.; Siegrist, J.; Wege, N.; Loerbroks, A.; Angerer, P.; Li, J. Do Effort and Reward at Work Predict Changes in Cognitive Function? First Longitudinal Results from the Representative German Socio-Economic Panel. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 1390. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14111390

AMA Style

Riedel N, Siegrist J, Wege N, Loerbroks A, Angerer P, Li J. Do Effort and Reward at Work Predict Changes in Cognitive Function? First Longitudinal Results from the Representative German Socio-Economic Panel. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2017; 14(11):1390. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14111390

Chicago/Turabian Style

Riedel, Natalie, Johannes Siegrist, Natalia Wege, Adrian Loerbroks, Peter Angerer, and Jian Li. 2017. "Do Effort and Reward at Work Predict Changes in Cognitive Function? First Longitudinal Results from the Representative German Socio-Economic Panel" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 14, no. 11: 1390. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14111390

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop