Next Article in Journal
Hepatoprotective Effect of Herb Formula KIOM2012H against Nonalcoholic Fatty Liver Disease
Previous Article in Journal
Low Maternal Vitamin B12 Status Is Associated with Lower Cord Blood HDL Cholesterol in White Caucasians Living in the UK
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Role of Dietary Protein and Thiamine Intakes on Cognitive Function in Healthy Older People: A Systematic Review

School of Medicine, University of Wollongong, New South Wales 2522, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Nutrients 2015, 7(4), 2415-2439; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7042415
Submission received: 18 February 2015 / Revised: 13 March 2015 / Accepted: 19 March 2015 / Published: 2 April 2015

Abstract

:
The effectiveness of nutritional interventions to prevent and maintain cognitive functioning in older adults has been gaining interest due to global population ageing. A systematic literature review was conducted to obtain and appraise relevant studies on the effects of dietary protein or thiamine on cognitive function in healthy older adults. Studies that reported on the use of nutritional supplementations and/or populations with significant cognitive impairment were excluded. Seventeen eligible studies were included. Evidence supporting an association between higher protein and/or thiamine intakes and better cognitive function is weak. There was no evidence to support the role of specific protein food sources, such as types of meat, on cognitive function. Some cross-sectional and case-control studies reported better cognition in those with higher dietary thiamine intakes, but the data remains inconclusive. Adequate protein and thiamine intake is more likely associated with achieving a good overall nutritional status which affects cognitive function rather than single nutrients. A lack of experimental studies in this area prevents the translation of these dietary messages for optimal cognitive functioning and delaying the decline in cognition with advancing age.

1. Introduction

Dementia is a condition in which cognitive and physical functionality gradually deteriorates, resulting in diminished self-care ability. The increasing burden of dementia impacts heavily on already stretched health care services, as well as adversely affecting quality of life and independence of affected older adults and their caregivers. The effectiveness of nutritional interventions to maintain optimal cognitive functioning and prevent cognitive decline in older adults has been gaining interest [1].
In terms of specific nutrients, those most intensively studied for a potential cognitive-enhancing effect include omega-3, antioxidants and B-vitamins [2,3,4]. More recently, intake of combinations of foods consumed within defined dietary patterns, in particular, the Mediterranean diet has been suggested to hold promise [5]. Protein-energy malnutrition is particularly common in the older population and has been found to be related to increased risk of sacropenia and frailty [6]. Nutrition plays an important role in the prevention of sacropenia and frailty which has been suggested to be highly associated with cognitive impairment [7]. A study investigating the nutritional status of hospitalized patients showed high rates of poor nutrition status (malnourished or at risk of malnutrition) in dementia and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients in comparison to patients with no cognitive impairment suggesting higher prevalence of malnutrition in older adults with cognitive impairment [8]. A study on young healthy men showed that a high protein diet improved both physical and cognitive function [9] but the effects are still unknown in older populations who may metabolize protein and amino acids less efficiently than younger people [10]. In that study, however, nutritional supplements provided the protein source which limits translation into food-based messages [9].
Thiamine or Vitamin-B1 has been suggested to be associated with neurodegenerative diseases through its role in oxidative and glucose metabolisms [11,12]. Thiamine deficiency is a common condition in older adults especially in hospitalized and institutionalized patients [13] and has also been suggested to be associated with a higher proportion of falls, Alzheimer Disease (AD) and depression [14]. Additionally, post-mortems show decreased activity of two major thiamine dependent enzymes in AD patients compared to age and gender matched cognitively intact controls [15]. In a review of randomized controlled trials involving high-dose thiamine supplementation of >3 mg/day, outcomes on cognitive function were inconsistent [16]. Overall, the evidence was not convincing due to small sample sizes and the use of inappropriate study design such as a cross-over study, which is unsuitable for progressive diseases, such as AD [17]. The aim of the current systematic literature review was to evaluate the available evidence regarding the association between either dietary protein or thiamine and cognitive function in healthy older adults.

2. Methods

2.1. Identification of Studies

A computerised literature search was conducted using the electronic databases of Cinahl, Medline, Science Direct, Web of Science and Scopus to identify studies that had been published up to mid-May 2014. The primary search terms used in the search strategy included the usage of Booleans (diet* OR nutri* OR food) AND (pattern* OR intake* OR protein OR thiamin*) AND (cogniti* OR mental OR brain OR Alzheimer* OR dementia) AND (elderly OR older OR age*). The review followed the PRISMA guidelines [18]. No restrictions were applied for type of study, year of publication, before the cut-off date or country.
Inclusion criteria: (i) Older adults (≥60 years), (ii) Human studies, (iii) Relatively healthy subjects (e.g., Absence of cancer, cardiovascular, renal and liver diseases), (iv) Dietary Intake (Not supplements), (v) English language, (vi) Free of significant cognitive impairment.

2.2. Data Extraction

Information regarding the study design, demographics of the population, type of cognitive and dietary measurements and results were extracted and tabulated into a summary table. This was conducted based on the evidence analysis manual from the American Dietetic Association (ADA) [19]. The National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) [20] levels of evidence were used to define the type of study, while the quality criteria checklist from the ADA was used to rate the quality of the studies reviewed. No authors were contacted for missing data.

3. Results

Of the 2987 studies that were identified during the search process, 20 studies met the inclusion criteria. From these, 16 studies were reviewed in full, as shown in Figure 1. Two studies were excluded because they were conducted on people aged ≥90 years and were deemed to be unsuitable because of potential survival bias and lack of generalizability of the results [21,22]. A cross-sectional study was excluded as meat and fish were collated within the same food category [23]. Fish has been suggested to have a protective effect on cognitive decline due to its polyunsaturated fat content [24] One cohort study did not provide follow-up results on cognitive function of the subjects and was therefore excluded [25]. In another cohort study, the reported dietary intakes by participants included supplementation [26]. The eligible studies were all observational in design, including six cohort studies, two case-control studies and seven cross-sectional studies. The most common diagnostic tool used for assessment of cognitive function was the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). Dietary assessment methods employed in the reviewed studies were either one or a combination of Food Frequency Questionnaires (FFQ), food records or 24-hour recalls. The collated papers included in the review provided data for a total of 18,302 study participants.
A detailed appraisal of the included studies summarized according to type of study design is attached as Table 1 for cohort studies, Table 2 for case-control studies and Table 3 for cross-sectional studies, while quality rating outcomes are shown in Table A1.
Figure 1. Flowchart of article selection process.
Figure 1. Flowchart of article selection process.
Nutrients 07 02415 g001
Table 1. Evidence from cohort studies according to year of publication.
Table 1. Evidence from cohort studies according to year of publication.
AuthorPopulationMeasurement of Cognition (Cutoff Point) and DietProteinProtein Food SourceThiamineAdjustmentsNHMRC Level of Evidence
La Rue et al. 1997 [27], USA304 community-dwelling healthy individuals, age between 66 and 90 years (6 years cohort).The Abstraction Scale from Shipley-Hartford Intelligence Test, The Logical Memory and Visual Reproduction subtests from Wechsler Memory Scale and Rey-Osterrieth Test 3-day Food Dietary recordsProtein (g/day)
Baseline (median): 75 g/day
6 Years Follow Up
(median): 72 g/day
Rey Osterrieth Recall Test
r = 0.19
p-value < 0.05
Logical Memory Test
r = 0.20
p-value < 0.05
Significantly positive association between dietary protein intake and Rey Osterrieth Recall and Logical Memory Test scores.
Thiamine (mg/day)
Baseline (median): 1.93
6 years Follow up (median): 2.47
Rey Osterrieth Copy Test
Baseline, r = −0.07
6 years Follow up, r = 0.16
p-value < 0.10
Rey Osterrieth Recall Test Baseline, r = 0.08
6 years Follow up,
r = 0.15
p-value < 0.10
Shipley Hartford Abstraction Test
Baseline, r = −0.08
6 years Follow up,
r = 0.29
p-value < 0.01
Significant positive association between dietary thiamine intake and Shipley Hartford Abstraction Test scores.
Body weightIII-2, Neutral
Deschamps et al. 2002 [28], France125 community-dwelling non-demented elderly, age >68 years (5 years cohort) 3-day food diary, Diet history and FFQMMSE (cognitive decline: reduction of ≥3 MMSE score over 5 years)Protein Median intake: 1.33 g/kg/day
<1.0 g/kg/day (n = 21), OR = 1.00 ≥1.0 g/kg/day (n = 104), OR = 1.92 (0.38–9.62)
p-value n.s
No significant association between dietary protein intake and cognitive decline.
Age, Gender and EducationIII-2, Neutral
McNeill et al. 2011 [29], UK882 subjects living around Edinburg, mean age 70 years. (57 to 60 years cohort)MHT, MMSE, NART, Verbal Fluency test, Wescler Adult Intelligence Scale–III. Semi-quantitative FFQ Thiamine (mg/day)
Mean Intake from diet: 1.51 ± 0.50
MMSE β = 0.057
p-value n.s
Verbal fluency
β = 0.004
p-value n.s
No significant association between dietary thiamine intake and cognitive function or verbal fluency.
Age, gender, IQ at age 11 years, smoking, social class, education, statin use, presence of APOE alleleIII-3, Neutral
Barberger-Gateau et al. 2007 [24], France8085 free-living non-demented elderly, age >65 years (4 years cohort)DSM- IV by neurologist FFQ Meat
All cause dementia
2–3 times/week:
Incidence = 1.13 (0.88–1.97),
4–6 times/week:
Incidence = 0.85 (0.68–1.01),
Daily: Incidence =1.03 (0.80–1.27)
p-value n.s
AD
2–3 times/week:
Incidence = 0.76 (0.58–0.96)
4–6 times/week:
Incidence = 0.53 (0.40–0.66),
Daily:
Incidence =0.65 (0.46–0.84)
p-value n.s
No significant association between meat consumption and dementia/AD.
AgeIII-2
Velho et al. 2008 [30], Portugal187 free-living elderly participants with normal cognition, age >65 years (8.5 ± 3.5 months cohort).MMSE (Improvement: Increase >0 in MMSE score, No Improvement: No increase in MMSE score) 3-day food diaryProtein (g/day)
No improvement:
70.9 ± 2.0
Improvement: 73.4 ± 1.8
t-test = 1.04
p-value n.s
No significant association between dietary protein intake and improvements in cognitive function.
Age, Total energy III-2, Neutral
Vercambre et al. 2009 [31], France4809 elderly women from E3N cohort, age between 63 and 68 years (13 years cohort).DECO scale (<33), Questionnaire for close relative/friend 208 item FFQ, 24 h recallsProtein (g/day)
Mean = 87.70 ± 24.55
Q3-Q1
OR = 0.92 (0.74–1.14)
Trend (p-value n.s)
No significant association between dietary protein intake and cognitive function.
Beef, pork, lamb (g/day)
Mean = 45.45 ± 35.53
Q3-Q1
OR = 0.87 (0.66–1.15)
Trend (p-value n.s)
Poultry (g/day)
Mean = 16.93 ± 17.89
Q3-Q1
OR = 0.73 (0.58–0.91) Trend
(p-value = 0.004)
No significant association for beef, pork and lamb.
Significantly higher poultry intake in participants with better cognition.
Age, Education, BMI, Frequency of average physical activity, Average daily energy intake, Smoking, Supplements, Post-menopausal hormones, Depression, Cancer, CHD, Stroke, T2DM, High cholesterol and Hypertension.III-2, Neutral
Roberts et al. 2012 [32], USA937 cognitively normal participants, age between 70 and 89 (median of 3.7 years cohort)CDR, Short test of mental status with 9 test assessing 4 domains of memory, executive function , language and visuospatial skills 128 item FFQProtein (g/day)
All participants (mean): 78 g/day, 18% energy
Q1 (20% energy)
HR = 0.79 (0.52–1.20)
Correlation of trend across quartiles,
p-value = 0.03
Significant association between dietary protein intake of 16%–20% of energy intake and reduced risk of MCI or dementia.
Gender, Education, Total daily energy, Non-participation at baseline, Single macronutrient, APOE e4, T2DM, Depression, BMI, Stroke, Marital status, Smoking, Alcohol, Occupation and Frequency of moderate physical activityIII-2, Neutral
Abbrevations: AD, Alzheimer’s Disease; CDR, Clinical Dementia Rating; DECO, Deterioration Cognitive Observée; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; FFQ, Food Frequency Questionnaire; MHT, Moray House Test; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination; NART, National Adult Reading Test.
Table 2. Evidence from case-control studies according to year of publication.
Table 2. Evidence from case-control studies according to year of publication.
AuthorPopulationMeasurement of Cognition (Cutoff Point) and DietProtein Protein Food Source ThiamineAdjustmentsNHMRC Level of Evidence
Burns et al. 1989 [33], UK78 elderly subjects (28 community-living demented, 21 hospitalized demented, 29 control)MMSE (control: ≥29, case: ≤24)
3-day weighed food record
Protein (g/day)
Community living demented:
61 ± 12.0
Control: 44 ± 15.2
p-value < 0.05
Hospitalized demented: 73 ± 8.3
Control: 44 ± 15.2
p-value < 0.05
Significantly higher dietary protein intake in controls than demented participants.
NoneIII-3, Positive
Nes et al. 1998 [34], Norway32 community-living elderly
(16- case, 16-control), age >75 years.
DSM-III
3-day weighed food record
Protein (g/day)
Men
(Control =75 ± 12, Dementia = 69 ± 14)
p-value n.s
Women
(Control = 64 ± 14, Dementia = 51 ± 12)
p-value ≤ 0.05
Significantly higher dietary protein intake in women controls than women with dementia. No significant difference detected for men.
Thiamine (mg/day)
Men
(Control =1.0 ± 0.1, Dementia = 1.0 ± 0.1)
p-value n.s
Women
(Control = 1.0 ± 0.3, Dementia = 0.7 ± 0.2)
p-value ≤ 0.05
Significantly higher dietary thiamine intake in women controls than women with dementia. No significant difference detected for men.
NoneIII-3, Positive
Abbrevations: DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination.
Table 3. Evidence from cross-sectional studies according to year of publication.
Table 3. Evidence from cross-sectional studies according to year of publication.
AuthorPopulationMeasurement of Cognition (Cutoff Point) and DietProtein Protein Food Source ThiamineAdjustmentsNHMRC Level of Evidence
Ortega et al. 1997 [35], Spain260 free living elderly (108 men and 152 women) aged between 65 and 90 years.MMSE (unsatisfactory: 0) 7-day weighed food record, FFQProtein (g/day)
Men
(Unsatisfactory MMSE: 81.0 ± 21.3, Satisfactory MMSE: 81.8 ± 18.7)
p-value n.s
Women
(Unsatisfactory MMSE: 71.8 ± 14.5, Satisfactory MMSE: 73.9 ± 18.8)
p-value n.s
No significant association between dietary protein intake and cognitive function.
Thiamine (mg/day)
Men
(Unsatisfactory MMSE: 1.18 ± 0.25, Satisfactory MMSE: 1.19 ± 0.36)
Women
(Unsatisfactory MMSE: 0.96 ± 0.26, Satisfactory MMSE: 1.06 ± 0.36)
r = 0.2225, p-value < 0.01
Significantly higher dietary thiamine intake in participants with satisfactory scores.
Age, GenderIV, Neutral
Lee et al. 2001 [36],, Korea449 free-living participants (210 men & 239 women, age >60 years.MMSE-Korean Version (Poor: ≤19, Inadequate: 20–23, Normal: ≥24) 24-hour recallProtein (g/day)
Men
(Normal: 65.1 ± 25.7, Inadequate: 63.9 ± 26.4, Poor: 60.0 ± 25.0)
p-value n.s
r = 0.078, p-value n.s
Women
(Normal: 57.0 ± 24.5, Inadequate: 58.4 ± 29.1, Poor: 42.5 ± 22.3)
p-value < 0.05
r = 0.181 (p-value < 0.01)
Significant association between higher dietary protein intake and better cognitive function only in women.
Meat (g/day)
Men
(Normal: 39.2 ± 47.4, Inadequate: 40.9 ± 50.2, Poor: 46.7 ± 47.3)
p-value n.s
r = −0.004,
p-value n.s
Women
(Normal: 37.7 ± 52.5, Inadequate: 34.6 ± 57.1, Poor: 20.7 ± 31.1)
p-value n.s
r = 0.096, p-value n.s
No significant association between meat intake and cognitive function.
Thiamine (mg/day)
Men
(Normal: 0.95 ± 0.35, Inadequate: 0.91 ± 0.34, Poor: 0.82 ± 0.27),
r = 0.083, p-value n.s
Women
(Normal: 0.91 ± 0.39, Inadequate: 0.90 ± 0.63, Poor: 0.71 ± 0.35)
p-value < 0.05,
r = 0.125, p-value n.s
Significantly higher intake of dietary thiamine in women normal cognition participants than cognitively impaired participants but not significant in men.
AgeIV, Neutral
Requejo et al. 2003 [37], Spain168 free-living elderly at day centres with normal cognition, age between 65 and 90 years.MMSE (unsatisfactory: <28, satisfactory: ≥28) 7-day food diary and 5-day weighted food record of lunch Meat (g/day)
Age ≥ 75 years
(MMSE < 28: 126.3 ± 66.6, MMSE ≥ 28: 98.9 ± 32.1)
Age < 75 years
(MMSE < 28: 127.6 ± 60.9, MMSE ≥ 28: 138.5 ± 77.7)
p-value n.s
No significant association between higher meat consumption with better cognitive function.
Thiamine
Age ≥ 75 years
(MMSE < 28: 1.05 ± 0.29, MMSE ≥ 28: 0.96 ± 0.23)
Age < 75 years
(MMSE < 28: 1.05 ± 0.29, MMSE ≥ 28: 1.12 ± 0.34)
p-value < 0.1 almost sig
r = 0.2332,
p-value < 0.01
Significant association between higher dietary thiamine intake and better cognitive function.
NoneIV, Neutral
Rahman et al. 2007 [38], USA1056 community dwelling elderly, mean age = 67.MSQ (cognitive impairment: <9, normal: ≥9)
Verbal FFQ (Yes: Once or twice a week, or most days, or everday.
No: Less often than once a week, or never.)
Pork, beef, lamb (g/day)
Yes (n = 904)
No (n = 152)
OR = 1.11 (0.67, 1.84)
p-value n.s
Chicken and turkey
Yes (n = 916)
No (n = 140)
OR= 0.81 (0.48, 1.36)
p-value n.s
No significant association between pork, beef, lamb, chicken or turkey.
Age, Gender, Education and Other dietary factorsIV
Mori et al. 2010 [39], Japan179 community dwelling elderly, aged ≥65 yearsSF-36 MCS (High MCS and Low MCS based on standardised score classified by age, 60–69 years = 52.0 and ≥70 years = 51.7 )
Semi-quantitative FFQ
Protein (g/day)
(High: 74.5 ± 1 Low: 73.5 ± 1.6)
p-value n.s
No significant association between dietary protein intake and cognitive function.
Meat (g/day)
(High: 44.0 ± 3.3 Low: 55.3 ± 5.3)
p-value n.s
No significant association between meat and cognition.
Age, GenderIV, Neutral
Aparicio Vizuete et al. 2010 [40], Spain178 institutionalised elderly, age ≥ 65 years.SMPSQ (0 = No error, >0 = Error) 7-day Weighed Food RecordsProtein (g/day)
Age < P50 years
(No error: 71.01 ± 14.30, Error: 67.63 ± 12.68)
Age ≥ P50 years
(No error: 70.02 ± 12.89, Error: 67.98 ± 10.67)
p-value n.s
r2 = 0.5899
p-value < 0.001
Significantly higher dietary protein and meat intake in participants with better cognitive functioning.
Meat
Age < P50 years
(No error: 98.14 ± 41.67, Error: 105.55 ± 40.38)
Age ≥ P50 years
(No error: 93.05 ± 39.81,
Error: 88.90 ± 35.73)
p-value n.s
r2 = 0.1086 p-value < 0.001
Significantly higher meat intake in participants with poorer cognitive functioning.
Thiamine (mg/day)
Age < P50 years
(No error: 1.11 ± 0.28, Error: 1.10 ± 0.24)
Age ≥ P50 years
(No error: 1.12 ± 0.25, Error: 1.09 ± 0.25)
p-value n.s
r2 = 0.3180
p-value < 0.001
Significant association between higher dietary thiamine intake and better cognitive function.
Energy intake and Education levelIV, Neutral
Katsiardanis et al. 2013 [41], Greece557 free-living elderly (m = 237, w = 320), age > 65 yearsMMSE (cognitive impairment: <24, normal: ≥24), GDS.
FFQ and Semi-quantitative FFQ
Protein (g/day)
Men
(Cognitive impairment: 82.5 ± 28.84, Normal: 81.0 ± 23.57)
p-value n.s
OR = 1.36 (0.92–2.02),
p-value n.s
Women
(Cognitive impairment: 75.5 ± 24.34, Normal: 74.8 ± 28.59)
p-value n.s
OR = 0.88 (0.56–1.37),
p-value n.s
No significant association between dietary protein intake and cognitive functioning.
Meat and Meat product
Men
(Cognitive impairment: 24.0 ± 14.67, Normal: 22.0 ± 10.25)
p-value n.s
OR = 1.03 (0.84–1.27),
p-value n.s
Women
(Cognitive impairment: 18.8 ± 11.47, Normal: 19.8 ± 12.27)
p-value n.s
OR = 0.96 (0.81–1.16),
p-value n.s
No significant association between meat and meat products with cognitive function.
Thiamine
Men
OR = 1.05 (0.76–1.44)
p-value n.s
Women
OR = 1.16 (0.65–1.38)
p-value n.s
No significant association between dietary thiamine intake and cognitive function.
Age, Education, Social Activity, Smoking, Metabolic syndrome, Geriatric Depression Scale and MedDiet Score. OR adjusted for core models and energy intake.IV, Neutral
Abbrevations: DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination; PMSQ, Pfeiffer’s Mental Status Questionnaire; FFQ, Food Frequency Questionnaire; SF-36 MCS, Short Form (36) Mental Component Score; SPMSQ, Short Portable Mental State Questionnaire.

3.1. Dietary Protein Intake

Out of the 11 studies, only six studies showed positive association between dietary protein intake and cognitive function [27,32,33,34,36,40]. Two out of the six studies were associations found in women, the study by Lee et al. [36] reported significantly lower protein intake in subjects with poor cognitive function and a positive association between protein intake and Mini Mental State Examination for Koreans (MMSE-K). Nes et al. [34] also reported women with dementia to have significantly lower protein intake compared to the control group. A study found significant association between dietary protein intake with specific cognitive components such as verbal and nonverbal learning and memory [27].

3.2. Different Types of Protein Food Sources

Eight of the reviewed studies investigated meat protein sources intake as a variable. Of these, only one study found a significant correlation between a higher meat intake and poorer cognitive functioning [40] while the remaining studies found no significant association in either direction. One study found that the incidence of AD was the lowest when meat was consumed 4–6 times/week, compared to when meat was consumed daily or ≤3 times/week [24]. However, there was no significant association between risk for all-cause dementia and consumption of meat [24].

3.3. Dietary Thiamine Intake

Based on available mean dietary intake of thiamine from the eligible studies, the mean dietary thiamine intake ranged between 0.7 and 1.51 mg/day. Seven of the nine studies regarding thiamine intake reported a significant association with higher intakes relating to better cognitive function [26,27,34,35,36,37,40]. The study by La Rue et al. [27] showed a positive association between dietary thiamine intake and abstract reasoning however there was no significant association between dietary thiamine intake and visuospatial skills or nonverbal learning and memory. In one study, women with poor cognition were shown to have significantly lower thiamine intake than women with normal cognition however there was no significant association between dietary thiamine intake and MMSE-K scores [36]. There are difficulties in interpreting the study outcomes as one of the two studies that reported no significant association was observed to have the highest mean thiamine intake among the other recorded means [29] while the other negative study did not report mean thiamine intake [41].

4. Discussion

The evidence regarding the association between dietary protein and cognitive function is weak which is similar to the findings of the previous literature review conducted by Van de Rest et al. [42]. Additionally, it is difficult to isolate the effects of protein on cognitive function as protein intake is directly associated with energy intake contributing to a potential effect through associations with nutritional status. Only four studies included adjustments for total energy, three of which showed no significant association after these adjustments. The only study that showed an association suggested that there was a protective effect when protein intake contributed 16%–20% of total energy intake, a level of intake that is considered to be a moderately high [32].
Even at the same protein content, different foods may impact cognition differently because of their specific macronutrient profile (amino acids and fatty acids composition) or micronutrient content (iodine, thiamine, folic acid, vitamin B12, etc.). As protein is available in various sources of foods, we investigated further to report the evidence available for specific meat sources however the evidences available were very weak, limited and no trends were identified. Although meat is a high protein food, the amounts of saturated fat present is highly dependent on the type of meat and the cut. A majority of the reviewed studies in relation to meat products found no significant association. One cross-sectional study reported higher meat consumption to be associated with poorer cognitive functioning [40]. However, in that study the association between meat intake and errors identified in Short Portable Mental State Questionnaire (SPMSQ) differed according to age of participants. A beneficial association was found between a higher meat intake and better performance in those aged below the median, while in those whose age was above the median, the association was in the opposite direction. [40]. Curiously, the authors did not perform analyses, which included age as a continuous variable, which is a limitation of the study [40].
Literature related to thiamine intake presented inconsistent findings. Those studies that reported an association between higher intake and better cognition were mainly cross-sectional. Only two cohort studies were identified and one showed a positive association [27] while the other study showed no association between dietary thiamine intake and cognitive function [29]. The overall health status of the study participants also needs to be considered as lifestyle factors such as physical activity, smoking, alcohol consumption and even anthropometric characteristics are well known potential confounding factors that are related to both dietary intake and cognitive function.
Our findings were somewhat surprising. Thiamine is an essential nutrient involved in brain metabolic and cellular functions, including carbohydrate metabolism and neurotransmitter production, notably acetylcholine and gamma-amino butyric acid (GABA). Reduced synthesis of acetylcholine leads to cognitive disorders such as delirium [43] while thiamine deficiency in older adults is associated with depression and Alzheimer’s Disease [14]. Our present review excluded studies of older people with marked cognitive dysfunction. Thus, the role of thiamine may only be evident in patients with senile dementia. Indeed, Alzheimer’s patients tend to have lower plasma thiamine concentrations and higher rates of thiamine deficiency compared to patients without SDAT [44,45]. It has been suggested from animal studies that thiamine deficiency damages neurons and results in tissue loss in the brain [46]. It has been hypothesised that therapeutic doses of thiamin intake may be beneficial in the treatment of neurodegenerative diseases [16], however our review does not support that view for healthy older adults.
The limited number of studies and a lack of randomized controlled trials indicate that there is insufficient evidence to draw conclusions regarding the role of either protein or thiamine on cognitive function in older adults. Observational data, apart from sufficiently long duration prospective cohort studies, of which there were only seven, is unable to provide information on temporality, in order to determine whether the intake or lack of intake of a nutrient caused cognitive decline or if the progressive cognitive decline caused the reduced intake. The appropriate type of study design is often questioned as cognitive function is also known to decrease with age and dementia is a progressive disease. For instance, nutritional supplementation is often used instead of food sources in experimental studies due to their convenience, cost effectiveness and higher levels of control during interventions. However, provision of nutrients within a food matrix is likely to be more effective than single nutrients alone as has been shown for the relationship between vitamin E and prostate cancer. In a large prospective study, γ-tocopherol, a form of vitamin E obtained through diet had a protective effect against prostate cancerin comparison to α-tocopherol, the more commonly available form of supplemental vitamin E that showed no association to prostate cancer risks [47]. Another study showed that supplemental vitamin E had no effect on reducing risk of prostate cancer in healthy men and was suggested to have adverse effects at high doses [48].
Comparison of the study outcomes is limited by the variety of cognitive tests used. The MMSE was the most preferred and common form of cognitive testing, however reference cut-off points differ between studies and population groups. Most of the studies included cognitive tests that had been validated for their study population except for two studies which had no information provided regarding this issue [27,28]. Additionally, generalizability of the findings is hampered by the diversity of study populations and their different dietary patterns.

5. Conclusions

There is insufficient evidence to support an association between dietary intake of either protein and/or thiamine and cognitive functioning in healthy older people. A lack of experimental studies prevents the translation of information into dietary messages for optimal cognitive functioning. Undoubtedly, there is a need for further well designed cohort and experimental studies in this area.

Acknowledgments

A grant from the Pork Co-Operative Research Centre funded this work, as part of a larger research study grant.

Author Contributions

F.K. performed the literature search, summarised the data and wrote the first draft of the paper. K.C., K.W. and A.M. refined the search terms and research question, provided supervision and edited the article.

Appendix

Table A1. Quality rating process.
Table A1. Quality rating process.
Study (1st Author, Year)Nes et al. 1988Burns et al. 1989Ortega et al. 1997La Rue et al. 1997Lee et al. 2001Deschamps et al. 2002Requejo et al. 2003Barberger et al. 2007Rahman et al. 2007Shatenstein et al. 2007Velho et al. 2008Vercambre et al. 2009Aparicio Vizuete et al. 2010Mori et al. 2010McNeill et al. 2011Roberts et al. 2012Katsiardanis et al. 2013
Reference Number[34][33][35][27][36][28][37][24][38][26][30][31][40][39][29][32][41]
VALIDITY QUESTIONS—PRIMARY STUDIES
1. Was the research question clearly stated?YYYYYYYYYYYYYYYYY
2. Was the selection of study subjects/patients free from bias?YYNYYYYYYYNYYNYYY
3. Were study groups comparable?YYNANANANANANNAYNANANANANANANA
4. Were intervention/therapeutic regimens/exposure factor or procedure and any comparison(s) described in detail? Were intervening factors described?YYYYYNYNNYYYYYYYY
5. Were outcomes clearly defined and the measurements valid and reliable?YYYYYYYYYYYYYYYYY
6. Was method of handling withdrawals described?YNNNNYNNNYYYNNNYY
7. Was blinding used to prevent introduction of bias?NYNNNNNNNNNNNNNNN
8. Was the statistical analysis appropriate for the study design and type of outcome indicators?NNYYYYNYYNYYYYYYY
9. Were conclusions supported by results with biases and limitations taken into consideration?YNYYYYNYYYYYYYYYY
10. Is bias due to study’s funding or sponsorship unlikely?YYYYYYYYYYYYYYYYY
OVERALL QUALITY—PRIMARY STUDIES
Negative/Neutral/Positive (N/0/P)PP0000000P0000000
If most (six or more) of the answers to the above validity questions are “No,” the report should be designated negative
If the answers to validity criteria questions 2, 3, 6, and 7 do not indicate that the study is exceptionally strong, the report should be designated neutral
If most of the answers to the above validity questions are “Yes” (including criteria 2, 3, 6, 7 and at least one additional “Yes”), the report should be designated positive
Sum
Yes (Y)77677756887876788
No (N)33322245322123211
Not Applicable (NA)00111110101111111

Conflicts of Interest

This review was conducted as part of a larger study that received funding from a research grant provided by the Pork Co-Operative Research Centre. The publication charges of the manuscript were included under this grant amount. The funding body was not involved in the collection, analysis, interpretation or presentation of data. Karen Charlton receives an honorarium from the Pork Co-operative Research Centre for her work as a sub-programme manager (3B: Inherent Properties of Australian Pork to Enhance Consumer Health). The other authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Spencer, J.P.E. Flavonoids: Modulators of brain function? Br. J. Nutr. 2008, 99, ES60–ES77. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  2. Del Parigi, A.; Panza, F.; Capurso, C.; Solfrizzi, V. Nutritional factors, cognitive decline, and dementia. Brain Res. Bull. 2006, 69, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Luchsinger, J.A.; Tang, M.X.; Shea, S.; Mayeux, R. Antioxidant vitamin intake and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 2003, 60, 203–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Otaegui-Arrazola, A.; Amiano, P.; Elbusto, A.; Urdaneta, E.; Martínez-Lage, P. Diet, cognition, and Alzheimer’s disease: Food for thought. Eur. J. Nutr. 2014, 53, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Singh, B.; Parsaik, A.K.; Mielke, M.M.; Erwin, P.J.; Knopman, D.S.; Petersen, R.C.; Roberts, R.O. Association of Mediterranean diet with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2014, 39, 271–282. [Google Scholar]
  6. Morais, J.A.; Chevalier, S.; Gougeon, R. Protein turnover and requirements in the healthy and frail elderly. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2006, 10, 272–283. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  7. Robertson, D.A.; Savva, G.M.; Kenny, R.A. Frailty and cognitive impairment—A review of the evidence and causal mechanisms. Ageing Res. Rev. 2013, 12, 840–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Orsitto, G. Different components of nutritional status in older inpatients with cognitive impairment. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2012, 16, 468–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Jakobsen, L.H.; Kondrup, J.; Zellner, M.; Tetens, I.; Roth, E. Effect of a high protein meat diet on muscle and cognitive functions: A randomised controlled dietary intervention trial in healthy men. Clin. Nutr. 2011, 30, 303–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Walrand, S.; Guillet, C.; Salles, J.; Cano, N.; Boirie, Y. Physiopathological mechanism of sarcopenia. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 2011, 27, 365–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Gibson, E.L.; Green, M.W. Nutritional influences on cognitive function: Mechanisms of susceptibility. Nutr. Res. Rev. 2002, 15, 169–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Gibson, G.E.; Hirsch, J.A.; Cirio, R.T.; Jordan, B.D.; Fonzetti, P.; Elder, J. Abnormal thiamine-dependent processes in Alzheimer’s Disease. Lessons from diabetes. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 2013, 55, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. O’Keeffe, S.T.; Tormey, W.P.; Glasgow, R.; Lavan, J.N. Thiamine deficiency in hospitalized elderly patients. Gerontology 1994, 40, 18–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Pepersack, T.; Garbusinski, J.; Robberecht, J.; Beyer, I.; Willems, D.; Fuss, M. Clinical relevance of thiamine status amongst hospitalized elderly patients. Gerontology 1999, 45, 96–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Gibson, G.E.; Sheu, K.F.R.; Blass, J.P.; Baker, A.; Carlson, K.C.; Harding, B.; Perrino, P. Reduced activities of thiamine-dependent enzymes in the brains and peripheral tissues of patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Arch. Neurol. 1988, 45, 836–840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Rodríguez-Martín, J.L.; Qizilbash, N.; López-Arrieta, J.M. Thiamine for Alzheimer’s disease. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2001, 2, CD001498. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  17. Meador, K.; Loring, D.; Nichols, M.; Zamrini, E.; Rivner, M.; Posas, H.; Thompson, E.; Moore, E. Preliminary findings of high-dose thiamine in dementia of Alzheimer’s type. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 1993, 6, 222–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G.; The PRSIMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. American Dietetic Association. Evidence Analysis Manual. Steps in the ADA Evidence Analysis Process; Scientific Affairs and Research: Chicago, IL, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  20. National Health and Medical Research Council. How to Review the Evidence: SystematicIdentification and Review of the Scientific Literature; NHMRC: Canberra, Australia, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  21. Gao, L.Y.; Dong, B.R.; Hao, Q.K.; Ding, X. Association between cognitive impairment and eating habits in elderly Chinese subjects over 90 years of age. J. Int. Med. Res. 2013, 41, 1362–1369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Wang, Z.; Dong, B.; Zeng, G.; Li, J.; Wang, W.; Wang, B.; Yuan, Q. Is there an association between mild cognitive impairment and dietary pattern in Chinese elderly? Results from a cross-sectional population study. BMC Public Health 2010, 10, 595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Chan, R.; Chan, D.; Woo, J. A cross sectional study to examine the association between dietary patterns and cognitive impairment in older Chinese people in Hong Kong. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2013, 17, 757–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Barberger-Gateau, P.; Raffaitin, C.; Letenneur, L.; Berr, C.; Tzourio, C.; Dartigues, JF.; Alpérovitch, A. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: The Three-City cohort study. Neurology 2007, 69, 1921–1930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Jesus, P.; Desport, J.C.; Massoulard, A.; Villemonteix, C.; Baptiste, A.; Gindre-Poulvelarie, L.; Lorgueuilleux, S.; Javerliat, V.; Fraysse, J.L.; Preux, P.M. Nutritional assessment and follow-up of residents with and without dementia in nursing homes in the Limousin region of France: A health network initiative. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2012, 16, 504–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Shatenstein, B.; Kergoat, M.; Reid, I. Poor nutrient intakes during 1-year follow-up with community-dwelling older adults with early-stage Alzheimer dementia compared to cognitively intact matched controls. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2007, 107, 2091–2099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. LaRue, A.; Koehler, K.M.; Wayne, S.J.; Chiulli, S.J.; Haaland, K.Y.; Garry, P.J. Nutritional status and cognitive functioning in a normally aging sample: A 6-y reassessment. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1997, 65, 20–29. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  28. Deschamps, V.; Astier, X.; Ferry, M.; Rainfray, M.; Emeriau, J.P.; Barberger-Gateau, P. Nutritional status of healthy elderly persons living in Dordogne, France, and relation with mortality and cognitive or functional decline. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2002, 56, 305–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. McNeill, G.; Jia, X.; Whalley, L.J.; Fox, H.C.; Corley, J.; Gow, A.J.; Brett, C.E.; Starr, J.M.; Deary, I.J. Antioxidant and B vitamin intake in relation to cognitive function in later life in the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2011, 65, 619–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Velho, S.; Marques-Vidal, P.; Baptista, F.; Camilo, M.E. Dietary intake adequacy and cognitive function in free-living active elderly: A cross-sectional and short-term prospective study. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 27, 77–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Vercambre, M.N.; Boutron-Ruault, M.C.; Ritchie, K.; Clavel-Chapelon, F.; Berr, C. Long-term association of food and nutrient intakes with cognitive and functional decline: A 13-year follow-up study of elderly French women. Br. J. Nutr. 2009, 102, 419–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Roberts, R.O.; Roberts, L.A.; Geda, Y.E.; Cha, R.H.; Pankratz, V.S.; O’Connor, H.M.; Knopman, D.S.; Petersen, R.C. Relative intake of macronutrients impacts risk of mild cognitive impairment or dementia. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2012, 32, 329–339. [Google Scholar]
  33. Burns, A.; Marsh, A.; Bender, D.A. Dietary intake and clinical, anthropometric and biochemical indices of malnutrition in elderly demented patients and non-demented subjects. Psychol. Med. 1989, 19, 383–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Nes, M.; Sem, S.W.; Rousseau, B.; Bjorneboe, G.E.A.; Engedal, K.; Trygg, K.; Pedersen, J.I. Dietary intakes and nutritional status of old people with dementia living at home in Oslo. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 1988, 42, 581–593. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  35. Ortega, R.M.; Requejo, A.M.; Andres, P.; Lopez-Sobaler, A.M.; Quintas, M.E.; Redondo, M.R.; Navia, B.; Rivas, T. Dietary intake and cognitive function in a group of elderly people. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1997, 66, 803–809. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  36. Lee, L.; Kang, S.A.; Lee, H.O.; Lee, B.H.; Park, J.S.; Kim, J.H.; Jung, I.K.; Park, Y.J.; Lee, J.E. Relationships between dietary intake and cognitive function level in Korean elderly people. Public Health (Nat.) 2001, 115, 133–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Requejo, A.M.; Ortega, R.M.; Robles, F.; Navia, B.; Faci, M.; Aparicio, A. Influence of nutrition on cognitive function in a group of elderly, independently living people. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2003, 57, S54–S57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Rahman, A.; Baker, P.S.; Allman, R.M.; Zamrini, E. Dietary factors and cognitive impairment in community-dwelling elderly. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2007, 11, 49–54. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  39. Mori, K.; Kawano, Y.; Tada, Y.; Hida, A.; Nagasawa, N.; Inoue, K.; Kamioka, H.; Inoue, K.; Ozeki, T. Relationship of dietary intake and lifestyle factors to health-related quality of life in the community-dwelling elderly. J. Nutr. Sci. Vitaminol. 2010, 56, 364–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Aparicio Vizuete, A.; Robles, F.; Rodríguez-Rodríguez, E.; López-Sobaler, A.M.; Ortega, R.M. Association between food and nutrient intakes and cognitive capacity in a group of institutionalized elderly people. Eur. J. Nutr. 2010, 49, 293–300. [Google Scholar]
  41. Katsiardanis, K.; Diamantaras, A.A.; Dessypris, N.; Michelakos, T.; Anastasiou, A.; Katsiardani, K.P.; Kanavidis, P.; Papadopoulos, F.C.; Stefanadis, C.; Panagiotakos, D.B.; et al. Cognitive Impairment and Dietary Habits Among Elders: The Velestino Study. J. Med. Food 2013, 16, 343–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Van de Rest, O.; van der Zwaluw, N.L.; de Groot, L.C. Literature review on the role of dietary protein and amino acids in cognitive functioning and cognitive decline. Amino Acids 2013, 45, 1035–1045. [Google Scholar]
  43. Gibson, G.E.; Blass, J.P.; Huang, H.M.; Freeman, G.B. The cellular basis of delirium and its relevance to age-related disorders including Alzheimer’s disease. Int. Psychogeriatr./IPA 1991, 3, 373–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Gold, M.; Chen, M.F.; Johnson, K. Plasma and red blood cell thiamine deficiency in patients with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Arch. Neurol. 1995, 52, 1081–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Gibson, G.E.; Blass, J.P. Thiamine-dependent processes and treatment strategies in neurodegeneration. Antioxid. Redox Signal. 2007, 9, 1605–1619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Langlais, P.J.; Savage, L.M. Thiamine deficiency in rats produces cognitive and memory deficits on spatial tasks that correlate with tissue loss in diencephalon, cortex and white matter. Behav. Brain Res. 1995, 68, 75–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Wright, M.E.; Weinstein, S.J.; Lawson, K.A.; Albanes, D.; Subar, A.F.; Dixon, L.B.; Mouw, T.; Schatzkin, A.; Leitzmann, M.F. Supplemental and dietary vitamin E intakes and risk of prostate cancer in a large prospective study. Cancer Epidemiol. Biomarkers Prev. 2007, 16, 1128–1135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Lippman, S.M.; Klein, E.A.; Goodman, P.J.; Lucia, M.S.; Thompson, I.M.; Ford, L.G.; Parnes, H.L.; Minasian, L.M.; Gaziano, J.M.; Hartline, J.A.; et al. Effect of selenium and vitamin E on risk of prostate cancer and other cancers: The selenium and vitamin E cancer prevention trial (SELECT). J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2009, 301, 39–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Koh, F.; Charlton, K.; Walton, K.; McMahon, A.-T. Role of Dietary Protein and Thiamine Intakes on Cognitive Function in Healthy Older People: A Systematic Review. Nutrients 2015, 7, 2415-2439. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7042415

AMA Style

Koh F, Charlton K, Walton K, McMahon A-T. Role of Dietary Protein and Thiamine Intakes on Cognitive Function in Healthy Older People: A Systematic Review. Nutrients. 2015; 7(4):2415-2439. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7042415

Chicago/Turabian Style

Koh, Freda, Karen Charlton, Karen Walton, and Anne-Therese McMahon. 2015. "Role of Dietary Protein and Thiamine Intakes on Cognitive Function in Healthy Older People: A Systematic Review" Nutrients 7, no. 4: 2415-2439. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7042415

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop