Next Article in Journal / Special Issue
Rethinking the Methods and Algorithms for Inner Speech Decoding and Making Them Reproducible
Previous Article in Journal
Developing the Rationale for Including Virtual Reality in Cognitive Rehabilitation and Exercise Training Approaches for Managing Cognitive Dysfunction in MS
Previous Article in Special Issue
Ultrastructural Analysis of a Forming Embryonic Embodiment in the Adult Zebrafish Optic Tectum Surviving in Organotypic Culture
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Alcohol Deprivation Differentially Changes Alcohol Intake in Female and Male Rats Depending on Early-Life Stressful Experience

by
Marielly Carvalho
1,2,†,
Gessynger Morais-Silva
3,4,†,
Graziele Alícia Batista Caixeta
1,2,
Marcelo T. Marin
3,4 and
Vanessa C. S. Amaral
1,2,*
1
Laboratory of Pharmacology and Toxicology of Natural and Synthetic Products, State University of Goias, Exact and Technological Sciences Campus, Anapolis 75132-903, CO, Brazil
2
Graduate Program in Sciences Applied to Health Products (PPGCAPS) UEG, Anápolis 75132-903, GO, Brazil
3
Laboratory of Pharmacology, School of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Sao Paulo State University (UNESP), Araraquara 14800-903, SP, Brazil
4
Joint Graduate Program in Physiological Sciences (PIPGCF) UFSCar/UNESP, São Carlos, Araraquara 14801-903, SP, Brazil
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
NeuroSci 2022, 3(2), 214-225; https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci3020016
Submission received: 22 February 2022 / Revised: 22 March 2022 / Accepted: 7 April 2022 / Published: 9 April 2022

Abstract

:
Experiencing early-life adverse events has enduring effects on individual vulnerability to alcohol abuse and the development of addiction-related behaviors. In rodents, it can be studied using maternal separation (MS) stress. Studies have shown that, depending on the protocol used, MS can affect the mother and pups’ behavior and are associated with behavioral alterations later in adulthood, associated with both positive or negative outcomes. However, it is not fully elucidated how MS affects relapse-like behaviors when experienced by female or male individuals. Therefore, the aim of our study was to evaluate the effects of brief and prolonged MS on the alcohol deprivation effect (ADE) in female and male rats. Female and male Wistar rats were exposed to brief (15 min/day) or prolonged (180 min/day) MS from postnatal day (PND) 2 to 10. Later, during adulthood (PND 70), animals were submitted to an ADE protocol. Brief MS exposure prevented the ADE in both females and males, while prolonged MS exposure also prevented the ADE in female rats. Moreover, the ADE was more robust in females when compared to males. In conclusion, we showed that male and female rats are differentially affected by alcohol deprivation periods depending on their early-life experiences.

1. Introduction

The main feature involved in alcohol addiction is the neuroplasticity that undergoes the neurocircuitry related to motivation, habit, learning, and cognitive control. These neuroplastic events occur due to chronic drug intake and involve complex interactions with the environment and genetic factors [1]. Such long-lasting neuronal alterations are a crucial part of the most challenging step related to the treatment of addiction, namely relapse [2].
Given the pivotal role of neuroplasticity in the development of addiction, it is not surprising that early-life adverse events have enduring effects on individual vulnerability to alcohol abuse and the development of addiction-related behaviors [3]. A history of childhood maltreatment increases the incidence of alcohol use disorder (AUD) in adolescents [4] and adults [5,6] and is related to an earlier onset of drinking and alcohol abuse [7,8] and persistence of alcohol-related disorders through life [9]. Moreover, the greater the severity of the childhood abuse experienced, the greater is the impact on the prevalence and severity of the psychiatric disorders later in life [6,10]. Some studies highlight that such effects could be sex-dependent since they are more prevalent in women [11,12]. Female and male rats exposed to early life stress show an early maturation of the connections between the basolateral amygdala and the medial prefrontal cortex, while this alteration occurs even earlier in females compared to males [13]. Female rats exposed to early life adversity also show an increase in serotonin metabolites in the ventral tegmental area compared to males [14]. Frontal cortex maturation occurs earlier in females than males [15].
Sex differences in alcohol drinking behavior and the incidence of AUD are extensively reported in the literature. In rodents, most studies show that ethanol intake in females is greater than males’ [16,17,18,19,20], while men drink more and have a greater prevalence of AUD [21]. Although such data seems to be contradictory to rodent studies, human drinking behavior is largely influenced by social factors [22,23]. Moreover, recent reports are showing that women are drinking more, facing more problems with alcohol drinking, and women who drink excessively are more susceptible to the development of AUD than men [24].
The alcohol deprivation effect (ADE) is a broadly used rodent paradigm to study relapse-like drinking and has been exploited to the screening of possible candidates for the treatment of AUD relapse [25,26,27]. It consists of the chronic exposition to voluntary ethanol intake followed by periods of ethanol deprivation and re-exposure, resulting in a temporary increase in intake and preference [25]. The FDA (USA Food and Drug Administration) approved medications naltrexone and acamprosate, block the ADE in rodents, and are used for the treatment of AUD in humans [28,29,30].
Maternal separation (MS) is an animal model widely used to study the effects of early-life stress on alcohol intake and abuse. It relies on the importance of the pups-mother relationship, especially during the first two weeks (at least in rodent models) and, can be roughly separated into two main categories: the brief and prolonged MS [31]. These conditions affect the mother and pups’ behavior and are associated with behavioral alterations later in adulthood, associated with both positive and negative outcomes, depending on the protocol used [31,32,33]. In the brief MS, the pups are kept apart from their dams for short time intervals (usually 3 to 15 min), which mimics the natural environment of these animals (in situations such as dams leaving the nest to go after food, for example). In prolonged MS, the pups are separated from their dams for prolonged time intervals (from 60 min to 24 h) [34]. While the brief MS has been associated with positive behavioral outcomes, reducing the effects of stress exposure later in life [33], the prolonged MS has been associated with negative outcomes, increasing the stress reactivity later in life [35].
In animal models, a history of early-life stress also seems to impact the individual vulnerability to addiction-related behaviors and alcohol abuse. As well as for stress responses, such effects are highly dependent on the protocol used. Most of the studies have shown that the brief MS seems to decrease while the prolonged MS increases ethanol intake in rats when the animals are tested in adult life [16,31,36,37,38]. Prolonged MS also increases the impulsivity for alcohol consumption in adult rats [39] and the ethanol conditioned place preference (CPP) acquisition in adolescent rats, while it does not impact CPP in adult rats [16]. However, there are no studies examining MS effects on animal models of ethanol intake relapse. Thus, considering the importance of the relapse behavior to understanding ethanol addiction and to the development of treatment strategies, we studied the effects of brief and prolonged MS on the alcohol deprivation effect, a rodent model for the study of ethanol intake relapse [25] in female and male rats.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Animals

Female and male Wistar rats from the State University of Goias were used for mating and as experimental animals. Experiments were carried out according to the principles and standards of the National Council for the Control of Animal Experimentation (CONCEA), based on NIH Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals as approved by the Commission on Ethics in Animal Use (CEUA) of the State University of Goias (protocol number 008/2016).
Forty-five adult Wistar rats (females: n = 30; males: n = 15) were used for mating. Primiparous females were mated with adult males in a 1:1 ratio, and gestation day 0 (GD0) was defined as the day when copulatory plugs and/or sperm were found. Pregnant rats were kept alone during gestation and checked daily for newborn pups.
Seventy-six Wistar rats (males: n = 39; females: n = 37) were used as experimental animals. After weaning (postnatal day 21 - PND 21), they were kept in groups of 3–5 animals/cage, until the beginning of the alcohol deprivation effect experiment (at PND 70), when they were individually housed. The females’ estrous cycle was not pharmacologically synchronized.
Food and water were available ad libitum through the experiments. Animals were maintained in polypropylene cages (41 × 34 × 16 cm), under a 12 h light/dark cycle in a temperature-controlled environment (22 ± 2 °C).

2.2. Maternal Separation (MS)

The brief and prolonged MS was performed as previously described [16]. From PND 2 to 10, pups were daily separated from their dams for 15 minutes (brief MS – MS15) or 180 min (prolonged MS – MS180). During the separation period, the dams were transferred to clean cages and moved to a separate room, away from the pups. The pups were kept in their nests, under a red incandescent light, maintaining the temperature at 32 ± 2 °C. Weaning was normally performed at PND 21. The control group was kept undisturbed with the dams (except for cage cleaning) until weaning. To avoid any bias related to the litter, a maximum of two pups of each sex, from each dam, were used in each experimental group.

2.3. Alcohol Deprivation Effect (ADE)

The ADE protocol was adapted in our laboratory from the previously described [40].
Starting on PND 70, animals were exposed, for two days, to two bottles containing ethanol (6%) as the only source of fluid. Next, animals were given free access to two bottles, one bottle containing crescent ethanol concentrations (6, 8, and 10%) and one containing filtered water, for 4 days each concentration. This phase was called habituation and was intended to initiate alcohol drinking and habituate animals to the taste of the ethanol and experimental procedure. The ethanol consumption of each concentration was analyzed separately and identified as follows: ETOH 6% phase, ETOH 8% phase, and ETOH 10% phase. After that, baseline consumption was recorded: animals were given free access to two bottles, one containing ethanol (10%) and one containing filtered water for 10 days. Baseline consumption was considered as the average of the 4 days before the beginning of the deprivation phase (days in both male and female control groups with less than 20% variation from the average intake of ethanol 10%).
The next day after the baseline consumption phase, animals were submitted to the alcohol deprivation phase. Each deprivation period was comprised of 14 days of alcohol deprivation when only one bottle containing water was available followed by 7 days of free access to one bottle of 10% ethanol solution and one containing filtered water. The ADE was analyzed using the average consumption of four days after the end of each deprivation period. The animals were submitted to two deprivation periods. The experimental procedure indicating each ethanol exposure and deprivation phase is depicted in Figure 1.
Ethanol solutions (v/v) were prepared from absolute ethanol (Neon Comercial, São Paulo, SP, Brazil) diluted in filtered water and offered in plastic bottles fitted with stainless steel sipper tubes with ball-valve nipples in rubber stoppers. Ethanol and fluid intake was measured by weighing the bottles and reported as relative to body weight (g/kg). Liquid loss by leakage or evaporation was accessed using bottles placed in empty cages and was subtracted from the amount consumed by each animal.
Solutions were prepared fresh every day and presented to the animals at the same time. The position of the bottles was alternated every day to avoid side preference.

2.4. Statistics

Values were expressed as mean ± SEM and analyzed by repeated-measures ANOVA considering the independent factors sex and MS and the repeated measure phases. When ANOVA showed significant differences (p ≤ 0.05), the Newman-Keuls post hoc test was performed.
Statistics were performed using Statistica 7.1 software (StatSoft, Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA) and graphs constructed using GraphPad Prism 7 software (GraphPad Software Inc., La Jolla, CA, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Ethanol Intake Prior to Deprivations

We found a significant difference in ethanol intake before deprivations, related to the sex and the phase of the experiment, but not MS (Figure 2). The repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant effect for the factor sex (F1,69 = 56.07; p < 0.001), phase (F3,207 = 13.14; p < 0.001), and the interaction between sex and phase (F3,207 = 3.50; p < 0.05). The Newman-Keuls test revealed that females consumed more ethanol than males in all phases of the experiment before deprivations (p < 0.001). In males, ethanol intake during the baseline phase was increased when compared to ethanol consumption in ETOH 6% phase (p < 0.001). In females, ethanol consumption during the ETOH 10% phase was the greater prior deprivations (p < 0.05), while baseline ethanol intake was increased when compared to ethanol intake during ETOH 6% (p < 0.01) and ETOH 8% phases (p < 0.05).

3.2. Alcohol Deprivation Effect

Alcohol deprivation differentially affected the ethanol consumption in males and females, depending on the early-life experiences (brief or prolonged MS) (Figure 3A and B). The repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant effect for MS (F2,69 = 4.52; p < 0.05), sex (F1,69 = 17.59; p < 0.001), phase (F2,138 = 29.07; p < 0.001), and for the interaction between MS and phase (F4,138 = 3.22; p < 0.05) and, MS, sex and phase (F4,138 = 4.06; p < 0.01).
Control males showed an increase in ethanol consumption only after the second deprivation (p < 0.05) compared to baseline. MS180 male group, similar to control males, showed a significant increase in ethanol consumption only after the second deprivation (p < 0.001) compared to baseline intake, while the MS15 male group did not show significant alterations in ethanol consumption after neither the first or second deprivation phases relative to baseline (Figure 3A).
Control females showed an increase in ethanol consumption after either the first (p < 0.01) or second deprivation (p < 0.001) compared to baseline. Ethanol intake after the second deprivation was also greater than ethanol intake after the first deprivation (p < 0.01). Both MS15 and MS180 female groups did not show any significant alterations in ethanol consumption after neither the first or second deprivation phases. Additionally, ethanol consumption of the control female group was greater than the MS15 female group (p < 0.05), MS180 female group (p < 0.05) and control male group (p < 0.05) after the second deprivation (Figure 3B).
We also found significant alterations in ethanol preference (Figure 3C,D). The repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant effect for MS (F2,69 = 3.70; p < 0.05), phase (F2,138 = 37.68; p < 0.001), and for the interaction between sex and phase (F2,138 = 4.06; p < 0.05) and, MS, sex and phase (F4,138 = 2.41; p < 0.01).
Control males showed an increase in ethanol preference only after the second deprivation compared to baseline (p < 0.001) and after the first deprivation (p < 0.05). MS180 male group, similar to control males, showed a significant increase in ethanol preference only after the second deprivation (p > 0.001) compared to baseline and after first deprivation (p < 0.05), while the MS15 male group did not show significant alterations in ethanol preference after neither the first or second deprivation phases relative to baseline (Figure 3C). Similar to control males, control females showed an increase in ethanol preference after the second deprivation compared to baseline (p < 0.001) and after the first deprivation (p < 0.01). MS15 and MS180 female groups did not show an increase in ethanol preference after neither the first or second deprivation phases. Ethanol preference of the control female group was greater than the MS180 female group (p < 0.05) after the second deprivation (Figure 3D).

3.3. Fluid Intake

There was a significant effect of sex and phase, independent of the MS, on fluid intake through the experiment (Figure 4). The repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant effect for sex (F1,69 = 201.83; p < 0.001), phase (F5,345 = 30.61; p < 0.001) and for the interaction between sex and phase (F5,345 = 2.60; p < 0.05). Overall, female fluid intake was increased compared to males (p < 0.001). Moreover, fluid intake, independent of the sex or MS decreased through the experimental phases. In males, fluid intake in ETOH 10%, baseline, 1st re-exposure, and 2nd re-exposure phases was smaller than fluid intake in ETOH 6% and ETOH 8% phases, while fluid intake during 1st re-exposure was smaller than ETOH 10% phase. In females, baseline, 1st re-exposure, and 2nd re-exposure fluid intake were smaller than ETOH 6%, ETOH 8%, and ETOH 10% phases. The female fluid intake in ETOH 10% phase was smaller than fluid intake in ETOH 6% phase.

3.4. Body Weight

Body weight was evaluated through the different phases of the ADE protocol and reported as body weight gain in each period (Figure 5). First, we found a significant effect in initial body weight (Figure 5A) for the factor maternal separation (F2,69 = 5.06; p < 0.01) and sex (F1,69 = 567.40; p < 0.001). The post-hoc analysis revealed that males began the alcohol deprivation effect protocol heavier than females (p < 0.001), and both brief or prolonged maternal separation increased body weight in both males and females (p < 0.05). On the other hand, total body weight (Figure 5B) through the experiment was only affected by sex (F1,69 = 55.57; p < 0.001), revealing that males gain more weight than females over time (p < 0.001). Next, we evaluated the body weight gain in each phase of our protocol (Figure 5C). Body weight gain during baseline phase was affected only by sex (F1,69 = 4.21; p < 0.05), revealing a greater body weight gain in males compared to females (p < 0.05). During the first deprivation period, the two-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect for maternal separation (F2,69 = 5.51; p < 0.01) and sex (F1,69 = 35.44; p < 0.001), showing that males gained more weight during this phase (p < 0.001) and maternal separation increased body weight gain during the ethanol deprivation phase (p < 0.05). We found similar results for the first re-exposure phase: a significant effect for maternal separation (F2,69 = 10.88; p < 0.01) and sex (F1,69 = 7.19; p < 0.001), showing that males gained more weight during this phase (p < 0.001) and maternal separation increased body weight gain during the ethanol deprivation phase (p < 0.001). We found a significant effect only for sex during the second deprivation phase (F1,69 = 7.90; p < 0.01), when males gained more weight than females (p < 0.01). Finally, we found a significant effect for the interaction between maternal separation and sex (F2,69 = 7.63; p < 0.01) in the second re-exposure phase, while the post-hoc test did not reveal any significant differences.

4. Discussion

Here we showed that male and female rats are differentially affected by alcohol deprivation periods depending on the early-life experiences (brief or prolonged MS). First, females naturally drink more when compared to males. In males, the ADE was evident only after the 2nd ethanol re-exposure, and brief MS exposure was able to prevent it. In females, the ADE emerged earlier, in the 1st re-exposure period, while both brief and prolonged MS exposure prevented the ADE.
As already reported, we found remarkable differences in alcohol consumption between females and males. In both mice and rats, studies using several animal models of voluntary ethanol consumption showed that females consume more ethanol than males [16,17,18,19,20]. In a recent study from our lab, we showed that such differences are influenced by the concentration of the ethanol solution offered to the animals [16]. On the other hand, in humans, opposite results have been reported in epidemiological studies: men’s ethanol intake and AUD prevalence are greater when compared to women, although women’s AUD prevalence has been growing recently [21]. Such results give strength to the view that alcohol drinking in humans is strongly influenced by cultural factors and that the underrepresentation of female individuals in biomedical research has a profound impact on substance use disorders research [41].
Our results also revealed that female Wistar rats are more susceptible to ADE since the increased alcohol intake in females showed up sooner than in males. As cited before, there is evidence suggesting that women who drink excessively are more prone to the development of medical problems compared to men [24], despite the greater incidence of AUD in men. In animal models, the results are highly dependent on the protocol used and the behavioral outcome tested. In this context, female rats showed enhanced sensitivity to the rewarding effects of ethanol [42] and increased ADE [19,43]. On the other hand, male rodents show increased behavioral signs of ethanol withdrawal [44] and greater withdrawal-induced ethanol-seeking behavior compared to females [45].
Studies in rodents, using mixed-sex analysis, showed that the prolonged disruption of pups-mother contact during early-life increases ethanol binge drinking and voluntary ethanol consumption in operant self-administration paradigms performed later in life [39,46,47,48]. Prolonged MS also increases ethanol consumption in male rats tested during adulthood in the 2-bottle [36,49,50] or 3-bottle choice paradigms [16]. On the other hand, brief MS during early life usually decreases ethanol intake in adult male rodents [36,50]. Both brief and prolonged periods of MS increase voluntary ethanol consumption in female rats submitted to a 3-bottle choice procedure [16], while there is no effect of MS when ethanol consumption is evaluated using the 2-bottle choice procedure [51,52]. Here, we found a more complex effect: while, in males, brief MS blocked the ADE, both brief and prolonged MS prevented the ADE in females. To date, few studies looked for the effects of MS on the ADE, especially using female individuals in the experimental procedure. One study found that neither brief nor prolonged MS affected the increased ethanol consumption after periods of intermittent ethanol exposure [53]. However, in this study, the authors did not use an animal facility hearing or handled control without separation periods, making difficult the comparison between the studies.
We found interesting results regarding body weight gain through different phases of our ADE protocol. As expected, females gained less body weight compared to males, regardless of the experimental phase, since male Wistar rats are larger and have an increased growth rate from puberty onwards than female Wistar rats [54]. Interestingly, animals exposed to MS, independent of MS length and sex, showed increased body weight. Moreover, maternally-separated rats showed increased body weight gain during periods of increased stress, that is, the first deprivation and first ethanol re-exposure phases. It should be related to an increased stress sensitivity in animals exposed to early-life stressors [35].
The variation in the period when brain maturation occurs could be related to the MS sex differences found in our study. Females’ prefrontal cortex and amygdala undergo maturation faster when compared to males [13,14,15] and thus are more likely to be affected by the MS during the postnatal period that we performed our experiments. Prefrontal cortex dendritic morphology is more susceptible to the effects of MS in females than in males [55]. In this sense, the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala are involved in alcohol craving and relapse for alcohol drinking [56].
Two variables presented in our study could have impacted our results: the single-housing condition and the females’ estrous cycle phase. First, social isolation is a known stressor for rodents and can affect voluntary ethanol intake [57,58]. This is an important limitation of our work since individual housing was necessary for the evaluation of ethanol intake. On the other hand, more recent works have been showing that social isolation does not impact consumption of ethanol solutions up to 10%, in both rats and mice [59,60]. Thus, the procedure used in our work should be taken into account when interpreting our results. Secondly, we did not evaluate the estrous cycle or synchronized females’ cycle in our work. Literature reports have been showing that the estrous phase does not have a substantial impact on ethanol intake in rats [20], although it seems to influence ethanol consumption microstructure [61]. It could be explained by the fact that ethanol administration itself disrupts the estrous cycle in rats [62,63]. Moreover, human studies are inconsistent regarding the correlation between the menstrual cycle phase and alcohol intake [64].
In conclusion, brief MS exerted a protective effect against the ADE in both female and male rats. Despite the increased susceptibility to the ADE shown by females, prolonged MS also prevented the ADE in females, revealing sex-dependent effects of MS in rats.

Author Contributions

M.C., G.M.-S. and G.A.B.C. performed the experiments. G.M.-S. analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. M.T.M. and V.C.S.A. designed the study, supervised the project, and revised the manuscript for intellectual content. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Fundação de Amparo a Pesquisa do Estado de Goias (FAPEG/CNPq Programa Primeiros Projetos [201610267001023]).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Experiments were carried out according to the principles and standards of the Brazilian National Council for the Control of Animal Experimentation (CONCEA), based on NIH Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals as approved by the Commission on Ethics in Animal Use (CEUA) of the State University of Goias (protocol number 008/2016).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Koob, G.F.; Volkow, N.D. Neurobiology of addiction: A neurocircuitry analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2016, 3, 760–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Feltenstein, M.W.; See, R.E.; Fuchs, R.A. Neural Substrates and Circuits of Drug Addiction. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 2020, 11, a039628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Walters, H.; Kosten, T.A. Early life stress and the propensity to develop addictive behaviors. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2019, 78, 156–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Harrison, P.A.; Fulkerson, J.A.; Beebe, T.J. Multiple substance use among adolescent physical and sexual abuse victims. Child Abus. Negl. 1997, 21, 529–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Dube, S.R.; Anda, R.F.; Whitfield, C.L.; Brown, D.W.; Felitti, V.J.; Dong, M.; Giles, W.H. Long-Term Consequences of Childhood Sexual Abuse by Gender of Victim. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2005, 28, 430–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Felitti, V.J.; Anda, R.F.; Nordenberg, D.; Williamson, D.F.; Spitz, A.M.; Edwards, V.; Koss, M.P.; Marks, J.S. Relationship of Childhood Abuse and Household Dysfunction to Many of the Leading Causes of Death in Adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am. J. Prev. Med. 1998, 14, 245–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Pilowsky, D.J.; Keyes, K.M.; Hasin, D.S. Adverse Childhood Events and Lifetime Alcohol Dependence. Am. J. Public Heal. 2009, 99, 258–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Green, J.; McLaughlin, K.; Berglund, P.A.; Gruber, M.J.; Sampson, N.A.; Zaslavsky, A.M.; Kessler, R.C. Childhood Adversities and Adult Psychiatric Disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication I. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 113–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. McLaughlin, K.; Green, J.; Gruber, M.J.; Sampson, N.A.; Zaslavsky, A.M.; Kessler, R.C. Childhood Adversities and Adult Psychiatric Disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication II. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 124–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Bulik, C.M.; Prescott, C.A.; Kendler, K.S. Features of childhood sexual abuse and the development of psychiatric and substance use disorders. Br. J. Psychiatry 2001, 179, 444–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Widom, C.S.; Ireland, T.; Glynn, P.J. Alcohol abuse in abused and neglected children followed-up: Are they at increased risk? J. Stud. Alcohol 1995, 56, 207–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Young-Wolff, K.C.; Kendler, K.S.; Prescott, C.A. Interactive Effects of Childhood Maltreatment and Recent Stressful Life Events on Alcohol Consumption in Adulthood. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 2012, 73, 559–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. A Honeycutt, J.; Demaestri, C.; Peterzell, S.; Silveri, M.M.; Cai, X.; Kulkarni, P.; Cunningham, M.G.; Ferris, C.F.; Brenhouse, H.C. Altered corticolimbic connectivity reveals sex-specific adolescent outcomes in a rat model of early life adversity. eLife 2020, 9, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Mesquita, A.; Pêgo, J.M.; Summavielle, T.; Maciel, P.; Almeida, O.F.; Sousa, N. Neurodevelopment milestone abnormalities in rats exposed to stress in early life. Neuroscience 2007, 147, 1022–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Perry, C.J.; Campbell, E.J.; Drummond, K.D.; Lum, J.S.; Kim, J.H. Sex differences in the neurochemistry of frontal cortex: Impact of early life stress. J. Neurochem. 2020, 157, 963–981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Leichtweis, K.S.; Carvalho, M.; Morais-Silva, G.; Marin, M.T.; Amaral, V.C.S. Short and prolonged maternal separation impacts on ethanol-related behaviors in rats: Sex and age differences. Stress 2019, 23, 162–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Blanchard, B.A.; Steindorf, S.; Wang, S.; Glick, S.D. Sex Differences in Ethanol-induced Dopamine Release in Nucleus Accumbens and in Ethanol Consumption in Rats. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1993, 17, 968–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Barker, J.M.; Torregrossa, M.M.; Arnold, A.P.; Taylor, J.R. Dissociation of Genetic and Hormonal Influences on Sex Differences in Alcoholism-Related Behaviors. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 9140–9144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Li, J.; Chen, P.; Han, X.; Zuo, W.; Mei, Q.; Bian, E.Y.; Umeugo, J.; Ye, J. Differences between male and female rats in alcohol drinking, negative affects and neuronal activity after acute and prolonged abstinence. Int. J. Physiol. Pathophysiol. Pharmacol. 2019, 11, 163–176. [Google Scholar]
  20. Priddy, B.M.; Carmack, S.A.; Thomas, L.C.; Vendruscolo, J.C.; Koob, G.F.; Vendruscolo, L.F. Sex, strain, and estrous cycle influences on alcohol drinking in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2016, 152, 61–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. WHO. Global Status Report on Alcohol and Health; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018; Available online: https://www.who.int/substance_abuse/publications/global_alcohol_report/en/ (accessed on 10 January 2022).
  22. Kerr-Corrêa, F.; Igami, T.Z.; Hiroce, V.; Tucci, A.M. Patterns of alcohol use between genders: A cross-cultural evaluation. J. Affect. Disord. 2007, 102, 265–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Rahav, G.; Wilsnack, R.; Bloomfield, K.; Gmel, G.; Kuntsche, S. The influence of societal level factors on men’s and women’s alcohol consumption and alcohol problems. Alcohol Alcohol. 2006, 41, i47–i55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Erol, A.; Karpyak, V.M. Sex and gender-related differences in alcohol use and its consequences: Contemporary knowledge and future research considerations. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015, 156, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Vengeliene, V.; Bilbao, A.; Spanagel, R. The alcohol deprivation effect model for studying relapse behavior: A comparison between rats and mice. Alcohol 2014, 48, 313–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Martin-Fardon, R.; Weiss, F. Modeling relapse in animals. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 2012, 13, 403–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Koob, G.F. Animal models of craving for ethanol. Addiction 2000, 95 (Suppl. 2), S73–S81. Available online: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11002904 (accessed on 10 January 2022). [CrossRef]
  28. Heyser, C.J.; Moc, K.; Koob, G.F. Effects of Naltrexone Alone and In Combination With Acamprosate on the Alcohol Deprivation Effect in Rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003, 28, 1463–1471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Zhou, Y.; Crowley, R.; Prisinzano, T.; Kreek, M.J. Effects of mesyl salvinorin B alone and in combination with naltrexone on alcohol deprivation effect in male and female mice. Neurosci. Lett. 2018, 673, 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Tomie, A.; Azogu, I.; Yu, L. Effects of naltrexone on post-abstinence alcohol drinking in C57BL/6NCRL and DBA/2J mice. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2013, 44, 240–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Nylander, I.; Roman, E. Is the rodent maternal separation model a valid and effective model for studies on the early-life impact on ethanol consumption? Psychopharmacology 2013, 229, 555–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Orso, R.; Creutzberg, K.C.; Wearick-Silva, L.E.; Viola, T.W.; Tractenberg, S.G.; Benetti, F.; Grassi-Oliveira, R. How Early Life Stress Impact Maternal Care: A Systematic Review of Rodent Studies. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2019, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Raineki, C.; Lucion, A.B.; Weinberg, J. Neonatal handling: An overview of the positive and negative effects. Dev. Psychobiol. 2014, 56, 1613–1625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Newport, D.J.; Stowe, Z.N.; Nemeroff, C.B. Parental Depression: Animal Models of an Adverse Life Event. Am. J. Psychiatry 2002, 159, 1265–1283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Vetulani, J. Early maternal separation: A rodent model of depression and a prevailing human condition. Pharmacol. Rep. 2013, 65, 1451–1461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Ploj, K.; Roman, E.; Nylander, I. Long-term effects of maternal separation on ethanol intake and brain opioid and dopamine receptors in male wistar rats. Neuroscience 2003, 121, 787–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Roman, E.; Nylander, I. The impact of emotional stress early in life on adult voluntary ethanol intake-results of maternal separation in rats. Stress 2005, 8, 157–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Cruz, F.C.; Quadros, I.; Planeta, C.D.S.; Miczek, K.A. Maternal separation stress in male mice: Long-term increases in alcohol intake. Psychopharmacology 2008, 201, 459–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Gondré-Lewis, M.C.; Warnock, K.T.; Wang, H.; June, H.L.; Bell, K.A.; Rabe, H.; Tiruveedhula, V.V.N.P.B.; Cook, J.; Lüddens, H.; Aurelian, L. Early life stress is a risk factor for excessive alcohol drinking and impulsivity in adults and is mediated via a CRF/GABAA mechanism. Stress 2016, 19, 235–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Funk, D.; Vohra, S. Influence of stressors on the rewarding effects of alcohol in Wistar rats: Studies with alcohol deprivation and place conditioning. Psychopharmacology 2004, 176, 82–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Hilderbrand, E.R.; Lasek, A.W. Studying Sex Differences in Animal Models of Addiction: An Emphasis on Alcohol-Related Behaviors. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 2017, 9, 1907–1916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Torres, O.V.; Walker, E.M.; Beas, B.S.; O’Dell, L.E. Female Rats Display Enhanced Rewarding Effects of Ethanol That Are Hormone Dependent. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2013, 38, 108–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. García-Burgos, D.; Zuluaga, T.M.; Torre, M.G.; Reyes, F.G. Diferencias sexuales en el efecto de privación alcohólica en ratas. Psicothema 2010, 22, 887–892. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  44. Becker, J.B.; Koob, G.F. Sex Differences in Animal Models: Focus on Addiction. Pharmacol. Rev. 2016, 68, 242–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Randall, P.A.; Stewart, R.T.; Besheer, J. Sex differences in alcohol self-administration and relapse-like behavior in Long-Evans rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2017, 156, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Gondré-Lewis, M.C.; Darius, P.J.; Wang, H.; Allard, J.S. Stereological analyses of reward system nuclei in maternally deprived/separated alcohol drinking rats. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 2016, 76, 122–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Bassey, R.; Gondré-Lewis, M.C. Combined early life stressors: Prenatal nicotine and maternal deprivation interact to influence affective and drug seeking behavioral phenotypes in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2018, 359, 814–822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Bertagna, N.B.; Favoretto, C.A.; Rodolpho, B.T.; Palombo, P.; Yokoyama, T.S.; Righi, T.; Loss, C.M.; Leão, R.M.; Miguel, T.T.; Cruz, F.C. Maternal Separation Stress Affects Voluntary Ethanol Intake in a Sex Dependent Manner. Front. Physiol. 2021, 12, 2054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Amancio-Belmont, O.; Meléndez, A.L.B.; Ruiz-Contreras, A.E.; Méndez-Díaz, M.; Prospéro-García, O. Opposed cannabinoid 1 receptor (CB1R) expression in the prefrontal cortex vs. nucleus accumbens is associated with alcohol consumption in male rats. Brain Res. 2019, 1725, 146485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Huot, R.L.; Thrivikraman, K.; Meaney, M.J.; Plotsky, P.M. Development of adult ethanol preference and anxiety as a consequence of neonatal maternal separation in Long Evans rats and reversal with antidepressant treatment. Psychopharmacology 2001, 158, 366–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Gustafsson, L.; Ploj, K.; Nylander, I. Effects of maternal separation on voluntary ethanol intake and brain peptide systems in female Wistar rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2005, 81, 506–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Berardo, L.R.; Fabio, M.C.; Pautassi, R.M. Post-weaning Environmental Enrichment, But Not Chronic Maternal Isolation, Enhanced Ethanol Intake during Periadolescence and Early Adulthood. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Lundberg, S.; Abelson, K.S.P.; Nylander, I.; Roman, E. Few long-term consequences after prolonged maternal separation in female Wistar rats. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0190042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. Slob, A.; Bosch, J.J.V.D.W.T. Sex differences in body growth in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 1975, 14, 353–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Farrell, M.; Holland, F.; Shansky, R.; Brenhouse, H. Sex-specific effects of early life stress on social interaction and prefrontal cortex dendritic morphology in young rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2016, 310, 119–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Seo, D.; Sinha, R. The neurobiology of alcohol craving and relapse. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014; Volume 125, pp. 355–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Parker, L.F.; Radow, B.L. Isolation stress and volitional ethanol consumption in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 1974, 12, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Mumtaz, F.; Khan, M.I.; Zubair, M.; Dehpour, A.R. Neurobiology and consequences of social isolation stress in animal model—A comprehensive review. Biomed. Pharmacother. 2018, 105, 1205–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Thorsell, A.; Slawecki, C.J.; Khoury, A.; Mathe, A.A.; Ehlers, C.L. Effect of social isolation on ethanol consumption and substance P/neurokinin expression in Wistar rats. Alcohol 2005, 36, 91–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Evans, O.; Rodríguez-Borillo, O.; Font, L.; Currie, P.J.; Pastor, R. Alcohol Binge Drinking and Anxiety-Like Behavior in Socialized Versus Isolated C57BL/6J Mice. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2019, 44, 244–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Ford, M.M.; Eldridge, J.C.; Samson, H.H. Microanalysis of Ethanol Self-Administration: Estrous Cycle Phase-Related Changes in Consumption Patterns. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2002, 26, 635–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Emanuele, N.V.; LaPaglia, N.; Steiner, J.; Kirsteins, L.; Emanuele, M.A. Effect of Chronic Ethanol Exposure on Female Rat Reproductive Cyclicity and Hormone Secretion. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2001, 25, 1025–1029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Sanchis, R.; Esquifino, A.; Guerri, C. Chronic ethanol intake modifies estrous cyclicity and alters prolactin and LH levels. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1985, 23, 221–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Warren, J.G.; Fallon, V.M.; Goodwin, L.; Gage, S.H.; Rose, A.K. Menstrual Cycle Phase, Hormonal Contraception, and Alcohol Consumption in Premenopausal Females: A Systematic Review. Front. Glob. Women’s Heal. 2021, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Experimental procedure timeline. Female and male rats were exposed to brief or prolonged maternal separation during early life and submitted to an alcohol deprivation effect protocol during adulthood. PND, postnatal day; ETOH, ethanol.
Figure 1. Experimental procedure timeline. Female and male rats were exposed to brief or prolonged maternal separation during early life and submitted to an alcohol deprivation effect protocol during adulthood. PND, postnatal day; ETOH, ethanol.
Neurosci 03 00016 g001
Figure 2. Ethanol intake before deprivations in female and male rats exposed to brief and prolonged maternal separation. Bars represent the mean ± SEM (n = 11–14 animals/group). Each concentration represents the average intake of the 4 days of exposition in each experimental phase of the procedure. x; p < 0.05 when compared to female groups of the same experimental phase, independent of manipulation; a, p < 0.05 when compared to the ETOH 6% phase of the same sex, independent of manipulation; b, p < 0.05 when compared to ETOH 8% phase of the same sex, independent of manipulation; c, p < 0.05 when compared to ETOH 10% phase of the same sex, independent of manipulation. ETOH, ethanol; MS15, brief maternal separation; MS180, prolonged maternal separation.
Figure 2. Ethanol intake before deprivations in female and male rats exposed to brief and prolonged maternal separation. Bars represent the mean ± SEM (n = 11–14 animals/group). Each concentration represents the average intake of the 4 days of exposition in each experimental phase of the procedure. x; p < 0.05 when compared to female groups of the same experimental phase, independent of manipulation; a, p < 0.05 when compared to the ETOH 6% phase of the same sex, independent of manipulation; b, p < 0.05 when compared to ETOH 8% phase of the same sex, independent of manipulation; c, p < 0.05 when compared to ETOH 10% phase of the same sex, independent of manipulation. ETOH, ethanol; MS15, brief maternal separation; MS180, prolonged maternal separation.
Neurosci 03 00016 g002
Figure 3. Alcohol deprivation effect (ADE) in female and male rats exposed to brief or prolonged maternal separation. The ADE was analyzed using the average ethanol intake or preference of four days after the end of each deprivation period compared to the last four days of the baseline period. The animals were submitted to two deprivation periods. Bars represent the mean ± SEM (n = 11–14 animals/group). (A), male ethanol consumption (g/kg); (B), female ethanol consumption (g/kg); (C), male ethanol preference; (D), female ethanol preference. *, p < 0.05 relative to the baseline ethanol consumption of the same gender control group; #, p < 0.05 relative to the ethanol consumption after the first deprivation of the same gender control group; &, p < 0.05 relative to the ethanol consumption after the second deprivation of the same gender control group; $, p < 0.05 relative to the ethanol consumption of the control male group after the second deprivation. MS15, brief maternal separation; MS180, prolonged maternal separation.
Figure 3. Alcohol deprivation effect (ADE) in female and male rats exposed to brief or prolonged maternal separation. The ADE was analyzed using the average ethanol intake or preference of four days after the end of each deprivation period compared to the last four days of the baseline period. The animals were submitted to two deprivation periods. Bars represent the mean ± SEM (n = 11–14 animals/group). (A), male ethanol consumption (g/kg); (B), female ethanol consumption (g/kg); (C), male ethanol preference; (D), female ethanol preference. *, p < 0.05 relative to the baseline ethanol consumption of the same gender control group; #, p < 0.05 relative to the ethanol consumption after the first deprivation of the same gender control group; &, p < 0.05 relative to the ethanol consumption after the second deprivation of the same gender control group; $, p < 0.05 relative to the ethanol consumption of the control male group after the second deprivation. MS15, brief maternal separation; MS180, prolonged maternal separation.
Neurosci 03 00016 g003
Figure 4. Fluid intake through the experiment in female and male rats exposed to brief and prolonged maternal separation. Bars represent the mean ± SEM (n = 11–14 animals/group). Each concentration represents the average intake of the 4 days of exposition in each experimental phase of the procedure. x; p < 0.05 when compared to female groups, independent of the manipulation or experimental phase; a, p < 0.05 when compared to the ETOH 6% phase, independent of sex or manipulation; b, p < 0.05 when compared to ETOH 8% phase, independent of sex or manipulation; c, p < 0.05 when compared to ETOH 10% phase, independent of sex or manipulation. ETOH, ethanol; MS15, brief maternal separation; MS180, prolonged maternal separation.
Figure 4. Fluid intake through the experiment in female and male rats exposed to brief and prolonged maternal separation. Bars represent the mean ± SEM (n = 11–14 animals/group). Each concentration represents the average intake of the 4 days of exposition in each experimental phase of the procedure. x; p < 0.05 when compared to female groups, independent of the manipulation or experimental phase; a, p < 0.05 when compared to the ETOH 6% phase, independent of sex or manipulation; b, p < 0.05 when compared to ETOH 8% phase, independent of sex or manipulation; c, p < 0.05 when compared to ETOH 10% phase, independent of sex or manipulation. ETOH, ethanol; MS15, brief maternal separation; MS180, prolonged maternal separation.
Neurosci 03 00016 g004
Figure 5. Initial body weight and body weight gain through the experiment in female and male rats exposed to brief and prolonged maternal separation. Bars represent the mean ± SEM (n = 11–14 animals/group). (A), initial body weight (g). (B), body weight gain during the whole period of alcohol deprivation effect (g/kg). (C), body weight gain in different phases of the alcohol deprivation effect protocol (g/kg). Body weight gain was calculated as the difference between the body weight on the last day of the respective phase and the first day of the same experimental phase. x; p < 0.05 when compared to female groups, independent of the manipulation; z, significant effect of maternal separation, regardless of sex. MS15, brief maternal separation; MS180, prolonged maternal separation.
Figure 5. Initial body weight and body weight gain through the experiment in female and male rats exposed to brief and prolonged maternal separation. Bars represent the mean ± SEM (n = 11–14 animals/group). (A), initial body weight (g). (B), body weight gain during the whole period of alcohol deprivation effect (g/kg). (C), body weight gain in different phases of the alcohol deprivation effect protocol (g/kg). Body weight gain was calculated as the difference between the body weight on the last day of the respective phase and the first day of the same experimental phase. x; p < 0.05 when compared to female groups, independent of the manipulation; z, significant effect of maternal separation, regardless of sex. MS15, brief maternal separation; MS180, prolonged maternal separation.
Neurosci 03 00016 g005
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Carvalho, M.; Morais-Silva, G.; Caixeta, G.A.B.; Marin, M.T.; Amaral, V.C.S. Alcohol Deprivation Differentially Changes Alcohol Intake in Female and Male Rats Depending on Early-Life Stressful Experience. NeuroSci 2022, 3, 214-225. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci3020016

AMA Style

Carvalho M, Morais-Silva G, Caixeta GAB, Marin MT, Amaral VCS. Alcohol Deprivation Differentially Changes Alcohol Intake in Female and Male Rats Depending on Early-Life Stressful Experience. NeuroSci. 2022; 3(2):214-225. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci3020016

Chicago/Turabian Style

Carvalho, Marielly, Gessynger Morais-Silva, Graziele Alícia Batista Caixeta, Marcelo T. Marin, and Vanessa C. S. Amaral. 2022. "Alcohol Deprivation Differentially Changes Alcohol Intake in Female and Male Rats Depending on Early-Life Stressful Experience" NeuroSci 3, no. 2: 214-225. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci3020016

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop