Next Article in Journal
No Consistent Antidepressant Effects of Deep Brain Stimulation of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis
Previous Article in Journal
Use of Ordered Beta Regression Unveils Cognitive Flexibility Index and Longitudinal Cognitive Training Signatures in Normal and Alzheimer’s Disease Pathological Aging
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Computational Model for the Simulation of Prepulse Inhibition and Its Modulation by Cortical and Subcortical Units

by
Thiago Ohno Bezerra
1,
Antonio C. Roque
2 and
Cristiane Salum
1,3,*
1
Center of Mathematics, Computation and Cognition, Universidade Federal do ABC, São Bernardo do Campo 09606-045, Brazil
2
Department of Physics, School of Philosophy, Sciences and Letters of Ribeirão Preto, University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto 14040-901, Brazil
3
Interdisciplinary Applied Neuroscience Unit, Universidade Federal do ABC, São Bernardo do Campo 09606-045, Brazil
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(5), 502; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14050502
Submission received: 19 April 2024 / Revised: 10 May 2024 / Accepted: 11 May 2024 / Published: 15 May 2024

Abstract

:
The sensorimotor gating is a nervous system function that modulates the acoustic startle response (ASR). Prepulse inhibition (PPI) phenomenon is an operational measure of sensorimotor gating, defined as the reduction of ASR when a high intensity sound (pulse) is preceded in milliseconds by a weaker stimulus (prepulse). Brainstem nuclei are associated with the mediation of ASR and PPI, whereas cortical and subcortical regions are associated with their modulation. However, it is still unclear how the modulatory units can influence PPI. In the present work, we developed a computational model of a neural circuit involved in the mediation (brainstem units) and modulation (cortical and subcortical units) of ASR and PPI. The activities of all units were modeled by the leaky-integrator formalism for neural population. The model reproduces basic features of PPI observed in experiments, such as the effects of changes in interstimulus interval, prepulse intensity, and habituation of ASR. The simulation of GABAergic and dopaminergic drugs impaired PPI by their effects over subcortical units activity. The results show that subcortical units constitute a central hub for PPI modulation. The presented computational model offers a valuable tool to investigate the neurobiology associated with disorder-related impairments in PPI.

1. Introduction

The acoustic startle response (ASR) is characterized by a motor reflex in response to a sudden high intensity and salient sound (called pulse) [1]. This reflex is modulated by the sensorimotor gating, a function of nervous system that prevents the brain from an overflow of information by avoiding interference of other stimuli during information processing [2]. The prepulse inhibition (PPI) phenomenon is an operational measure of sensorimotor gating, defined as the reduction in ASR amplitude to a pulse when this is preceded, in milliseconds, by a weaker stimulus (the prepulse). The prepulse on its own does not generate an ASR response.
Brainstem nuclei are the core regions involved in the mediation of ASR and PPI. Electrophysiological, pharmacological, and lesion experiments conducted in animals [1,3,4,5] showed that the cochlear root nucleus, the caudal pontine reticular nucleus, and the motor neurons in the spinal cord mediate the ASR. Similarly, the inferior and superior colliculi and the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus are responsible for the inhibition of ASR circuit and generating the PPI [5,6]. More recent works with PET scan in rats corroborated that these same units are involved in the mediation of ASR and PPI [7,8].
Other studies showed that the cortical and subcortical regions are associated with the modulation of both ASR and PPI. Injection of a GABAergic agonist into the amygdala in rats [9] or optogenetic activation of the amygdala in mice [10] and muscimol injection into the rat nucleus accumbens [11] led to deficits in the PPI. PET scan studies conducted with rats also showed the involvement of the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and ventral pallidum [7,8]. Similarly, dopaminergic transmission also modulates PPI. Dopaminergic receptors are expressed in the amygdala, prefrontal cortex, and striatum [12,13,14,15,16,17]. Administration of dopaminergic agonists into the rat amygdala [18,19], nucleus accumbens [20], or via systemic injection [18,21,22,23] affects PPI. Because dopamine signals stimulus salience to the nucleus accumbens [24], it may be linked to regulating the flow of information to other brain regions by controlling sensorimotor gating [22,25]. Indeed, systemic administration and injection into the rat nucleus accumbens of D 1 / D 2 or D 2 receptor agonists, such as amphetamine, apomorphine, and quinpirole, reduce PPI [20,21,26,27,28], while D 2 antagonists promote an increase [29,30]. Evidence shows a more robust PPI deficit caused by D 1 receptor agonists administration in mice [31] compared to rats [21,31,32]. The differences in PPI modulation by D 1 and D 2 receptors may stem from their differing expression levels in the striatum and the distinct neural pathways within this region [13,33,34].
Pathological conditions, such as Parkinson’s and Huntington’s diseases, [35,36] and psychiatric disorders, as schizophrenia, bipolar, and anxiety disorders [35,37,38,39,40], show deficits in the sensorimotor gating revealed by reduced PPI [41,42], making this test a valuable tool to understand disorders of the nervous system. In particular, a recent meta-analysis showed a significant reduction in PPI of patients with schizophrenia [40]. This psychiatric disorder was associated with functional alterations in the striatum and in the striatal dopaminergic transmission (for a review, see [43]), and with increased amygdala activity and reduced prefrontal cortex metabolism [44,45]. Chronic administration of amphetamine can stimulate dopaminergic system activity and increase dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens [46], partially mimicking the hyperdopaminergia observed in the associative striatum in patients with schizophrenia [47]. These alterations are accompanied by deficits in PPI [46]. In addition, neurodevelopmental animal models of schizophrenia also show activity alterations in the ventral tegmental area, basolateral amygdala, and prefrontal cortex associated with PPI impairment [46,48]. The study of how alterations and dopaminergic transmission in these regions affect PPI could offer insights about the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.
However, it is not yet clear how these modulatory units and the dopaminergic transmission modulate both ASR and PPI. Previous computational models of PPI implemented and simulated brain stem units related to ASR and PPI [49,50,51]. These models reproduced some basic features of PPI detected in animal and human experiments, such as the effect of the variation of the prepulse intensity and the interstimulus interval (ISI) [52,53,54,55]. A recent computational model studied the cellular mechanism for the PPI in the Drosophila larvae [56,57], also showing that a similar cellular circuit controls the PPI. However, these models relied mostly on the brainstem units and did not implement neither the units related to the modulation of ASR and PPI nor the dopaminergic transmission. As a result, it was impossible to study the involvement of the modulatory components in these computational models.
In this work, we present a computational model that simulates ASR and PPI by implementing the modulatory units (amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, ventral pallidum) and dopaminergic transmission (represented by the ventral tegmental area and its connections). Schizophrenic patients present, in general, a significant deficit in PPI and this disease is extensively investigated via pharmacological studies in animal models [40,46,48]. Therefore, in this work, we focused on implementing the modulatory components of PPI that have been associated with schizophrenia to understand how these units interact and impact PPI. The model was used to reproduce basic features of PPI (effect of the variation of prepulse intensity, ISI, and the habituation in ASR) and to simulate the effects of systemic or local (amygdala, ventral pallidum, and nucleus accumbens) GABAergic and dopaminergic drugs. With these tests, we characterized the role of the modulatory units on PPI and ASR.

2. Methods

The model includes three different neural pathways, with the first associated with the mediation of ASR, the second associated with the mediation of PPI, and the third associated with the modulation of both ASR and PPI (Figure 1). Each unit in the model represents a different structure of the nervous system that was associated to ASR and PPI in animal studies [5,7,8]. The model input, representing a sound stimulus that reaches the cochlea, is processed through the network, and elicits a motor response represented by the activity of motor neurons in the ASR pathway. The magnitudes of the sound stimuli were chosen to reflect the sound intensities used in experimental settings [58,59]. The model parameters were adjusted to simulate a virtual background with an intensity of 60 dB. Every sound stimuli simulated in this work is considered above a virtual background of 60 dB. Hence, an input I = 60 represents a sound with intensity 60 dB above background, i.e., it represents a sound with intensity of 120 dB.
The activity of each unit was modeled by the leaky-integrator model for population activity [50]:
d A d t = 1 τ A + D · G A B A · E · I ,
where A represents the unit activity, τ is the time constant, D is the activity of dopaminergic receptors, G A B A represents the GABAergic modulation factor (used in drug tests as described in the Section Test Procedures below), E and I are the excitatory and inhibitory inputs, respectively.
Unless otherwise specified, the excitatory and inhibitory inputs were given by the equations:
E = e E e 2 k e 2 + e 2
I = i I 1 i 2 k i 2 + i 2 ,
where E and I are, respectively, the sets of all units that make excitatory and inhibitory connections to unit A, and k e and k i are semi-saturation constants that control, respectively, the strengths of each excitatory and inhibitory input. Since the excitatory inputs were assumed to be independent of each other (with the exception of the NAc input, as specified in the Appendix A. Model Equations), the total excitatory input to unit A was implemented by a sum. In contrast, the total inhibitory input was implemented by a product so that a single inhibitory input, if sufficiently strong, can silence the unit.
Dopaminergic modulation was implemented by D 1 and D 2 dopamine receptors, with activities given by the sigmoid functions [60]:
D 1 = 1 + D m a x 1 + e k D A ( M V T A + D A l D 1 )
D 2 = 1 D m a x 1 + e k D A ( M V T A + D A l D 2 ) ,
where D m a x is the maximum effect of dopaminergic receptor activation, k D A is the slope of the sigmoid function, l D 1 and l D 2 are, respectively, the semi-saturation constants for D 1 and D 2 receptor activations, D A is a factor that simulates the effect of dopaminergic agonists and antagonists (more details in the Test Procedures Section below), and M V T A is the activity of VTA. The units receiving dopaminergic modulation are Amyg, mPFC, NAc, and VP.
In order to implement a threshold function, we used the p function defined as follows:
p ( x l ) = 1 if x > l 0 otherwise ,
where l represents the threshold value. This threshold function was implemented in the equations for CPRN, NAc, VTA, phasic released dopamine, and the synaptic weight in the CRN–CPRN connection.
All the model equations and parameters are described in more detail in the Appendix A. Model Equations and Appendix B. Model Parameters.

2.1. Test Procedures

A PPI trial is defined as a sequence of stimulus presentations. A pulse alone trial consists of the presentation of a single high intensity sound stimulus (60 dB above background). This trial was denoted as P60. A prepulse + pulse trial consists of the presentation of a prepulse with an intensity of X dB above background followed by a pulse of 60 dB. It was denoted by PPX + P60. The time interval between the prepulse and the pulse is called the interstimulus interval (ISI). The percentage of inhibition is defined by
% P P I = 100 × m a x ( A M N , P ) m a x ( A M N , P P ) m a x ( A M N , P ) ,
where A M N is the motor neuron activity, and m a x ( A M N , P ) and m a x ( A M N , P P ) are the maximum values of A M N for pulse trials and prepulse + pulse trials, respectively. Greater %PPI values mean high inhibition, while lower %PPI values indicate low inhibition. We chose to calculate the %PPI to facilitate the comparison with the PPI literature.
In order to test whether the model can reproduce the basic features observed in PPI experiments, we conducted tests varying ISI and prepulse intensity. In the first type of test, the ISI varied from 0 ms to 250 ms in steps of 10 ms between simulations. In this test, the pulse intensity was 60 dB above background and the prepulse intensities were 15, 20, and 25 dB above background. Both stimuli had the duration of 30 ms. In the second type of test, the prepulse intensity varied from 0 dB to 100 dB above background in steps of 5 dB between simulations. The ISIs applied were 60, 70, and 80 ms. The pulse intensity was 60 dB above background and both stimuli had the duration of 30 ms. We calculated the %PPI for each ISI and prepulse intensity in these tests. We chose to test PPI with the above indicated sound intensity above background and ISI values to avoid floor and ceiling effects, and because they are commonly used in PPI experiments [28,52,53,55,61,62].
A PPI session was defined as a sequence of trials of pulse alone, prepulse alone, prepulse + pulse or no stimulus repeated by eight times for each trial type separated by a random intertrial interval (ITI). To verify if the model can produce ASR habituation by repeated pulse presentation, we carried a test with 10 repeated P60 trials. In this test, the pulse intensity was 60 dB above background with duration of 30 ms and the ITI was randomly drawn from an uniform distribution of integers in the intervals [5 s, 15 s], [10 s, 15 s], and [20 s, 25 s].
In order to simulate different conditions, mimicking an experiment with a group of different animals, the PPI session tests (habituation test, GABAergic test, and dopaminergic test) were simulated with variability in the model parameters. In each simulation, the values of all model parameters were randomly drawn from uniform distributions in the intervals centered at the parameter values in Table A1 with widths of 10% of those values in each side. This process was repeated 10 times for each parameter at each new simulation, resulting in 10 different sets of parameters at each simulation to mimic an experiment with 10 different animals. For example, the 10 values of parameter k I at each new simulation were drawn from the uniform distribution in the interval [31.5 dB, 38.5 dB].

2.2. Drug Action Tests

In the tests with the simulation of GABAergic drugs, we used values of the G A B A factor smaller than 1 ( 0.00 G A B A < 1.00 ) to simulate a GABAergic agonist drug, which reduced the activity of the unit under test. In contrast, to simulate the effect of a GABAergic antagonist, we used values of G A B A greater than 1 ( 1.00 < G A B A 2.00 ), which increase the unit activity. The control case was defined as G A B A = 1.00 . The maximum inhibition of the unit occurs for G A B A = 0.00 and the maximum hyperactivation for G A B A = 2.00 .
To test the interaction between Amyg and VP in a GABAergic test, both the GABA factors in Amyg (Equation (A11)) and VP (Equation (A21)) were varied from 0.00 (maximum inhibition) to 2.00 (maximum hyperactivation) in steps of 0.10 between the simulations. In this test, we simulated prepulse + pulse trials of PPI with prepulse intensity of 25 dB above background, pulse of 60 dB above background, ISI of 80 ms, and duration of both stimuli of 30 ms.
We conducted a test simulating a PPI session with GABAergic manipulation in Amyg and VP. The session started with the presentation of 10 pulse trials (60 dB above background, 30 ms) followed by a pseudorandom presentation of 64 trials of pulse, prepulse (15, 20, or 25 dB above background, 30 ms), prepulse + pulse (ISI of 80 ms), or no stimulus. Each trial type was repeated eight times along the session. The ITI was 10–15 s. In this test we analyzed both ASR for the pulse and each prepulse + pulse intensity and the %PPI for each prepulse intensity. To mimic an experiment conducted with ten different animals, the session was simulated ten times with different values of model parameters (as described above). This test had the following groups: Control ( G A B A = 1.00 in Amyg and VP), Amyg group ( G A B A = 0.20 in Amyg), VP group ( G A B A = 0.20 in VP), and Amyg-VP group ( G A B A = 0.20 in Amyg and VP). The average ASR and %PPI between the groups were compared with a two-way repeated measure ANOVA with Trat1 (Control × GABA Amyg) and Trat2 (Control × GABA VP) as between subject factor and intensity (15, 20, or 25 dB above background) as within subject factor. The level of significance was p < 0.050. We performed the statistical analysis using the open platform JAMOVI (release 2.5.2). Tests for normality were made with the Shapiro–Wilk test (see Appendix C) and for homoscedasticity with Mauchly’s sphericity test [63].
The effects of dopaminergic drugs were modeled by using 1.00 D A < 0.00 to simulate the effect of a dopaminergic antagonist, so that the activity of the dopaminergic receptor is reduced; on the other hand, the effect of a dopaminergic agonist was simulated by using 0.00 < D A 1.00 , which increased the activity of the dopaminergic receptor. The control condition was defined as D A = 0.00 . The maximum inhibition of the dopaminergic receptor occurs with D A = 1.00 and the maximum hyperactivation, with D A = 1.00 . In the systemic simulation test, all dopaminergic receptors in the model had their DA factor manipulated. Whereas in the Amyg simulation test, only the dopaminergic receptors in the Amyg unit were manipulated (Equations (A10) and (A11)). In the NAc simulation test, only the dopaminergic receptors in NAc were manipulated (Equations (A15) and (A17)). When simulating drugs with action on both D 1 and D 2 receptors, the DA factor on both receptors was manipulated. For drugs with action on only one of the receptors, only the DA factor of the respective receptor was manipulated.
We also conducted a test simulating a whole PPI session with manipulation of the DA factor. The session was the same as the session in the GABAergic test and we simulated 10 different parameter sets (representing 10 different animals). The dopaminergic session test had the following groups: Control (DA factor in all receptors set to 0.00), Systemic group (DA factor in all receptors set to 0.50), Amyg group (DA factor of D 1 and D 2 receptors in Amyg set to 0.50), and NAc group (DA factor of D 1 and D 2 receptors in NAc set to 0.50). The average ASR and %PPI between the groups were analyzed with a one-way repeated measure ANOVA with Group (Control × Systemic × Amyg × NAc) as between subject factor and intensity (15, 20, or 25 dB above background) as within subject factor. The level of significance was p < 0.050 .

2.3. Computational Methods

The system of differential equations was numerically integrated using the Euler method with time step d t = 0.02 ms. The numerical routines were implemented in Python 3.11.5 using the packages NumPy (1.24.3) and Numba (0.57.1) (for JIT compilation) and the graphs were built with Matplotlib (3.7.2).

3. Results

A PPI trial was simulated by the presentation of a prepulse with intensity of 25 dB above background, a pulse with intensity of 60 dB above background, ISI of 80 ms, and duration of both stimuli of 30 ms. The sound stimuli presentation increased every unit activity (Figure 2). In the ASR pathway, both Ch and CRN present two peaks of activity, the first one corresponding to the prepulse presentation and the second one to the pulse. However, in CPRN, due to the transfer threshold and the inhibitory input from PPTg, there is only one peak of activity corresponding to the pulse with smaller amplitude when compared to the peaks in Ch and CRN. The same is observed in the MN. That reduction in the activity amplitude in the response for the pulse is what characterizes the PPI. A similar response is observed in the PPI pathway. Both IC and SC show two peaks of activity. The PPTg shows one peak with high amplitude corresponding to the prepulse and after that a lower activity caused by the pulse. The activities of the ASR and PPI pathways also propagate to the modulatory pathway. In the units representing the dopaminergic transmission in NAc, it is possible to note slight increases in the extracellular dopamine concentration and in the activity of presynaptic D 2 receptors. The excitatory and inhibitory subpopulations in the mPFC and in the amygdala showed two peak responses, except for the excitatory subpopulation in the amygdala. Since the amygdala’s excitatory subpopulation received a direct inhibitory input from the inhibitory subpopulation, the peak response amplitude for the pulse was attenuated in the excitatory subpopulation. In the same way, both direct and indirect NAc pathways responded for the stimuli. However, the response for the pulse in the NAcI was also attenuated. Since the VP only receives an inhibitory input from NAcI, its activity was reduced by the stimuli presentation. Finally, the VTA also showed a peak response for the prepulse while the activity in response for the pulse was attenuated.

3.1. Basic Features of PPI

In order to verify if the present model would reproduce some basic features observed in animal experiments of PPI, we conducted tests varying the duration of ISI, the prepulse intensity, and verified the habituation of ASR by repeated pulse presentations.

3.1.1. ISI Test

We simulated trials of prepulse + pulse in which the ISI varied from 0 ms to 250 ms in steps of 10 ms between the simulations. The pulse intensity was fixed at 60 dB above background and the prepulse intensities tested were 15, 20, and 25 dB above background. The duration of pulse and prepulse were 30 ms.
The results show the %PPI are dependent on the ISI variation (Figure 3A). The %PPI first increase as the ISI rises and reaches its maximum value with ISI between 70 and 100 ms. For a prepulse of 15 dB above background, the maximum %PPI was 88.59% with ISI = 90 ms; for a prepulse of 20 dB, the maximum %PPI was 85.70% with ISI = 80 ms and for a prepulse of 25 dB, the maximum %PPI was 84.82% with ISI = 80 ms. Above these values of ISI, the %PPI decreases to a baseline around 0% of inhibition. For ISIs below 50 ms, the %PPIs obtained in the simulations were negative. For an ISI of 30 ms, there was a %PPI of −9.06% for the prepulse with intensity of 15 dB, a %PPI of −14.15% for the prepulse with 20 dB and a %PPI of −18.89% for the 25 dB prepulse.

3.1.2. Prepulse Intensity Test

To examine the relationship between %PPI and the intensity of the prepulse, we simulated trials of prepulse + pulse in which the intensity of prepulse varied from 0 dB to 100 dB above the background in steps of 5 dB between the simulations. The ISI was set to 60, 70, or 80 ms and the duration of both pulse and prepulse to 30 ms.
This test shows that the %PPI depends also on the prepulse intensity (Figure 3B). For prepulse intensities from 0 dB to 20 dB above background, the %PPI increases, reaching its maximum with prepulse intensities between 20 dB to 40 dB above background. For the ISI of 60 ms, the maximum inhibition was 55.35% for a prepulse of 40 dB; for ISI of 70 ms, the maximum %PPI was 82.02% with the prepulse of 35 dB and for the ISI of 80 ms 85.70% for a prepulse of 20 dB. For prepulses with intensities above 40 dB, the %PPI decreases, reaching negative values for prepulse intensities greater than 60 dB for all ISI. For a prepulse with intensity of 80 dB, the %PPI obtained was −11.00% for the three ISIs. For a prepulse of 100 dB, the %PPI was −14.92%, independetly of the ISI value.
For the remaining tests, the stimuli parameters were set according to the results shown in this section: the ISI was fixed in 80 ms, the prepulse intensity was 25 dB above background, the pulse was fixed in 60 dB above background, and the duration of both prepulse and pulse fixed at 30 ms. Simulating trials of PPI with these values led to a %PPI of 84.82%. We selected these parameters in order to avoid ceiling effect and reflect experimental settings.

3.1.3. ASR Habituation Test

The PPI session was composed of 10 pulse trials and ITIs of 5–15 s, 10–15 s, and 20–25 s. For each ITI, we ran 10 simulations with random initial parameter values, as described above. Repeated presentation of pulses decreased the MN activity amplitude (Figure 4) for all ITI used. For each pulse presentation, there was a decrease in MN activity, from 0.612 (in all ITIs) to 0.543 (ITI 5–15 s, Figure 4A), 0.561 (ITI 10–15 s, Figure 4B) and 0.595 (ITI 20–25 s, Figure 4C). The percentage of decrease in MN activity was calculated in reference to the activity in response to the first pulse (Figure 4D). The simulation showed that the percentage of decrease in MN activity depends on the ITI used in the test. After 5 pulse presentations, the MN activity decreased by 10.51% for the ITI of 5–15 s, by 6.87% for the ITI of 10–15 s, and by 2.53% for the ITI of 20–25 s. After 10 pulse presentations, the MN activity decreased by 11.14% for ITI of 5–15 s, by 8.31% for the ITI of 10–15 s, and by 2.79% for the ITI of 20–25 s. Thus, the highest habituation was observed for the shortest ITI used (5–15 s).

3.2. Manipulation of GABAergic Transmission

3.2.1. GABAergic Test: Trial

To test the influence of the amygdala and ventral pallidum on PPI and simulate the effect of GABAergic drugs, we manipulated the GABA factor in only the amygdala (Equations (A10) and (A11)), only in the VP (Equation (A21)), or in both units. The GABA factor varied from 0.0 (highest inhibition, simulating a GABAergic agonist) to 2.0 (highest hyperactivation, simulating a GABAergic antagonist) in steps of 0.10 between the simulations (mode details in Section 2.2). In this test, the trials had a prepulse + pulse with pulse intensity of 60 dB above background, prepulse intensity of 25 dB, ISI of 80 ms, and duration of prepulse and pulse of 30 ms.
Affecting amygdala activity resulted in a decrease in %PPI, for both inhibition (GABA factor below 1.00) and hyperactivation (GABA factor above 1.00, Figure 5) of this unit. The simulation with GABA factor at 0.50 had a %PPI of 75.48% (reduction of 9.34% compared to control) and with GABA factor at 0.00 (maximum inhibition) had a %PPI of 59.81% (reduction of 25.01% compared to control). The simulation of GABA factor at 1.50 (hyperactivation of amygdala) had a %PPI of 63.30% (reduction of 21.52% compared to control) and with GABA factor at 2.00 had %PPI of 58.77% (reduction of 26.05% compared to control).
Manipulating only the GABA factor in VP reduced the %PPI only for the inhibition (GABA factor lower than 1.00, Figure 5). The simulation with GABA factor at 0.50 had %PPI of 75.60% (reduction of 9.22% compared to control) and GABA factor at 0.00 (maximum inhibition) had a %PPI of 68.23% (reduction of 16.59% compared to control). However, for GABA factor above 1.00, there was only a little difference from control. For GABA factor of 1.50, the simulation had a %PPI of 85.70% (increase of 0.88% compared to control) and for GABA factor at 2.00 had a %PPI of 83.43% (reduction of 1.39% compared to control).
The reduction in %PPI caused by amygdala inhibition was further increased by the hyperactivation of VP (Figure 5). The simulation with GABA factor at 0.50 in amygdala and 1.50 in VP had a %PPI of 36.25% (reduction of 48.56% compared to control). With GABA factor at 0.00 in amygdala and 2.00 in VP, the simulation resulted in a %PPI of 19.38% (reduction of 65.44% compared to control).
A similar reduction was observed by hyperactivating amygdala and inhibiting VP (Figure 5). The simulation with GABA factor in amygdala at 1.50 and in VP at 0.50 had a %PPI of 56.07% (reduction of 28.75% compared to control) and the simulation with GABA factor in amygdala at 2.00 and in VP at 0.00 had a %PPI of 49.29% (reduction of 35.53% compared to control).
Likewise, hyperactivating both amygdala and VP reduced the %PPI in the simulation (Figure 5). For GABA factor at 1.50 in both amygdala and VP, the %PPI simulated was 71.34% (reduction of 13.48% compared to control) and at 2.00, the %PPI was 74.08% (reduction of 10.73% compared to control).
Finally, inhibition of both amygdala and VP prevented the reduction in the %PPI caused by the inhibition of only amygdala or only VP (Figure 5). The simulation with GABA factor at 0.50 in both amygdala and VP resulted in a %PPI of 87.20% (increase of 2.32% compared to control), a %PPI 8.62% higher than the condition with inhibition in amygdala (GABA factor of 0.50 in amygdala), and 8.73% higher than the condition with inhibition of VP (GABA factor of 0.50 in VP). The simulation with GABA factor at 0.00 in amygdala and VP had a %PPI of 84.22% (reduction of 0.60% compared to control), 24.41% higher than %PPI for the condition with GABA factor at 0.00 in amygdala, and 15.99% higher than the condition with GABA factor at 0.00 in VP.

3.2.2. GABAergic Test: Session

In order to test the effect of simulating GABAergic agonist during a PPI session and if the effects of the manipulation of GABA factor could be attributed to alterations in the MN activity (ASR), we conduct a session test composed of 74 trials (as described in Section 2.1) including pulse (60 dB above background), prepulses (intensities of 15, 20, and 25 dB above background), prepulse + pulse, and no stimuli. This test session was repeated ten times varying the model’s initial parameters. The session was conducted with four groups: the VP group (GABA factor at 0.20 in VP), the Amygdala group (GABA factor at 0.20 in amygdala), Amygdala-VP group (GABA factor at 0.20 in both VP and amygdala), and Control group (GABA factor at 1.00 in both units). This process mimics an experiment with ten different animals in each treatment group with the administration of muscimol (GABAergic agonist) intra-amygdala, intra-VP, or in both regions. The data obtained in this test were analyzed with a two-way repeated measure ANOVA, with intensity as within subject factor and Treat1 (Control × GABA factor in amygdala at 0.20) and Treat2 (Control × GABA factor in VP at 0.20) as between subject factors. Since the data violated homoscedasticity (Mauchly’s sphericity test; ASR W = 0.133, p < 0.001 , Greenhouse-Geisser ε = 0.622; %PPI W = 0.219, p < 0.001 , Greenhouse-Geisser ε = 0.561), we applied the ANOVA test with Greenhouse-Geisser correction [63]. Both ASR and %PPI are normally distributed (the results of the Shapiro–Wilk test can be found in Table A2 in Appendix C).
The ANOVA showed a main effect of intensity [F(1.87, 67.22) = 2674.2, p < 0.001], Treat1 [F(1, 36) = 10.3, p < 0.01] and Treat2 [F(1, 36) = 12.3, p < 0.001], and a significant interaction Treat1 × Treat2 [F(1, 36) = 28.3, p < 0.01] (Figure 6A). The Tukey Post-hoc test showed that the ASR for the pulse was higher than the ASR for the prepulse + pulse for every prepulse intensity [p < 0.001]. There was no difference in the ASR to P60 between the four groups. However, the Amygdala group showed a higher amplitude for the PP15 + P60 and PP20 + P60 compared to Control group [p < 0.001], VP group [p < 0.001], and Amygdala-VP group [p < 0.001]. The amygdala group showed a higher ASR amplitude for the PP25 + P60 just compared to Control group [p < 0.001] and to Amygdala-VP group [p < 0.001].
The two-way repeated measure ANOVA for the %PPI revealed a main effect of Treat1 [F(1, 36) = 26.8, p < 0.001] and Treat2 [F(1, 36) = 32.7, p < 0.001] and a significant interaction Treat1 × Treat2 [F(1, 36) = 74.3, p < 0.001] (Figure 6B). There was no effect of intensity in the %PPI [F(1.12, 40.42) = 2.26, p = 0.139]. The Tukey Post-hoc test showed that the Amygdala group had a lower %PPI compared to control group [p < 0.001] and to the Amygdala-VP group [p < 0.001]. However, the Amygdala-VP group did not differ from the control group [p = 0.088]. The Amygdala group showed a lower %PPI for PP15 and PP20 compared to the Control group [p < 0.001], VP group [p < 0.001], and Amygdala-VP group [p < 0.001]. The %PPI for the PP25 was lower in Amygdala group compared to Control group [p < 0.001] and to Amygdala-VP group [p < 0.001]. The Amygdala-VP group showed a higher %PPI for PP25 compared just to the VP group [p < 0.001] and the VP group had a lower %PPI for PP25 compared to the Control group [p < 0.010].

3.3. Manipulation of Dopaminergic Transmission

3.3.1. Dopaminergic Test: Trial

We tested the effect of dopaminergic transmission in the model manipulating the DA factor in the equations which represent the dopaminergic receptors (Equations (4) and (5)) in all units of the model (systemic test), only in amygdala (amygdala test), or only in NAc (NAc test). To distinguish the manipulation over the D 1 and D 2 subtypes of dopaminergic receptor, only the DA factor in the equations representing the subtype of receptor under test was manipulated. Thus, in the test of D 1 receptors, only in these receptors the DA factor varied. In the test of D 1 and D 2 receptors, both types of receptors had the DA factor manipulated. The DA factor varied from −1.00 (simulating an antagonist, inhibiting the receptor activity) to 1.00 (simulating an antagonist, activating the receptor) in steps of 0.10 between the simulations. The trials had a pulse intensity of 60 dB above background, prepulse intensity of 25 dB, ISI of 80 ms, and stimuli duration of 30 ms.

3.3.2. Systemic Test

In the systemic test with manipulation of both receptors types, there was a reduction in the %PPI by the increase in the DA factor (simulating an agonist) and a slight increase in %PPI by the reduction of DA factor (simulating an antagonist) (Figure 7). For the DA factor at 0.50 the simulation had a %PPI of 19.23% (reduction of 65.59% compared to control) and at 1.00 the simulation had a %PPI of 13.71% (reduction of 71.11% compared to control). In contrast, the simulation with the DA factor at −0.50 had a %PPI of 89.13% (increase of 4.31% compared to control) and with the DA factor at −1.00 had a %PPI of 89.12% (increase of 4.30% compared to control).
Similarly, in the systemic test with manipulation of only D 1 receptors, the simulation of increased DA factor caused a decrease in the %PPI, whereas reducing the factor’s value led to an increase in the %PPI (Figure 7). For the DA factor at 0.50 the simulation led to a %PPI of 59.84% (reduction of 24.98% compared to control) and at 1.00 to a %PPI of 53.77% (reduction of 31.05% compared to control). For the DA factor at −0.50 the %PPI was 88.66% (increase of 3.85% compared to control) and at −1.00 the %PPI was 89.13% (increase of 4.31% compared to control).
Increasing the DA factor only in D 2 receptors caused a reduction in the %PPI while the reduction of the DA factor caused an increase in the %PPI (Figure 7). For the DA factor at 0.50 the %PPI was 36.70% (reduction of 48.12% compared to control) and at 1.00 the %PPI was 21.41% (reduction of 63.41% compared to control). In contrast, the simulation with DA factor at −0.50 had a %PPI of 89.17% (increase of 4.35% compared to control) and the factor DA at −1.00 had a %PPI of 89.13% (increase of 4.31% compared to control).

3.3.3. Amygdala Test

In the amygdala test, increasing the value of DA factor in both receptor types led to a reduction in the %PPI, while reducing the DA factor in both receptors caused an increase in the %PPI (Figure 7A). The simulation with the DA factor at 0.50 led to a %PPI of 58.21% (reduction of 26.60% compared to control) and for the factor DA at 1.00 the %PPI was of 55.11% (reduction of 29.71%). In contrast, for the factor DA at −0.50 the %PPI was 88.35% (increase of 3.53% compared to control) and at −1.00 the %PPI was 88.25% (increase of 3.44% compared to control).
The amygdala tests with manipulation of only D 1 receptors (Figure 7A), when DA factor was set at 0.50, %PPI was 62.66% (decrease of 22.16% compared to control) and at 1.00 had a %PPI of 58.16% (decrease of 26.66% compared to control). The increased DA factor in D 1 receptors in amygdala also caused an increase in the activity of excitatory subpopulation and a decrease in the inhibitory subpopulation (Figure 7B,C). For DA factor at −0.50 the simulated %PPI was 88.29% (increase of 3.47% compared to control) and at −1.00 the %PPI was 89.06% (increase of 4.25%). This was accompanied by a decrease in excitatory amygdala subpopulation activity and a slight increase in the inhibitory subpopulation activity (Figure 7B,C).
The amygdala tests with manipulation of only D 2 receptors (Figure 7A), when DA factor was set at 0.50, %PPI was 65.63% (decrease of 19.18% compared to control) and at 1.00, %PPI was 65.25% (decrease of 19.57% compared to control). The increase in DA factor in the D 2 receptor in amygdala caused an increase in the excitatory subpopulation activity but a slight decrease in the inhibitory subpopulation activity (Figure 7B,C). The simulation with DA factor at −0.50 led to a %PPI of 88.49% (increase of 3.67% compared to control) and at −1.00 to a %PPI of 88.49% (increase of 3.67% compared to control, Figure 7A). This manipulation was associated with a decrease in the activity of excitatory subpopulation and a slight increase of inhibitory subpopulation (Figure 7B,C).

3.3.4. NAc Test

The NAc tests with manipulation of DA factor for both receptors or only D 2 receptor affected the %PPI. The manipulation of DA factor only in D 1 receptors led to a slight change in the %PPI compared to the manipulation of D 2 receptors or both types receptors (Figure 7A). The simulation with the DA factor of both D 1 and D 2 receptors in NAc at 0.50 led to a %PPI of 62.89% (reduction of 21.93% compared to control) and at 1.00 to a %PPI of 38.48% (reduction of 46.34% compared to control). This manipulation of DA factor in NAc was accompanied by a slight increase in NAc activity for an intermediate value of the factor DA (at 0.2, Figure 7D) and a reduction in NAc activity for high values of DA factor (Figure 7D). In contrast, the intermediate value of DA factor in NAc caused a slight increase in VP activity while higher values also led to higher increases in VP activity (Figure 7E). For the DA factor at −0.50 the %PPI obtained was 89.80% (increase of 4.98% compared to control) and at −1.00 the %PPI was 90.21% (increase of 5.39% compared to control). This manipulation caused a decrease in NAc activity and an increase in VP activity (Figure 7D,E).
For the manipulation of only D 2 receptors in NAc (Figure 7A), the simulation with the DA factor at 0.50 had a %PPI of 68.08% (reduction of 16.74% compared to control) and at 1.00 had a %PPI of 52.58% (reduction of 32.24% compared to control). Similar to the manipulation in both receptors, the increase in the DA factor of D 2 receptors in NAc caused in NAc activity a slight increase for intermediate values (at 0.2) and a decrease for higher values (Figure 7D). There was an increase in VP activity for the increased DA factor in NAc D 2 receptors (Figure 7E). For DA factor at −0.50, the simulated %PPI was 89.56% (increase of 4.74% compared to control) and at −1.00 the %PPI was 90.10% (increase of 5.28% compared to control, Figure 7A). The reduction of DA factor in NAc led to a reduction in NAc activity and an increase in VP activity (Figure 7D,E).
In contrast, for the manipulation of only D1 receptors in NAc (Figure 7A), for the DA factor at 0.50 the %PPI was 84.92% (increase of 0.10% compared to control) and at 1.00, the %PPI was 85.16% (increase of 0.34% compared to control). This manipulation was associated with a slight increase in NAc activity (Figure 7D). For the DA factor at −0.50, the %PPI was 86.50% (increase of 1.68% compared to control) and at −1.00 the %PPI was 86.76% (increase of 1.94% compared to control). The reduction in DA factor of D 1 receptors in NAc led to a reduction of NAc activity (Figure 7D) and an increase in VP activity (Figure 7E).

3.3.5. Dopaminergic Test: Session

The PPI session was composed of 74 stimuli presentation including pulse (60 dB above background), prepulse (intensities of 15, 20, and 25 dB above background), prepulse + pulse, and background. This test was repeated ten times varying the model’s parameters. The tests groups were as follows: Systemic group (DA factor at 0.50 in both D 1 and D 2 receptors and all receptors in every unit of the model), Amygdala group (DA factor at 0.50 in both D 1 and D 2 receptors in amygdala), NAc group (DA factor at 0.50 in both D 1 and D 2 receptors in NAc), and Control group (DA factor at 0.00 in all receptors). The data was analyzed with an one-way repeated measure ANOVA, with intensity as within subject factor and Group as between subject factor. Since the ASR and %PPI obtained in this test violated homoscedasticity (Mauchly’s sphericity test; ASR W = 0.093, p < 0.001 , Greenhouse-Geisser ε = 0.589; %PPI W = 0.230, p < 0.001 , Greenhouse-Geisser ε = 0.565), we conducted the ANOVA test with Greenhouse-Geisser correction [63]. Both ASR and %PPI are normally distributed (the results of Shapiro-Wilk test are shown in Table A3 in Appendix C).
For the ASR (MN activity in the present model), the ANOVA showed a main effect for intensity [F(1.77, 63.57) = 1060.40, p < 0.001] and Group [F(3, 36) = 37.9, p < 0.001] (Figure 8A). The Tukey post-hoc test revealed that the amplitude of ASR for the pulse was higher than any ASR for prepulse + pulse [p < 0.001]. There was no difference between the groups in the ASR amplitude in response to P60. The ASR for PP15 + P60 in the Systemic group was higher than the Control group [p < 0.001] and Amygdala group [p < 0.001], and the ASR for PP15 + P60 in the NAc group was higher than the Control [p < 0.001] group and Amygdala group [p < 0.001] but lower than the Systemic group [p < 0.001]. The ASR for PP20 + P60 in the Systemic group was higher than the Control group [p < 0.001], Amygdala group [p < 0.001] and NAc group [p < 0.001], in Amygdala group was higher than the Control group [p < 0.001], and in NAc was higher than Control group [p < 0.001]. The ASR for PP25 + P60 in the Systemic group was higher than the Control group [p < 0.001], Amygdala group [p < 0.001], and NAc group [p < 0.001], in Amygdala group was higher than the Control group [p < 0.001], and in NAc group was higher than Control group [p < 0.001].
The ANOVA also showed a main effect of intensity [F(1.13, 40.69) = 3.95, p = 0.049 < 0.05] and Group [F(3, 36) = 89.5, p < 0.001] for the %PPI (Figure 8B). The Tukey post-hoc test showed that the %PPI for the 15 dB prepulse was lower than the %PPI for the 20 dB prepulse [p < 0.050] and that the control group had higher %PPI than any other group [p < 0.001]. The %PPIs for PP15 in Systemic group and in NAc group were lower than the Control group [p < 0.001], and both Amygdala group and NAc group had higher %PPIs than the Systemic group [p < 0.001], and NAc group showed a lower %PPI than the control group [p < 0.001]. The Systemic group, Amygdala group, and NAc group showed lower %PPIs than the Control group [p < 0.001] for PP20. Both Amygdala and NAc groups showed higher %PPIs for PP20 than the Systemic group [p < 0.001]. All groups showed lower %PPIs for PP25 compared to Control group [p < 0.001]. Again, the Amygdala and NAc groups showed higher %PPIs than the Systemic group [p < 0.001].

4. Discussion

The model developed in the present work reflects the brain regions involved in the mediation of ASR and PPI as shown by the electrophysiological, pharmacological, and lesion reports [1,3,4,5,6]. The model developed showed that %PPI depends on the ISI and the prepulse intensity. It also demonstrated the habituation in the ASR by repeated pulse presentation. These three features are observed in experiments of PPI in animals and humans. Furthermore, ASR and PPI modulatory units were implemented in the present model, allowing us to simulate how these units regulate PPI and ASR pathways and to evaluate the effects of GABAergic and dopaminergic drugs. The simulation of GABAergic agonists and antagonists caused a decrease in the %PPI without affecting the ASR for the pulse. Likewise, the simulation of dopaminergic agonists produced a decrease in the %PPI, impacted the amygdala, NAc and VP activities, but did not change the ASR to pulse. In contrast, the simulation of dopaminergic antagonists led to an increase in %PPI, also modulating the activity of amygdala, NAc, and VP.
The startle response can be mediated by a circuit connecting a sensory input (like the Ch for auditory inputs in ASR, but also for vision and tactile inputs from different pathways) with a motor response unit. In rats, the ASR pathway is composed by the Ch, CRN, CPRN, and MN [5]. Similarly, the PPI can be mediated by a parallel circuit starting from a sensory system that terminates at a nucleus that inhibits the ASR pathway. In the rat brain, this circuit is composed by the SC, IC, and PPTg, the last nucleus inhibiting the CPRN [5,6]. However, other animals also show the startle modulation by the sensorimotor gating and some have a similar circuit organization involving a pathway connecting sensory receptors with a motor system, like the larvae of Drosophila melanogaster and the molusk Tritonia diomedea [64,65]. This fundamental mechanism indicates that sensorimotor gating remains conserved across the animals and similar circuits mediate this function [2]. Thus, the conditions and parameters, such as the ISI and prepulse intensity, for which we detect an inhibition of the startle response, reflects the functioning and connections within this brain circuit.
The intervals obtained in the present study for the ISI and prepulse intensity for which there was a maximum %PPI are consistent with the experimental reports. In rats, the maximum %PPI is obtained for an ISI between 20 ms and 100 ms and for prepulses with 15 to 20 dB above the background [52,53,54,55]. The interval between the prepulse and the pulse for the inhibition of ASR reflects the circuits and the parallel mechanism involved in the mediation of PPI. Since the PPI pathway has an inhibitory output to the ASR pathway, the ISI should last enough for the output of the PPI pathway to inhibit the ASR pathway activated by the pulse. Similarly, the prepulse intensity should be able to activate the PPI pathway without reaching the threshold to activate the ASR pathway. Hence, there are optimal intervals for the ISI and prepulse intensity for which the PPI is observed.
The habituation in startle response also reflects the functioning of the ASR pathway. In rats, the habituation of ASR is mediated by the STD mechanism in the connection between the CRN and CPRN [66]. The tests conducted with the present computational model showed that a higher habituation is achieved with shorter ITIs. As observed in rats, the percentage of decrease in ASR amplitude is also greater for shorter ITI [52,53,54,55]. Since the habituation mechanism depends mainly on the STD in the CRN-CPRN connection, the observation that shorter ITIs leads to higher decreases in ASR is consistent with the proposed mechanism.
The units chosen to compose the modulatory pathway reflect the pharmacological studies [5] and reports with neuroimaging techniques [7,8]. Rohlender and colleagues [7,8] using PET scan with the metabolic tracer [18F]fluoro-2-deoxyglucose in rats showed that the amygdala, mPFC, NAc, VP, and VTA are indeed involved on the PPI modulation. In that study, they found that the VTA activation is negatively correlated with the PPI and that NAc and basolateral amygdala are positively correlated with PPI. They also showed that the modulatory regions could be divided into a lateral system, related to the emotional processing of stimuli, and a medial one, involved in the cognitive processing. The VTA and the left prelimbic PFC (part of the mPFC) are part of the medial system. The amygdala, PPTg, and right prelimbic PFC are part of the lateral system. Since the sensorimotor gating acts protecting the interference of information processing by a stimulus that evokes the ASR [67], the modulatory units regulate the efficacy of the prepulse to inhibit the pulse ASR. Thus, while the lateral system is related to the regulation of prepulse inhibition tonus, the medial system regulates the ASR, increasing the response.
The present model offers a mechanistic way to interpret the effect of modulatory regions (amygdala, NAc, and VP) and dopaminergic transmission over the PPI. Simulation of amygdala and VP inhibition resulted in a reduction of %PPI, which is consistent with findings in rats showing that the administration of muscimol into amygdala [9] and the lesion of VP [68] reduced %PPI. However, in the simulations with weaker inhibition of VP, there was no %PPI reduction, which is consistent with another report showing no effect over the %PPI when VP is inactivated by muscimol local administration in rats [9]. In contrast, the simultaneous inhibition of amygdala and VP attenuated the reduction in the %PPI caused by the inhibition of each unit. This result can be interpreted considering the amygdala-NAc-VP pathway. Since amygdala increases NAc activity and VP inhibits PPTg, the reduction of amygdala activity also reduces NAc inhibition over VP, which in turn increases the inhibition over PPTg, leading to reductions in %PPI. By inhibiting both amygdala and VP, the effect of this last unit over PPTg is attenuated. It is noteworthy that this result follows the work of Forcelli et al. [9] showing that the co-administration of muscimol intra-amygdala and intra-VP reverted the PPI deficits caused by the injection of muscimol intra-amygdala.
The dopaminergic modulation of PPI was implemented in the present model by adding a unit representing the VTA and its connections with amygdala, mPFC, NAc, and VP. The simulation of dopaminergic agonists in the systemic test, in amygdala, or in NAc caused a decrease in the %PPI. Similar results are shown in studies with the administration of dopaminergic drugs in rats by systemic injection [22,23,62], intra-amygdala [18], or intra-NAc [20,26,27]. In contrast, the simulation of the dopaminergic antagonists produced a slight increase in the %PPI. This result is also consistent with the literature, showing that the administration of dopaminergic antagonists in rats also slightly increases the %PPI [29,30].
The effect of the dopaminergic modulation over the PPI can be interpreted considering the effects of dopamine in each unit. In the amygdala, the inhibitory interneurons express the D 2 receptors, while the projection neurons express the D 1 receptors [14]. The simulation of dopaminergic agonists in the amygdala increases its activity and, as shown previously, the increased amygdala activity has as a result the inhibition of PPTg, causing the reduction in the %PPI. However, our simulation of dopaminergic antagonists in amygdala caused a modest increase in the %PPI. A study conducted by Stevenson and Gratton [19] detected a reduction in the %PPI by injecting raclopride (a D 2 antagonist) in a rat’s amygdala. As detailed above, the amygdala model implemented in the present work was based on rat studies [14] with different strains (Wistar and Sprague–Dawley) [18,22], while the the study of Stevenson and Gratton [19] used the Long–Evans strain. Since there are differences in the effect of amphetamine between Sprague–Dawley and Long–Evans [26], the difference detected in the simulation of D 2 antagonist with the study of Stevenson and Gratton could be attributed to the distinct strains used to implement the present model.
As shown by Rohlender and colleagues [7,8], the amygdala is part of the lateral system, regulating the emotional processing and the PPI efficacy. The amygdala signalizes the context aversiveness, increasing or decreasing the PPI tonus. Thus, the increased amygdala activity (by GABAergic antagonist or dopaminergic agonists) could signalize an aversive context, increasing the animal responsivity to environmental stimuli, leading to deficits in PPI. Similarly, since amygdala regulates the PPI tonus, by decreasing its activity simulating the GABAergic agonist, it could interfere with the prepulse efficacy to inhibit the pulse ASR, also impairing the PPI. Thus, the amygdala shows a bimodal effect over the PPI modulation, either hyperactivating or inhibiting the impact of the prepulse.
The dopaminergic transmission also modulates NAc activity. For lower values of DA factor in NAc (representing a lower concentration of dopamine or dopaminergic drugs), there was an increase in its activity, but for higher values of DA factor in NAc, there was a decrease in the activity. Some studies found in animals a decrease in NAc activity caused by dopamine [69,70] while others found an increased activity [71]. In the present model, the reduction in the %PPI was seen only for higher values of DA factor accompanied by the decrease in NAc activity. Thus, these results suggest that the PPI impairment caused by the dopamine agonists is dependent on the inhibition of NAc. The reduction in %PPI caused by the simulation of a dopaminergic drug in NAc was dependent on the VP activity. This was also detected in reports testing the effect of dopaminergic drugs in rats [72]. Hence, the modulation of NAc over the PPI also involves its connection with VP. Since in the present work the reduction of NAc activity was associated with an overall increase in VP activity, and the VP has an inhibitory connection with PPTg, the impact of dopaminergic transmission and NAc activity depends on the VP activation. It is interesting to note that there is a convergence of inputs from both ventral and dorsal striatum to PPTg that modulates PPI [73,74,75]. As implemented in the present model, the NAc controls the PPI tonus. Moreover, dopaminergic input to NAc encodes stimulus salience [24]. Therefore, by the NAc-PPTg connection, dopamine may regulate the sensorimotor gating tonus by increasing or decreasing PPTg activity, controlling the flow of information to other brain regions [22,25]. This pathway links higher order brain regions to regions associated with sensorial processing.
These results together show that the NAc and the VP constitute a central hub for the PPI modulation. Several brain regions are connected with the ventral striatum, as the amygdala, hippocampus, and mPFC. As shown above, the effects of amygdala manipulation are dependent on VP activity. A similar result is obtained by NAc manipulation. Since both NAc and VP are connected with PPTg, they form a bridge between cortical regions with the PPI pathway. However, studies with rats also found that the dorsal striatum and the dopaminergic transmission from substantia nigra also modulates the PPI by their connections with VP [73,76]. Disruption in striatal function could also lead to deficits in PPI by affecting the NAc- and VP-PPTg connections. Intrinsic alterations in the NAc and VP functioning or their afferences could account for the ASR and PPI alterations observed in psychiatry conditions (as schizophrenia and bipolar disorder) or neurological diseases (such as Parkinson’s disease) [41]. In particular, schizophrenia and animal models of schizophrenia have robust impairment in PPI [40] and interference in striatal function [43].
The present computational model may also be used to simulate alterations in dopaminergic transmission and striatal activity as seem in schizophrenia. Regarding these disturbances with its PPI deficits, the effect has been simulated (work in progress). Furthermore, the present model also shows that alterations in NAc, VP, and dopaminergic modulation in these regions can cause PPI deficits by the connections with the PPTg, suggesting that the PPTg is a possible target for studies evaluating strategies for recovering PPI deficits.
As mentioned above, the dorsal striatum also modulates PPI by its projections to PPTg [73,76]. Therefore, the implementation of dorsal striatum and its dopaminergic modulation is a possible extension of the present computational model. Other neuromodulators such as serotonin and adrenaline are also involved in the PPI [5,77,78]. Interestingly, the systemic administration or injection into NAc of cannabidiol attenuated the deficit caused by amphetamine in mice [79]. These neurotransmitters could be implemented by adding units representing the locus coeruleus and raphe nucleus and the connections with cortical and subcortical units, being a second extension of the present work.

Limitations and Future Works

Some limitations should be addressed in the present work to avoid a misinterpretation of the results. First, the background sound used in the experiments was not directly implemented as did the model proposed by Schmajuk and Larrauri [51]. The model developed by these authors also showed an effect of background intensity [51] over ASR. Since in the present model we did not implement the background, it was impossible to investigate how that could interact with the tests conducted here. Accordingly, the sound intensity above the background was indirectly taken considering a background intensity fixed at 60 dB. Most of the experimental studies with GABAergic and dopaminergic drugs that we used to construct and validate this computational model used a background around 60 dB. Thus, following previous computational models [49,50], we chose to leave this factor out and adjust the model parameters to reflect the response considering a virtual background of 60 dB. Our aim was to implement a model that had the modulatory units and could account for the effects of GABAergic and dopaminergic drugs, so we focused on implementing the minimal amount of details needed to accomplish this objective. However, the model could be expanded to account for the background sound by: (1) adjusting the model parameters to decrease the units’ responsivity to the sound stimulus; and (2) implementing a similar mechanism as the one described by Schmajuk and Larrauri [51].
Second, in the model proposed here, the amygdala can increase the release of DA within NAc by a connection with VTA. Although there is a direct connection between those structures [80], some reports showed that the mechanisms for amygdala regulation of dopamine release in NAc takes place within NAc and that this regulation was independent from VTA activity [81,82]. However, there are subclasses of neurons in VTA that fire to salient or aversive events [83,84,85] and both amygdala and VTA are associated with processing stimulus salience [86,87]. Furthermore, it cannot be excluded that there is an indirect connection between amygdala and VTA. The central nucleus of amygdala, for example, was shown to send direct projections to substantia nigra in the midbrain [88]. There is also a connection between amygdala and hypothalamus [89] and from hypothalamus to VTA [90,91], suggesting a possible pathway for DA regulation by the amygdala.
In the present study, we focused on implementing the traditional units associated to the ventral striatum, namely nucleus accumbens, amygdala, and ventral pallidum. However, we could not omit a more complex scenario involving these regions with the modulation of dopamine. Both dorsal and ventral striatum show an intricate interconnection and regulation of the dopaminergic system, involving the VTA and substantia nigra, and an interaction with cortical regions [33,34]. The striatum and dopaminergic system form loops in which the ventral striatum can regulate the dopaminergic activity of both VTA and substantia nigra. From there, the dopaminergic neurons project back to the striatum [34]. Thus, this intercommunication between ventral and dorsal striatum within the dopaminergic system could also be involved in PPI modulation. Indeed, lesions of the substantia nigra pars reticulata and injection of muscimol in this region impairs PPI in rats [73,92,93]. In addition, injection of the D 1 receptor antagonist SCH23390 into the dorsal striatum reduced the PPI [76]. However, even with this modulation of PPI, the effects associated with substantia nigra alteration seem to depend on the PPTg and CPN [73,93]. Thus, the units implemented here, in particular the PPTg and CPN, are convergent units for the PPI mediation and modulation. Even if the dorsal striatum and substantia nigra were not directly implemented, in principle their effect over PPI could then rely on these two above-mentioned model units.
In addition to the dopaminergic modulation of PPI, other neuromodulators, such as noradrenaline and ATP, also influence dopaminergic transmission in the striatum. Noradrenaline controls the VTA activity and also promotes the release of dopamine from dopaminergic terminals [94]. A study associated the activation of P2Y receptors by ATP with the release of dopamine in a rat striatum [95]. Furthermore, glutamate and acetylcholine also regulate dopamine release in this region [96]. Thus, dopaminergic transmission has an intricate regulation by other neuromodulators and neurotransmitters within the striatum. These mechanisms could also be implemented in the present model, enabling the study of their interaction with the current mechanisms and their impact on PPI.
Since the present model has the flexibility to incorporate other units, such as the dorsal striatum and substantia nigra, and neuromodulators, including noradrenaline and serotonin, without changing its general form, it is possible to implement these units and investigate their roles on PPI mediation and modulation. It is important to mention, however, that the addition of new units will also make the model more complex, increasing the number of parameters that must be fitted in order to correctly replicate the experimental findings and making it more difficult to grasp an intuitive understanding and obtain insights that a simpler model can offer.
All the reports for the anatomical and functional basis of PPI considered for developing this model refer to studies with rodents. Although there is an overlap between different species in the structures involved in the generation and modulation of PPI and the effect of pharmaceutical action on the dopaminergic system [2,64,65], there are also some specific differences across species and among rodent strains in the effect of glutamatergic, dopaminergic, and serotonergic drugs [23,26,31,62,97,98]. Regarding the dopaminergic system, mice have more robust deficits in PPI for D1 receptor agonists, while rats show stronger PPI deficits with D 2 receptor agonists [31]. Similarly, although not implemented in the present model, the inhibition of substantia nigra pars reticulata decreases PPI in rats while facilitating PPI in monkeys [92]. Amphetamine administration reduces PPI in rats but causes a PPI deficit in humans only for short-intervals ISI and for within-subject analyses [99]. These differences could be associated to distinct connectivity between the dopaminergic system with cortical and subcortical regions across species [33,100], spiking pattern of dopaminergic neurons in VTA [101], and receptor binding specificities [102]. Thus, even though PPI displays similarities among different species [2], the differences between them should not be overlook in interpreting and extrapolating the results of the present model to humans and other species. Since the majority and most robust studies on the neurophysiological and anatomical basis of the ASR and PPI responses were conducted in rodents, we focused in validating the model parameters with these studies. However, it is possible, in principle, to change the model parameters to reproduce the ASR and PPI responses of other species.
Finally, the current model provides a tool for investigating how changes in simulated brain function may be linked to psychiatric disorders. We conducted different tests to understand how functional alterations could induce PPI deficits similar to those observed in patients with schizophrenia (which will be shown in a forthcoming paper). Other psychiatric disorders also display PPI deficits and our model could be extended to account for specific alterations associated with them [35,37]. For example, the implementation of dorsal striatum and substantia nigra could help in understanding their involvement in the PPI deficits observed in Parkinson’s disease [35]. Moreover, other psychiatric disorders share genetic and functional alterations with schizophrenia, such as bipolar disorder [37,103]. Thus, the present model could be used, at least in a first approximation, to study these psychiatric conditions.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the present computational model implements a neural network that has the basic features observed in animal experiments and shows a valuable approach to understanding the influence of the cortical and subcortical structures and the dopaminergic transmission over PPI. It enables the study and simulation of GABAergic and dopaminergic drugs and offers a mechanistic way to interpret their effects. According to the simulation results, NAc and VP constitutes a central hub for the PPI modulation by interfering with the PPTg activity. Ultimately, this work contributes to the understanding of how disturbances in cortical and subcortical structures can impair the sensorimotor gating.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.O.B. and C.S.; methodology, T.O.B.; writing—original draft preparation, T.O.B.; writing—review and editing, C.S. and A.C.R.; supervision, C.S. and A.C.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES; grant number: 88887.701080/2022-00) and by the Federal University of ABC (UFABC). TOB. is supported by a São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP) PhD scholarship (grant 2021/12832-7). ACR is partially supported by FAPESP (grant 2013/07699-0; CEPID NeuroMat) and by the Brazilian National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq) Research Productivity Grant 303359/2022-6.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The codes used to implement and simulate the model can be found in: https://github.com/ThiagoTakechi/PPIModel.git, accessed on 13 May 2024.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
ASRAcoustic startle response
ChCochlea
CRNCochlear root nucleus
CPRNCaudal pontine reticular nucleus
MNMotor neuron
ICInferior colliculus
ISIInterstimulus interval
ITIIntertrial interval
SCSuperior colliculus
PPIPrepulse inhibition
PPTgPedunculopontine tegmental nucleus
mPFCMedial prefrontal cortex
AmygAmygdala
NAcNucleus accumbens
NAcDNucleus accumbens direct pathway
NAcINucleus accumbens indirect pathway
VPVentral pallidum
VTAVentral tegmental area

Appendix A. Model Equations

Appendix A.1. ASR Pathway

The ASR pathway is composed by four units, namely cochlea (Ch), cochlear root nucleus (CRN), caudal pontine reticular nucleus (CRPN), and motor neurons (MN), which were associated to ASR generation [4,5,104]. The Ch activity A Ch was modeled as follows:
d A Ch d t = 1 τ A Ch + I 2 I 2 + k I 2 + ϵ ( t ) ,
where τ is the time constant (assumed to be the same for all equations below unless otherwise specified), I is the input sound intensity in dB, k I is the semi-saturation constant for sound transduction, and ϵ ( t ) is a random variable drawn from a uniform distribution in the interval [ 0.001 , 0.001 ] at each simulation time step. The ASR pathway starts in the Ch [5,105], which receives the sound input and transduces it into neural activity.
The CRN activity A CRN was determined by the equation:
d A CRN d t = 1 τ A CRN + A Ch .
The CRN receives a glutamatergic projection from Ch and sends projections to CPRN and SC [5,105,106].
The C P R N activity was described by:
d A CPRN d t = 1 τ [ A CPRN + W · p A CRN l CRN · A CRN 2 A CRN 2 + k CRN 2 · 1 P PPtg 2 P PPtg 2 + k PPTg 2 ] ,
where W is the weight of the synapse between CRN and CPRN (Equation (A5)), p ( x l ) is the threshold function (see Equation (6)), l CRN is the transmission threshold (Equation (A4)), and k CRN and k P P T g are, respectively, the semi-saturation constants for the CRN and PPTg inputs. The activity of CRN is transmitted to CPRN only if it reaches the threshold l CRN . This ensures that only strong stimuli are transmitted from CRN to downstream units in the ASR pathway [49]. The synaptic weight W was introduced to account for the STD observed in the CRN-CPRN connection [66]. The projection from CRN to CPRN is excitatory, and the projection from PPTg is inhibitory [74,75]. The PPTg inhibitory connection mediates prepulse inhibition.
The threshold l CRN was given by:
l CRN = l 0 , CRN + k l , VTA · M VTA 2 k VTA 2 + M VTA 2 ,
where l 0 , CRN is the threshold initial value, k l , VTA is the maximum value of the modulatory dopaminergic influence on the threshold, M VTA is the VTA activity, and k VTA is the semi-saturation constant for the VTA input. The threshold changes as a function of the dopaminergic input, so when DA is released in the CRN the threshold also increases.
The synaptic weight of the CRN-CPRN connection was modeled as:
d W d t = 1 τ W W + 1 k W · p A CRN l W · A CRN 2 A CRN 2 + k CRN 2 ,
where τ W is the time-constant of short-term synaptic depression, k W is a scale constant for the maximum decrease of the synaptic weight due to CRN activity, p ( x l ) is the threshold function, l W is the synaptic weight threshold, and k CRN is the CRN input semi-saturation constant. This equation implements ASR habituation [66] by a STD mechanism as a function of the CRN input. The STD mechanism was adapted from the model by Abbott et al. [107]. If the CRN activity is above the threshold l W , the weight reduction is scaled by the factor k W . Without any sound stimulus, the weight returns exponentially to its resting value 1.
The M N activity was descried by
d A MN d t = 1 τ A MN + A CPRN .
The motor neurons unit receives direct excitatory input from CPRN [5,105]. The MN activity represents the ASR in response to a sound stimulus and was taken as the output of the model.

Appendix A.2. PPI Pathway

The PPI pathway is composed by the inferior colliculus (IC), superior colliculus (SC), and pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus (PPTg) [5].
The IC activity P IC was modeled as:
d P IC d t = 1 τ P IC + A CRN 2 A CRN 2 + k CRN 2 ,
where k CRN is the CRN input semi-saturation constant. The connection from CRN to IC [108] is excitatory. The IC, on its turn, sends a direct excitatory projection to SC [109] and indirect excitatory projections to mPFC and Amyg [5,105]. In order to model these indirect projections, delays of 60 ms (see Table A1) were set for these connections.
The SC activity P SC was described by
d P SC d t = 1 τ P SC + P IC 2 P IC 2 + k IC 2 ,
where k IC is the IC input semi-saturation constant. The SC sends an excitatory projection to PPTg [74,75,109].
Table A1. Model Parameters.
Table A1. Model Parameters.
ParameterValue
Connection Strengths
k CRN (strong connections)0.10
k PPTg , k IC , k SC , k NAcI , k NAcD , k VP , k mPFC (mild connections)0.30
k Amyg , k VTA (weak connections)0.50
k I 35
k W 90
Dopaminergic Transmission Parameters
k p 0.06
k D 0.20
k mPFC , DA 0.81
D max 0.60
Thresholds
l 0 , CRN 0.45
l W 0.50
l NAcD 0.70
l NAcI 0.30
l Amyg 0.45
l D 2 pre 0.30
l D 2 pos 0.40
l D 1 pos 0.50
k l , VTA 0.10
Tonic Activities
t mPFC , DA 0.30
t NAc 0.20
t VP 0.40
Time Constants
τ 10 ms
d e l a y 60 ms
τ w 15 s
τ DA 285 ms
τ p 5 ms
The PPTg activity P PPTg was given by
d P PPTg d t = 1 τ [ P PPTg + P SC 2 P SC 2 + k SC 2 1 M VP 2 M VP 2 + k VP 2 · 1 M NAcD 2 M NAcD 2 + k NAcD 2 ] ,
where k SC , k VP and k NAcD are, respectively, the semi-saturation constants of the SC, VP, and NAcD inputs. PPTg receives an excitatory projection from SC and inhibitory projections from VP and NAcD [74,75]. In order to adjust the temporal scale of CRN inhibition by the prepulse, a delay of 60 ms (Table A1) was implemented for the connection from SC to PPTg. The delay ensures that the prepulse input goes to PPTg at the same time as the ASR pathway response to a stimulus goes to CRN [49].

Appendix A.3. Modulatory Pathway

The modulatory pathway is composed by the amygdala (Amyg), medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), nucleus accumbens (NAc), divided in a direct (NAcD) and an indirect (NAcI) pathway, ventral pallidum (VP) and ventral tegmental area (VTA, which represents the dopaminergic system and its connections). All these units were shown to modulate the ASR and PPI [5,11,110].
Amyg and mPFC were divided into excitatory and inhibitory subpopulations in order to account for the effects of dopamine in these regions [14,15,16,17,111]. As mentioned above, the same delay used for the SC to PPTg connection was also set for the connections from IC to mPFC and Amyg to reflect the indirect nature of these connections.
The activity of the inhibitory Amyg subpopulation, M AmygI , was described by:
d M AmygI d t = 1 τ M AmygI + G A B A · D 2 M mPFC 2 k mPFC 2 + M mPFC 2 ,
where D 2 represents the activity of D 2 dopaminergic receptors in this subpopulation and k mPFC is the mPFC input semi-saturation constant. The inhibitory subpopulation of Amyg receives an excitatory projection from mPFC [17] and has its activity modulated by the inhibitory D 2 receptors (modeled by Equation (5)). The activation of these receptors reduces the influence of the input from mPFC and, consequently, reduces the activity of the Amyg inhibitory subpopulation [14]. The GABA term in (A10) simulates the effects of the administration of GABAergic agonists and antagonists in the amygdala. More details about this term are given in Section 2.2. The Amyg inhibitory subpopulation projects only to the excitatory Amyg subpopulation.
The excitatory Amyg subpopulation activity M Amyg was given by:
d M Amyg d t = 1 τ [ M Amyg + G A B A · D 1 P IC 2 k IC 2 + P IC 2 · 1 M AmygI 2 k AmygI 2 + M AmygI 2 ] ,
where D 1 represents the activity of D 1 dopaminergic receptors and k IC and k Amyg are, respectively, the semi-saturation constants of the IC and AmygI inputs. The D 1 receptor was modeled by Equation (4). The GABA term in this equation simulates the effect of GABAergic drugs in the amygdala (see Section 2.2 for more details). The Amyg excitatory subpopulation projects to NAc (direct and indirect pathways) and to both the inhibitory and excitatory subpopulations of mPFC [15,16].
The inhibitory mPFC subpopulation activity M mPFCI was modeled as:
d M mPFCI d t = 1 τ M mPFCI + D 1 M Amyg 2 k Amyg 2 + M Amyg 2 ,
where D 1 represents the activity of D 1 dopaminergic receptors in the mPFC inhibitory subpopulation and k Amyg is the semi-saturation constant of the Amyg excitatory subpopulation input. D 1 was modeled according to Equation (4). The inhibitory mPFC subpopulation projects to the excitatory mPFC subpopulation [14,15,16,17].
The activity of the mPFC excitatory subpopulation, M mPFC , was described by:
d M mPFC d t = 1 τ [ M mPFC + P IC 2 k IC 2 + P IC 2 + M Amyg 2 k Amyg 2 + M Amyg 2 · 1 D 2 M mPFCI 2 k mPFCI 2 + M mPFCI 2 ] ,
where k IC , k Amyg and k mPFCI are the semi-saturation constants of the inputs from IC, Amyg excitatory subpopulation, and mPFC inhibitory subpopulation, respectively, and D 2 is the activity of D 2 dopaminergic receptors in the mPFC excitatory subpopulation (modeled by Equation (5)). The targets of the mPFC excitatory subpopulation are NAc (direct and indirect pathways) and the inhibitory subpopulation of Amyg [16,17].
In mPFC, the D 1 receptor is expressed only in the inhibitory subpopulation while the D 2 receptors are located at the presynaptic terminal of the inhibitory to excitatory subpopulation synapse. The dopaminergic receptors in mPFC were implemented in accordance to the known effects of dopamine in this region [16,17]. When dopamine is released in mPFC, it decreases the input from the inhibitory to the excitatory subpopulation and increases the overall excitability of the excitatory subpopulation [14,17], then Amyg inputs start activating both mPFC subpopulations [15,16].
NAc was also subdivided into two units, one corresponding to the direct pathway (NAcD), which projects to PPTg, and the other to the indirect pathway (NAcI), which sends projections to VP [13].
The NAcD activity M NAcD was implemented as:
i n p u t NAcD = p M Amyg l NAcD M Amyg 2 k A m y g 2 + M Amyg 2 + M mPFC 2 k mPFC 2 + M mPFC 2 ,
d M NAcD d t = 1 τ M NAcD + D 1 i n p u t NAcD + t NAc D 2 M NAcI 2 k NAcI 2 + D 2 M NAcI 2 ,
where i n p u t NAcD represents the excitatory input from both Amyg and mPFC to NAcD, p ( x l ) is the threshold function, l NAcD is the transmission threshold for the input from Amyg to NAcD, k Amyg , k mPFC and k NAcI are, respectively, the semi-saturation constants for the inputs from the Amyg excitatory, mPFC excitatory, and NAc inhibitory subpopulations, D 1 and D 2 are, respectively, the activities of the D 1 and D 2 dopamine receptors in NAcD (Equations (4) and (5)), and t NAc is the basal tonic activity of NAcD. The NAcD receives excitatory inputs from Amyg and mPFC and an inhibitory input from NAcI [13,16,112], and makes an inhibitory connection to PPTg.
The NAcI activity M NAcI was described by
i n p u t NAcI = p M Amyg l NAcI M Amyg 2 k A m y g 2 + M Amyg 2 + M mPFC 2 k mPFC 2 + M mPFC 2 ,
d M NAcI d t = 1 τ M NAcI + D 2 i n p u t NAcI + t NAc ,
where i n p u t NAcI is the excitatory input from both Amyg and mPFC to NAcI, p ( x l ) is the threshold function, l NAcI is the transmission threshold for the input from Amyg to NAcI, k Amyg and k mPFC are, respectively, the the semi-saturation constants for the inputs from Amyg and mPFC to NAcI, D 2 is the activity of the D 2 dopaminergic receptors in NAcI (modeled by (5)), and t NAc is the basal tonic activity of NAcI (assumed to be the same of NAcD). NAcI receives excitatory projections from the excitatory subpopulations of Amyg and mPFC [16,112], and sends an inhibitory projection to VP [9]. In contrast to NAcD, NAcI only expresses D 2 dopaminergic receptors and has an intermediate connection to VP before affecting PPTg [13].
The threshold mechanism implemented in both NAcD and NAcI represents the interaction between Amyg and mPFC inputs to NAc [112,113,114,115,116,117,118,119]. When the Amyg activity is below both the thresholds ( l NAcD and l NAcI ), neither Amyg nor mPFC will affect NAc. However, if the Amyg activity is above the thresholds both Amyg and mPFC influence NAc.
The dopaminergic neurotransmission in NAc was modeled according to the known mechanisms of phasic and tonic transmission [120,121,122]. The phasic dopamine release depends on the phasic activity of VTA, which leads to dopamine release in the synaptic cleft following a fast dopamine uptake. In contrast, the tonic release depends on both the tonic activity of VTA and the tonic glutamatergic stimulation from mPFC. The tonic release increases the extracellular dopamine concentration and regulates the level of inhibitory feedback on the dopaminergic terminal by activating presynaptic D 2 receptors. The model implemented here follows an early model by Salum et al. [121,122], which was adapted to the formalism of the leaky-integrator equations.
The extracellular dopamine concentration D A extra was described by
d D A extra d t = 1 τ DA D A extra + k mPFC , DA · t mPFC , DA + k p D A p ,
where τ DA is the time constant for D A extra [120], k mPFC , DA is the strength of the efference from mPFC that regulates tonic extracellular dopamine release, t mPFC , DA is the level of tonic stimulation of mPFC, D A p is the concentration of phasic dopamine (see below), and k p is a scale factor for the contribution of phasic dopamine to the extracellular dopamine concentration.
The concentration of dopamine released by phasic mechanism D A p was modeled as follows:
d D A p d t = 1 τ p D A p + p M VTA k D D pre · M VTA k D D pre ,
where τ p is the D A p time constant, p ( x l ) is the threshold function (Equation (6)), D pre represents the inhibitory feedback by the activation of dopaminergic D 2 presynaptic receptors (modeled as (5)), and k D is a scale factor for the strength of D pre . Hence, phasic dopamine is released only if the VTA activity is above the threshold k D D pre .
The total dopamine concentration D A p , total that activates postsynaptic receptors was given by:
D A p , total = k D D A extra + D A p ,
where k D is a scale factor for the extracellular dopamine contribution to the activation of dopaminergic synaptic receptors, assumed to be the same as k D in Equation (A19). The scale factor k D was introduced to model the weak contribution of extracellular dopamine to the activation of synaptic dopamine [120].
The VP activity M VP was modeled as:
d M VP d t = 1 τ M VP + G A B A · t VP · D 2 M NAcI 2 k NAcI 2 + D 2 M NAcI 2 ,
where GABA simulates the effects of intra-VP administration of GABAergic agonists and antagonists (more details in Section 2.2), t VP is the tonic activity of VP, k NAcI is the NAcI input semi-saturation constant, and D 2 is the activity of dopaminergic D 2 receptors in VP (modeled as Equation (5)). In VP, the D 2 receptors are expressed in the axon terminals of NAcI neurons [13] regulating the inhibition of VP by NAcI.
The VTA activity M VTA was described by the following equation:
i n p u t = p M Amyg l Amyg · M Amyg 2 k Amyg 2 + M Amyg 2 + P PPTg 2 k PPTg 2 + P PPTg 2 · 1 M VP 2 k VP 2 + M VP 2 d M VTA d t = 1 τ M VTA + i n p u t ,
where i n p u t represents the input received by the VTA unit (excitatory inputs from both Amyg and PPTg and inhibitory input from VP), p ( x l ) is the threshold function, l Amyg is the Amyg activity threshold, and k Amyg , k PPTg and k VP are, respectively, the semi-saturation constants for the inputs from Amyg, PPTg, and VP. In this model, VTA receives excitatory projections from both Amyg and PPTg. The Amyg input has a threshold that ensures that only strong inputs can induce release of dopamine by Amyg. As reported previously [123], PPTg input to VTA indicates the presence of a salient stimulus, causing the dopaminergic neurons to fire phasically. In contrast, VP controls the tonic activity of dopaminergic neurons in VTA, regulating the number of dopaminergic active neurons [123].
The dopaminergic receptors D 1 and D 2 were modeled as described in Equations (4) and (5) and by their semi-saturation constant l D . Since the D 2 receptor has a higher affinity for dopamine [124,125], its semi-saturation constant is lower than the one of the D1 receptor (see Table A1). For the same reason, the semi-saturation constant of the presynaptic receptor D 2 is lower than the semi-saturation constant of the postsynaptic receptor D 2 [124,125].

Appendix B. Model Parameters

The parameters of the present model were determined following previous computational models of PPI [49,50,51]. The connection parameters were restricted to the values of 0.10 (strong connections), 0.30 (mild connections), and 0.50 (weak connection) in order to limit the possible connection parameter values.
The parameters of the phasic and tonic dopamine release mechanisms followed an early work [121,122] adapted to the leaky-integrator model formalism. The semi-saturation constant parameters for dopaminergic receptors were chosen to reflect the relative affinities of dopamine to D 1 and presynaptic and postsynaptic D 2 receptors [124,125]. Since the D 1 receptor has the least affinity for dopamine of the three receptors, its semi-saturation constant has the highest value of all whereas the semi-saturation constant of the presynaptic D 2 receptor has the lowest value because its dopamine affinity is the highest of all.
All the activities above were considered as dimensionless, so their rates have units of s−1. The activity time constant τ is close to experimental values [126,127,128,129,130] and is consistent with previous PPI computational models [50]. This short time constant also reflects the short latency observed in the brainstem nuclei and in ASR response [4,49,131]. The delay parameter in the connections between SC-PPTg, IC-Amyg, and IC-mPFC was taken from [49].
Since this computational model abstracts the detailed biological mechanisms and focuses more on the phenomenological aspects of PPI, the parameters are not readily interpretable in terms of their biological values.

Appendix C. Shapiro–Wilk Test for Normality

Table A2 shows the results of the Shapiro–Wilk test for normality of the GABAergic session test, and Table A3 shows the results of this test for the dopaminergic session test. In both cases, all ASR and %PPI are normally distributed.
Table A2. Shapiro-Wilk test result of the GABAergic session test ASR (PP0 + P60, PP15 + P60, PP20 + P60, PP25 + P60) and %PPI (PPI15, PPI20, PPI25). Data presented as Shapiro-Wilk W (p-value).
Table A2. Shapiro-Wilk test result of the GABAergic session test ASR (PP0 + P60, PP15 + P60, PP20 + P60, PP25 + P60) and %PPI (PPI15, PPI20, PPI25). Data presented as Shapiro-Wilk W (p-value).
GroupSAL/SALSAL/VPAmyg/SALAmyg/VP
PP0 + P600.922 (0.374)0.918 (0.339)0.901 (0.223)0.894 (0.187)
PP15 + P600.939 (0.547)0.938 (0.528)0.974 (0.924)0.937 (0.519)
PP20 + P600.946 (0.617)0.939 (0.540)0.927 (0.418)0.971 (0.901)
PP25 + P600.985 (0.985)0.924 (0.392)0.904 (0.243)0.977 (0.946)
PPI150.975 (0.937)0.937 (0.523)0.956 (0.735)0.957 (0.745)
PPI200.949 (0.655)0.942 (0.570)0.953 (0.700)0.978 (0.951)
PPI250.941 (0.564)0.967 (0.864)0.946 (0.619)0.966 (0.850)
Table A3. Shapiro-Wilk test result of the dopaminergic session test ASR (PP0 + P60, PP15 + P60, PP20 + P60, PP25 + P60) and %PPI (PPI15, PPI20, PPI25). Data presented as Shapiro-Wilk W (p-value).
Table A3. Shapiro-Wilk test result of the dopaminergic session test ASR (PP0 + P60, PP15 + P60, PP20 + P60, PP25 + P60) and %PPI (PPI15, PPI20, PPI25). Data presented as Shapiro-Wilk W (p-value).
GroupControlSystemicAmygNAc
PP0 + P600.860 (0.077)0.877 (0.119)0.886 (0.151)0.884 (0.145)
PP15 + P600.967 (0.859)0.957 (0.748)0.924 (0.390)0.984 (0.981)
PP20 + P600.969 (0.877)0.955 (0.723)0.933 (0.474)0.964 (0.835)
PP25 + P600.982 (0.976)0.947 (0.630)0.925 (0.397)0.968 (0.872)
PPI150.994 (1.000)0.865 (0.087)0.967 (0.864)0.924 (0.391)
PPI200.986 (0.988)0.913 (0.301)0.951 (0.677)0.968 (0.872)
PPI250.952 (0.687)0.916 (0.321)0.976 (0.939)0.977 (0.945)

References

  1. Davis, M. The mammalian startle response. In Neural Mechanisms of Startle Behavior; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 1984; pp. 287–351. [Google Scholar]
  2. Swerdlow, N.R.; Braff, D.L.; Geyer, M.A. Cross-species studies of sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999, 877, 202–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Davis, M.; Gendelman, D.S.; Tischler, M.D.; Gendelman, P.M. A primary acoustic startle circuit: Lesion and stimulation studies. J. Neurosci. 1982, 2, 791–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Lee, Y.; López, D.E.; Meloni, E.G.; Davis, M. A primary acoustic startle pathway: Obligatory role of cochlear root neurons and the nucleus reticularis pontis caudalis. J. Neurosci. 1996, 16, 3775–3789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Swerdlow, N.R.; Geyer, M.; Braff, D. Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: Current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology 2001, 156, 194–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Fulcher, N.; Azzopardi, E.; De Oliveira, C.; Hudson, R.; Schormans, A.L.; Zaman, T.; Allman, B.L.; Laviolette, S.R.; Schmid, S. Deciphering midbrain mechanisms underlying prepulse inhibition of startle. Prog. Neurobiol. 2020, 185, 101734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Rohleder, C.; Jung, F.; Mertgens, H.; Wiedermann, D.; Sué, M.; Neumaier, B.; Graf, R.; Leweke, F.M.; Endepols, H. Neural correlates of sensorimotor gating: A metabolic positron emission tomography study in awake rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Rohleder, C.; Wiedermann, D.; Neumaier, B.; Drzezga, A.; Timmermann, L.; Graf, R.; Leweke, F.M.; Endepols, H. The functional networks of prepulse inhibition: Neuronal connectivity analysis based on FDG-PET in awake and unrestrained rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Forcelli, P.A.; West, E.A.; Murnen, A.T.; Malkova, L. Ventral pallidum mediates amygdala-evoked deficits in prepulse inhibition. Behav. Neurosci. 2012, 126, 290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Cano, J.C.; Huang, W.; Fénelon, K. The amygdala modulates prepulse inhibition of the auditory startle reflex through excitatory inputs to the caudal pontine reticular nucleus. BMC Biol. 2021, 19, 116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Pothuizen, H.H.; Jongen-Rêlo, A.L.; Feldon, J. The effects of temporary inactivation of the core and the shell subregions of the nucleus accumbens on prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex and activity in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 2005, 30, 683–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Vallone, D.; Picetti, R.; Borrelli, E. Structure and function of dopamine receptors. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2000, 24, 125–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Dobbs, L.K.; Kaplan, A.R.; Lemos, J.C.; Matsui, A.; Rubinstein, M.; Alvarez, V.A. Dopamine regulation of lateral inhibition between striatal neurons gates the stimulant actions of cocaine. Neuron 2016, 90, 1100–1113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Kröner, S.; Rosenkranz, J.A.; Grace, A.A.; Barrionuevo, G. Dopamine modulates excitability of basolateral amygdala neurons in vitro. J. Neurophysiol. 2005, 93, 1598–1610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Floresco, S.B.; Maric, T.T. Dopaminergic regulation of inhibitory and excitatory transmission in the basolateral amygdala–prefrontal cortical pathway. J. Neurosci 2007, 27, 2045–2057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. McGinty, V.B.; Grace, A.A. Selective activation of medial prefrontal-to-accumbens projection neurons by amygdala stimulation and Pavlovian conditioned stimuli. Cereb. Cortex. 2008, 18, 1961–1972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rosenkranz, J.A.; Grace, A.A. Dopamine attenuates prefrontal cortical suppression of sensory inputs to the basolateral amygdala of rats. J. Neurosci. 2001, 21, 4090–4103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Salum, C.; Issy, A.; Brandão, M.; Guimarães, F.; Bel, E.D. Nitric oxide modulates dopaminergic regulation of prepulse inhibition in the basolateral amygdala. J. Psychopharmacol. 2011, 25, 1639–1648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Stevenson, C.W.; Gratton, A. Role of basolateral amygdala dopamine in modulating prepulse inhibition and latent inhibition in the rat. Psychopharmacology 2004, 176, 139–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Swerdlow, N.R.; Braff, D.; Masten, V.; Geyer, M. Schizophrenic-like sensorimotor gating abnormalities in rats following dopamine infusion into the nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology 1990, 101, 414–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Salum, C.; Guimaraes, F.; Brandao, M.; Del Bel, E. Dopamine and nitric oxide interaction on the modulation of prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response in the Wistar rat. Psychopharmacology 2006, 185, 133–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Zhang, J.; Forkstam, C.; Engel, J.A.; Svensson, L. Role of dopamine in prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. Psychopharmacology 2000, 149, 181–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Caine, S.B.; Plant, S.; Furbish, K.; Yerton, M.; Smaragdi, E.; Niclou, B.; Lorusso, J.; Chang, J.; Bitter, C.; Basu, A.; et al. Sprague Dawley rats from different vendors vary in the modulation of prepulse inhibition of startle (PPI) by dopamine, acetylcholine, and glutamate drugs. Psychopharmacology 2023, 240, 2005–2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Kutlu, M.G.; Zachry, J.E.; Melugin, P.R.; Cajigas, S.A.; Chevee, M.F.; Kelly, S.J.; Kutlu, B.; Tian, L.; Siciliano, C.A.; Calipari, E.S. Dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens core signals perceived saliency. Curr. Biol. 2021, 31, 4748–4761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Lisman, J.E.; Grace, A.A. The hippocampal-VTA loop: Controlling the entry of information into long-term memory. Neuron 2005, 46, 703–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Swerdlow, N.R.; Shoemaker, J.M.; Bongiovanni, M.J.; Neary, A.C.; Tochen, L.S.; Saint Marie, R.L. Strain differences in the disruption of prepulse inhibition of startle after systemic and intra-accumbens amphetamine administration. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2007, 87, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Wan, F.; Swerdlow, N.R. Intra-accumbens infusion of quinpirole impairs sensorimotor gating of acoustic startle in rats. Psychopharmacology 1993, 113, 103–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Auclair, A.L.; Kleven, M.S.; Besnard, J.; Depoortere, R.; Newman-Tancredi, A. Actions of novel antipsychotic agents on apomorphine-induced PPI disruption: Influence of combined serotonin 5-HT 1A receptor activation and dopamine D 2 receptor blockade. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006, 31, 1900–1909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Schwarzkopf, S.B.; Bruno, J.P.; Mitra, T. Effects of haloperidol and SCH 23390 on acoustic startle and prepulse inhibition under basal and stimulated conditions. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 1993, 17, 1023–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hoffman, D.C.; Donovan, H. D1 and D2 dopamine receptor antagonists reverse prepulse inhibition deficits in an animal model of schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology 1994, 115, 447–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ralph, R.J.; Caine, S.B. Dopamine D1 and D2 agonist effects on prepulse inhibition and locomotion: Comparison of Sprague-Dawley rats to Swiss-Webster, 129X1/SvJ, C57BL/6J, and DBA/2J mice. JPET 2005, 312, 733–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Schwienbacher, I.; Fendt, M.; Hauber, W.; Koch, M. Dopamine D1 receptors and adenosine A1 receptors in the rat nucleus accumbens regulate motor activity but not prepulse inhibition. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2002, 444, 161–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Heilbronner, S.R.; Rodriguez-Romaguera, J.; Quirk, G.J.; Groenewegen, H.J.; Haber, S.N. Circuit-based corticostriatal homologies between rat and primate. Biol. Psychiatry 2016, 80, 509–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Haber, S.N. Corticostriatal circuitry. DCNS 2016, 18, 7–21. [Google Scholar]
  35. Santos-Carrasco, D.; De la Casa, L.G. Prepulse inhibition deficit as a transdiagnostic process in neuropsychiatric disorders: A systematic review. BMC Psychol. 2023, 11, 226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Swerdlow, N.R.; Paulsen, J.; Braff, D.L.; Butters, N.; Geyer, M.A.; Swenson, M.R. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic and tactile startle response in patients with Huntington’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 1995, 58, 192–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Mao, Z.; Bo, Q.; Li, W.; Wang, Z.; Ma, X.; Wang, C. Prepulse inhibition in patients with bipolar disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry 2019, 19, 282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. San-Martin, R.; Zimiani, M.I.; de Ávila, M.A.V.; Shuhama, R.; Del-Ben, C.M.; Menezes, P.R.; Fraga, F.J.; Salum, C. Early Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder Patients Display Reduced Neural Prepulse Inhibition. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Storozheva, Z.I.; Akhapkin, R.V.; Bolotina, O.V.; Korendrukhina, A.; Novototsky-Vlasov, V.Y.; Shcherbakova, I.V.; Kirenskaya, A.V. Sensorimotor and sensory gating in depression, anxiety, and their comorbidity. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 2021, 22, 183–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. San-Martin, R.; Castro, L.A.; Menezes, P.R.; Fraga, F.J.; Simões, P.W.; Salum, C. Meta-analysis of sensorimotor gating deficits in patients with schizophrenia evaluated by prepulse inhibition test. Schizophr. Bull. 2020, 46, 1482–1497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gómez-Nieto, R.; Hormigo, S.; López, D.E. Prepulse inhibition of the auditory startle reflex assessment as a hallmark of brainstem sensorimotor gating mechanisms. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kohl, S.; Heekeren, K.; Klosterkötter, J.; Kuhn, J. Prepulse inhibition in psychiatric disorders–apart from schizophrenia. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2013, 47, 445–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. McCutcheon, R.A.; Abi-Dargham, A.; Howes, O.D. Schizophrenia, dopamine and the striatum: From biology to symptoms. Trends Neurosci. 2019, 42, 205–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Mitelman, S.A.; Bralet, M.C.; Haznedar, M.M.; Hollander, E.; Shihabuddin, L.; Hazlett, E.A.; Buchsbaum, M.S. Positron emission tomography assessment of cerebral glucose metabolic rates in autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia. Brain Imaging Behav. 2018, 12, 532–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Pinkham, A.E.; Liu, P.; Lu, H.; Kriegsman, M.; Simpson, C.; Tamminga, C. Amygdala hyperactivity at rest in paranoid individuals with schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 2015, 172, 784–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Białoń, M.; Wąsik, A. Advantages and limitations of animal schizophrenia models. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2022, 23, 5968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. McCutcheon, R.A.; Beck, K.; Jauhar, S.; Howes, O.D. Defining the locus of dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis and test of the mesolimbic hypothesis. Schizophr. Bull. 2018, 44, 1301–1311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Uliana, D.L.; Zhu, X.; Gomes, F.V.; Grace, A.A. Using animal models for the studies of schizophrenia and depression: The value of translational models for treatment and prevention. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 935320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Leumann, L.; Sterchi, D.; Vollenweider, F.; Ludewig, K.; Früh, H. A neural network approach to the acoustic startle reflex and prepulse inhibition. Brain Res. Bull. 2001, 56, 101–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Ramirez-Moreno, D.F.; Sejnowski, T.J. A computational model for the modulation of the prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex. Biol. Cybern. 2012, 106, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Schmajuk, N.; Larrauri, J.; Hagenbuch, N.; Levin, E.; Feldon, J.; Yee, B. Startle and prepulse inhibition as a function of background noise: A computational and experimental analysis. Behav. Brain Res. 2006, 170, 182–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Hoffman, H.S.; Stitt, C.L.; Leitner, D.S. The optimum interpulse inverval for inhibition of acoustic startle in the rat. JASA 1980, 68, 1218–1220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ison, J.; McAdam, D.; Hammond, G. Latency and amplitude changes in the acoustic startle reflex of the rat produced by variation in auditory prestimulation. Physiol. Behav. 1973, 10, 1035–1039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Reijmers, L.G.; Peeters, B.W. Effects of acoustic prepulses on the startle reflex in rats: A parametric analysis. Brain Res. 1994, 661, 274–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Plappert, C.F.; Pilz, P.K.; Schnitzler, H.U. Factors governing prepulse inhibition and prepulse facilitation of the acoustic startle response in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2004, 152, 403–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Furuya, K.; Katsumata, Y.; Ishibashi, M.; Matsumoto, Y.; Morimoto, T.; Aonishi, T. Computational model predicts the neural mechanisms of prepulse inhibition in Drosophila larvae. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 15211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Jovanic, T.; Schneider-Mizell, C.M.; Shao, M.; Masson, J.B.; Denisov, G.; Fetter, R.D.; Mensh, B.D.; Truman, J.W.; Cardona, A.; Zlatic, M. Competitive disinhibition mediates behavioral choice and sequences in Drosophila. Cell 2016, 167, 858–870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Braff, D.L.; Geyer, M.A.; Swerdlow, N.R. Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: Normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology 2001, 156, 234–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Valsamis, B.; Schmid, S. Habituation and prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in rodents. JoVE 2011, 55, e3446. [Google Scholar]
  60. Roell, K.R.; Reif, D.M.; Motsinger-Reif, A.A. An introduction to terminology and methodology of chemical synergy—Perspectives from across disciplines. Front. Pharmacol. 2017, 8, 158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Xue, C.; Li, X.H.; Liu, M.Q.; Yang, X.C.; Li, G.H.; Xu, R.J.; Wang, J.; Zhang, W.N. Enhancing excitatory projections from the ventral subiculum to the nucleus accumbens shell contribute to the MK-801-induced impairment of prepulse inhibition. Neurosci. Lett. 2020, 731, 135024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Mosher, L.J.; Frau, R.; Pardu, A.; Pes, R.; Devoto, P.; Bortolato, M. Selective activation of D1 dopamine receptors impairs sensorimotor gating in Long-Evans rats. Br. J. Pharmacol. 2016, 173, 2122–2134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Blanca, M.J.; Arnau, J.; García-Castro, F.J.; Alarcón, R.; Bono, R. Repeated measures ANOVA and adjusted F-tests when sphericity is violated: Which procedure is best? Front. psychol. 2023, 14, 1192453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Frost, W.N.; Tian, L.M.; Hoppe, T.A.; Mongeluzi, D.L.; Wang, J. A cellular mechanism for prepulse inhibition. Neuron 2003, 40, 991–1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Matsumoto, Y.; Shimizu, K.; Arahata, K.; Suzuki, M.; Shimizu, A.; Takei, K.; Yamauchi, J.; Hakeda-Suzuki, S.; Suzuki, T.; Morimoto, T. Prepulse inhibition in Drosophila melanogaster larvae. Biol. Open 2018, 7, bio034710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Simons-Weidenmaier, N.S.; Weber, M.; Plappert, C.F.; Pilz, P.K.; Schmid, S. Synaptic depression and short-term habituation are located in the sensory part of the mammalian startle pathway. BMC Neurosci. 2006, 7, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Blumenthal, T.D.; Reynolds, J.Z.; Spence, T.E. Support for the interruption and protection hypotheses of prepulse inhibition of startle: Evidence from a modified Attention Network Test. Psychophysiology 2015, 52, 397–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Kodsi, M.; Swerdlow, N.R. Prepulse inhibition in the rat is regulated by ventral and caudodorsal striato-pallidal circuitry. Behav. Neurosci. 1995, 109, 912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Johansen, P.; Hu, X.T.; White, F. Relationship between D 1 dopamine receptors, adenylate cyclase, and the electrophysiological responses of rat nucleus accumbens neurons. J. Neural Transm. 1991, 86, 97–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Nicola, S.M.; Surmeier, D.J.; Malenka, R.C. Dopaminergic modulation of neuronal excitability in the striatum and nucleus accumbens. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2000, 23, 185–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Hopf, F.W.; Cascini, M.G.; Gordon, A.S.; Diamond, I.; Bonci, A. Cooperative activation of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors increases spike firing of nucleus accumbens neurons via G-protein βγ subunits. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 5079–5087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Kretschmer, B.D.; Koch, M. The ventral pallidum mediates disruption of prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response induced by dopamine agonists, but not by NMDA antagonists. Brain Res. 1998, 798, 204–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Rodrigues, S.; Ferreira, T.L. Muscimol injection into the substantia nigra but not globus pallidus affects prepulse inhibition and startle reflex. Neuropharmacology 2020, 162, 107796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Gut, N.K.; Winn, P. The pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus—A functional hypothesis from the comparative literature. J. Mov. Disord. 2016, 31, 615–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Martinez-Gonzalez, C.; Bolam, J.P.; Mena-Segovia, J. Topographical organization of the pedunculopontine nucleus. Front. Neuroanat. 2011, 5, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Rodrigues, S.; Salum, C.; Ferreira, T.L. Dorsal striatum D1-expressing neurons are involved with sensorimotor gating on prepulse inhibition test. J. Psychopharmacol. 2017, 31, 505–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Swerdlow, N.R.; Bongiovanni, M.J.; Tochen, L.; Shoemaker, J.M. Separable noradrenergic and dopaminergic regulation of prepulse inhibition in rats: Implications for predictive validity and Tourette Syndrome. Psychopharmacology 2006, 186, 246–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Ou, H.; Tang, J.; Guo, G.; Shi, M.; Yang, C.; Chen, W. TCB-2, a 5-hydroxytryptamine 2A receptor agonist, disrupts prepulse inhibition in the ventral pallidum and nucleus accumbens. Behav. Brain Res. 2023, 437, 114127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Pedrazzi, J.F.C.; Issy, A.; Gomes, F.; Guimarães, F.S.; Del-Bel, E.A. Cannabidiol effects in the prepulse inhibition disruption induced by amphetamine. Psychopharmacology 2015, 232, 3057–3065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Zahm, D.S.; Cheng, A.Y.; Lee, T.J.; Ghobadi, C.W.; Schwartz, Z.M.; Geisler, S.; Parsely, K.P.; Gruber, C.; Veh, R.W. Inputs to the midbrain dopaminergic complex in the rat, with emphasis on extended amygdala-recipient sectors. J. Comp. Neurol. 2011, 519, 3159–3188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Floresco, S.B.; Yang, C.R.; Phillips, A.G.; Blaha, C.D. Association basolateral amygdala stimulation evokes glutamate receptor-dependent dopamine efflux in the nucleus accumbens of the anaesthetized rat. Eur. J. Neurosci 1998, 10, 1241–1251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Jones, J.L.; Day, J.J.; Aragona, B.J.; Wheeler, R.A.; Wightman, R.M.; Carelli, R.M. Basolateral amygdala modulates terminal dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens and conditioned responding. Biol. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 737–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Brischoux, F.; Chakraborty, S.; Brierley, D.I.; Ungless, M.A. Phasic excitation of dopamine neurons in ventral VTA by noxious stimuli. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 4894–4899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Matsumoto, M.; Hikosaka, O. Two types of dopamine neuron distinctly convey positive and negative motivational signals. Nature 2009, 459, 837–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Matsumoto, M.; Takada, M. Distinct representations of cognitive and motivational signals in midbrain dopamine neurons. Neuron 2013, 79, 1011–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Tang, W.; Kochubey, O.; Kintscher, M.; Schneggenburger, R. A VTA to basal amygdala dopamine projection contributes to signal salient somatosensory events during fear learning. J. Neurosci. 2020, 40, 3969–3980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Zheng, J.; Anderson, K.L.; Leal, S.L.; Shestyuk, A.; Gulsen, G.; Mnatsakanyan, L.; Vadera, S.; Hsu, F.P.; Yassa, M.A.; Knight, R.T.; et al. Amygdala-hippocampal dynamics during salient information processing. Nat. Commun 2017, 8, 14413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Lee, H.J.; Gallagher, M.; Holland, P.C. The central amygdala projection to the substantia nigra reflects prediction error information in appetitive conditioning. Learn. Mem. 2010, 17, 531–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Reppucci, C.J.; Petrovich, G.D. Organization of connections between the amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and lateral hypothalamus: A single and double retrograde tracing study in rats. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016, 221, 2937–2962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Fadel, J.; Deutch, A. Anatomical substrates of orexin–dopamine interactions: Lateral hypothalamic projections to the ventral tegmental area. Neuroscience 2002, 111, 379–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Leinninger, G.M.; Jo, Y.H.; Leshan, R.L.; Louis, G.W.; Yang, H.; Barrera, J.G.; Wilson, H.; Opland, D.M.; Faouzi, M.A.; Gong, Y.; et al. Leptin acts via leptin receptor-expressing lateral hypothalamic neurons to modulate the mesolimbic dopamine system and suppress feeding. Cell Metab. 2009, 10, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Aguilar, B.L.; Forcelli, P.A.; Malkova, L. Inhibition of the substantia nigra pars reticulata produces divergent effects on sensorimotor gating in rats and monkeys. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 9369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Koch, M.; Fendt, M.; Kretschmer, B.D. Role of the substantia nigra pars reticulata in sensorimotor gating, measured by prepulse inhibition of startle in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2000, 117, 153–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Ranjbar-Slamloo, Y.; Fazlali, Z. Dopamine and noradrenaline in the brain; overlapping or dissociate functions? Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2020, 12, 334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Zhang, Y.X.; Yamashita, H.; Ohshita, T.; Sawamoto, N.; Nakamura, S. ATP increases extracellular dopamine level through stimulation of P2Y purinoceptors in the rat striatum. Brain Res. 1995, 691, 205–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Cachope, R.; Cheer, J.F. Local control of striatal dopamine release. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Swerdlow, N.R.; Braff, D.L.; Geyer, M.A. Sensorimotor gating of the startle reflex: What we said 25 years ago, what has happened since then, and what comes next. J. Psychopharmacol. 2016, 30, 1072–1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Swerdlow, N.R.; Krupin, A.S.; Bongiovanni, M.J.; Shoemaker, J.M.; Goins, J.C.; Hammer, R.P. Heritable differences in the dopaminergic regulation of behavior in rats: Relationship to D 2-like receptor G-protein function. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006, 31, 721–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Swerdlow, N.R.; Stephany, N.; Wasserman, L.C.; Talledo, J.; Shoemaker, J.; Auerbach, P.P. Amphetamine effects on prepulse inhibition across-species: Replication and parametric extension. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003, 28, 640–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Smith, Y.; Wichmann, T.; DeLong, M.R. Corticostriatal and mesocortical dopamine systems: Do species differences matter? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 15, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Marinelli, M.; McCutcheon, J.E. Heterogeneity of dopamine neuron activity across traits and states. Neuroscience 2014, 282, 176–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Creese, I.; Stewart, K.; Snyder, S.H. Species variations in dopamine receptor binding. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 1979, 60, 55–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Devlin, B.; Kelsoe, J.R.; Sklar, P.; Daly, M.J.; O’Donovan, M.C.; Craddock, N.; Sullivan, P.F.; Smoller, J.W.; Kendler, K.S.; Wray, N.R.; et al. Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nat. Genet. 2013, 45, 984–994. [Google Scholar]
  104. Koch, M.; Lingenhöhl, K.; Pilz, P. Loss of the acoustic startle response following neurotoxic lesions of the caudal pontine reticular formation: Possible role of giant neurons. Neuroscience 1992, 49, 617–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Zheng, A.; Schmid, S. A review of the neural basis underlying the acoustic startle response with a focus on recent developments in mammals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2023, 148, 105129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Weisz, C.J.; Williams, S.P.G.; Eckard, C.S.; Divito, C.B.; Ferreira, D.W.; Fantetti, K.N.; Dettwyler, S.A.; Cai, H.M.; Rubio, M.E.; Kandler, K.; et al. Outer hair cell glutamate signaling through type II spiral ganglion afferents activates neurons in the cochlear nucleus in response to nondamaging sounds. J. Neurosci. 2021, 41, 2930–2943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Abbott, L.F.; Varela, J.; Sen, K.; Nelson, S. Synaptic depression and cortical gain control. Science 1997, 275, 221–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Ryugo, D.K.; Milinkeviciute, G. Differential projections from the cochlear nucleus to the inferior colliculus in the mouse. Front. Neural Circuits 2023, 17, 1229746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Liu, X.; Huang, H.; Snutch, T.P.; Cao, P.; Wang, L.; Wang, F. The superior colliculus: Cell types, connectivity, and behavior. Neurosci. Bull. 2022, 38, 1519–1540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Fendt, M.; Schwienbacher, I.; Koch, M. Amygdaloid N-methyl-d-aspartate and γ-aminobutyric acidA receptors regulate sensorimotor gating in a dopamine-dependent way in rats. Neuroscience 2000, 98, 55–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Rosenkranz, J.A.; Grace, A.A. Cellular mechanisms of infralimbic and prelimbic prefrontal cortical inhibition and dopaminergic modulation of basolateral amygdala neurons in vivo. J. Neurosci. 2002, 22, 324–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. McGinty, V.; Grace, A. Activity-dependent depression of medial prefrontal cortex inputs to accumbens neurons by the basolateral amygdala. Neuroscience 2009, 162, 1429–1436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Belujon, P.; Grace, A.A. Critical role of the prefrontal cortex in the regulation of hippocampus–accumbens information flow. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 9797–9805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Floresco, S.B.; Blaha, C.D.; Yang, C.R.; Phillips, A.G. Modulation of hippocampal and amygdalar-evoked activity of nucleus accumbens neurons by dopamine: Cellular mechanisms of input selection. J. Neurosci. 2001, 21, 2851–2860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Gill, K.M.; Grace, A.A. Heterogeneous processing of amygdala and hippocampal inputs in the rostral and caudal subregions of the nucleus accumbens. IJNP 2011, 14, 1301–1314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Goto, Y.; Grace, A.A. Dopaminergic modulation of limbic and cortical drive of nucleus accumbens in goal-directed behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 2005, 8, 805–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. McGinty, V.B.; Grace, A.A. Timing-dependent regulation of evoked spiking in nucleus accumbens neurons by integration of limbic and prefrontal cortical inputs. J. Neurophysiol. 2009, 101, 1823–1835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. O’Donnell, P.; Grace, A.A. Synaptic interactions among excitatory afferents to nucleus accumbens neurons: Hippocampal gating of prefrontal cortical input. J. Neurosci. 1995, 15, 3622–3639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. West, A.R.; Grace, A.A. Opposite Influences of Endogenous Dopamine D1 and D2 Receptor Activation on Activity States and Electrophysiological Properties of Striatal Neurons: Studies CombiningIn Vivo Intracellular Recordings and Reverse Microdialysis. J. Neurosci. 2002, 22, 294–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Holloway, Z.R.; Freels, T.G.; Comstock, J.F.; Nolen, H.G.; Sable, H.J.; Lester, D.B. Comparing phasic dopamine dynamics in the striatum, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex. Synapse 2019, 73, e22074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Salum, C.O.R. Estudo comportamental, farmacológico e computacional dos sistemas cerebrais envolvidos na ansiedade, no medo condicionado e na atenção seletiva. Ph.D. Thesis, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil, 2000. (In Portuguese). [Google Scholar]
  122. Salum, C.; Roque da Silva Filho, A.C.; Pickering, A. Modelagem Computacional dos Mecanismos Dopaminérgicos Envolvidos na Aprendizagem e na Atenção Seletiva. In Proceedings of the V Brazilian Conference on Neural Networks, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil, 2–5 April 2001; pp. 631–636. (In Portuguese). [Google Scholar]
  123. Floresco, S.B.; West, A.R.; Ash, B.; Moore, H.; Grace, A.A. Afferent modulation of dopamine neuron firing differentially regulates tonic and phasic dopamine transmission. Nat. Neurosci. 2003, 6, 968–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Missale, C.; Nash, S.R.; Robinson, S.W.; Jaber, M.; Caron, M.G. Dopamine receptors: From structure to function. Physiol. Rev. 1998, 78, 189–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Stahl, S.M. Drugs for psychosis and mood: Unique actions at D3, D2, and D1 dopamine receptor subtypes. CNS Spectr. 2017, 22, 375–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Ahuja, T.; Wu, S. Intrinsic membrane properties and synaptic response characteristics of neurons in the rat’s external cortex of the inferior colliculus. Neuroscience 2007, 145, 851–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Carp, J.S.; Tennissen, A.M.; Mongeluzi, D.L.; Dudek, C.J.; Chen, X.Y.; Wolpaw, J.R. An in vitro protocol for recording from spinal motoneurons of adult rats. J. Neurophysiol. 2008, 100, 474–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Ehrlich, D.; Ryan, S.; Rainnie, D. Postnatal development of electrophysiological properties of principal neurons in the rat basolateral amygdala. J. Physiol. 2012, 590, 4819–4838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Mancilla, J.G.; Manis, P.B. Two distinct types of inhibition mediated by cartwheel cells in the dorsal cochlear nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 2009, 102, 1287–1295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Taverna, S.; Van Dongen, Y.C.; Groenewegen, H.J.; Pennartz, C.M. Direct physiological evidence for synaptic connectivity between medium-sized spiny neurons in rat nucleus accumbens in situ. J. Neurophysiol. 2004, 91, 1111–1121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Lingenhöhl, K.; Friauf, E. Giant neurons in the caudal pontine reticular formation receive short latency acoustic input: An intracellular recording and HRP-study in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 1992, 325, 473–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Representation of the neural network for ASR, PPI, and their modulation. The model is composed by the following structures: ASR Pathway (blue boxes): Cochlea (Ch); Cochlear Root Nucleus (CRN); Caudal Pontine Reticular Nucleus (CPRN); Motor Neuron (MN); PPI Pathway (yellow boxes): Inferior Colliculus (IC); Superior Colliculus (SC); Pedunculopontine Tegmental Nucleus (PPTg); Modulatory Pathway (red boxes): Excitatory (mPFC) and Inhibitory (mPFCI) Medial Prefrontal Cortex Subpopulations; Excitatory (Amyg) and Inhibitory (AmygI) Amygdala Subpopulations; Nucleus Accumbens (NAc); Ventral Pallidum (VP); Ventral Tegmental Area (VTA). Black lines with arrow heads represent glutamatergic excitatory projections, red lines with round heads represent GABAergic inhibitory connections, and green lines with diamond heads represent dopaminergic connections. The connection between CRN and CPRN is subjected to short-term depression (STD), as indicated by the dotted circle. The dotted black line from mPFC to NAc represents the tonic glutamatergic projection that regulates the dopaminergic system. The gray rectangle represents the sound input to the cochlea, and the parabola represents the activity of the motor neurons, which is the network output.
Figure 1. Representation of the neural network for ASR, PPI, and their modulation. The model is composed by the following structures: ASR Pathway (blue boxes): Cochlea (Ch); Cochlear Root Nucleus (CRN); Caudal Pontine Reticular Nucleus (CPRN); Motor Neuron (MN); PPI Pathway (yellow boxes): Inferior Colliculus (IC); Superior Colliculus (SC); Pedunculopontine Tegmental Nucleus (PPTg); Modulatory Pathway (red boxes): Excitatory (mPFC) and Inhibitory (mPFCI) Medial Prefrontal Cortex Subpopulations; Excitatory (Amyg) and Inhibitory (AmygI) Amygdala Subpopulations; Nucleus Accumbens (NAc); Ventral Pallidum (VP); Ventral Tegmental Area (VTA). Black lines with arrow heads represent glutamatergic excitatory projections, red lines with round heads represent GABAergic inhibitory connections, and green lines with diamond heads represent dopaminergic connections. The connection between CRN and CPRN is subjected to short-term depression (STD), as indicated by the dotted circle. The dotted black line from mPFC to NAc represents the tonic glutamatergic projection that regulates the dopaminergic system. The gray rectangle represents the sound input to the cochlea, and the parabola represents the activity of the motor neurons, which is the network output.
Brainsci 14 00502 g001
Figure 2. Activity of each model unit in a PPI prepulse + pulse trial. In each graph, the x axis represents the time of simulation in milliseconds (ms) and the y axis represents the unit activity (arbitrary units). The black, green, and blue arrows represent glutamatergic connections, the red arrows with circle endings represent GABAergic connections, and the green arrows with diamond endings represent the dopaminergic modulatory connections. The black dashed arrow represents the tonic glutamatergic projection from mPFC to NAc, regulating the extracellular concentration of dopamine. The dashed circle in the connection from CRN to CPRN indicates the STD mechanism between these units. In the trial simulated, the prepulse had an intensity of 25 dB above background, pulse was 60 dB above background, and ISI was 80 ms. Both stimuli had durations of 30 ms.
Figure 2. Activity of each model unit in a PPI prepulse + pulse trial. In each graph, the x axis represents the time of simulation in milliseconds (ms) and the y axis represents the unit activity (arbitrary units). The black, green, and blue arrows represent glutamatergic connections, the red arrows with circle endings represent GABAergic connections, and the green arrows with diamond endings represent the dopaminergic modulatory connections. The black dashed arrow represents the tonic glutamatergic projection from mPFC to NAc, regulating the extracellular concentration of dopamine. The dashed circle in the connection from CRN to CPRN indicates the STD mechanism between these units. In the trial simulated, the prepulse had an intensity of 25 dB above background, pulse was 60 dB above background, and ISI was 80 ms. Both stimuli had durations of 30 ms.
Brainsci 14 00502 g002
Figure 3. Variation of ISI and prepulse intensity on the %PPI simulation. In both tests, it was simulated trials of prepulse + pulse with pulse intensity set to 60 dB above background and pulse and prepulse duration of 30 ms. (A) ISI test. The ISI varied between 0 ms to 250 ms in steps of 10 ms between the simulations. This test was conducted with the prepulse intensities of 15, 20, and 25 dB above background. The maximum value of PPI is reached around 70 ms to 100 ms (depending on the prepulse intensity), below and above which the %PPI obtained in the simulation decreased, indicating an optimal ISI within this interval. (B) Prepulse intensity test. The ISIs used in this tests were 60, 70, and 80 ms. The prepulse intensity varied from 0 dB to 100 dB above background in steps of 5 dB between the simulations. The %PPI increases as the prepulse intensity increases from 0 dB and reaches a maximum %PPI for intensities between 20 dB and 40 dB (depending on the ISI). Above these intensities, the %PPI starts to decrease and above 60 dB there are negative values of %PPI.
Figure 3. Variation of ISI and prepulse intensity on the %PPI simulation. In both tests, it was simulated trials of prepulse + pulse with pulse intensity set to 60 dB above background and pulse and prepulse duration of 30 ms. (A) ISI test. The ISI varied between 0 ms to 250 ms in steps of 10 ms between the simulations. This test was conducted with the prepulse intensities of 15, 20, and 25 dB above background. The maximum value of PPI is reached around 70 ms to 100 ms (depending on the prepulse intensity), below and above which the %PPI obtained in the simulation decreased, indicating an optimal ISI within this interval. (B) Prepulse intensity test. The ISIs used in this tests were 60, 70, and 80 ms. The prepulse intensity varied from 0 dB to 100 dB above background in steps of 5 dB between the simulations. The %PPI increases as the prepulse intensity increases from 0 dB and reaches a maximum %PPI for intensities between 20 dB and 40 dB (depending on the ISI). Above these intensities, the %PPI starts to decrease and above 60 dB there are negative values of %PPI.
Brainsci 14 00502 g003
Figure 4. Habituation of ASR units. Repeated pulse presentation led to a reduction in ASR amplitude (MN activity). The test session was composed of 10 pulse trials and ITIs of 5–15 s, 10–15 s, and 20–25 s. For each ITI, ten simulations were ran with randomly selected model parameters. The percentage of decrease in MN activity was calculated related to the first pulse presentation. (A) Example of a simulation with ITI of 5–15 s. (B) Example of a simulation with ITI of 10–15 s. (C) Example of a simulation with ITI of 20–25 s. (D) Percentage of reduction in ASR Amplitude (MN activity) by repeated pulse presentation for each ITI tested. The MN activity reductions by the repeated pulse presentation were different across the ITIs used. There was a higher percentage reduction in MN activity for the ITI of 5–15 s (maximum reduction of 11.14%) compared to the ITI of 10–15 s (maximum of 8.31%) and 20–25 s (3.69%).
Figure 4. Habituation of ASR units. Repeated pulse presentation led to a reduction in ASR amplitude (MN activity). The test session was composed of 10 pulse trials and ITIs of 5–15 s, 10–15 s, and 20–25 s. For each ITI, ten simulations were ran with randomly selected model parameters. The percentage of decrease in MN activity was calculated related to the first pulse presentation. (A) Example of a simulation with ITI of 5–15 s. (B) Example of a simulation with ITI of 10–15 s. (C) Example of a simulation with ITI of 20–25 s. (D) Percentage of reduction in ASR Amplitude (MN activity) by repeated pulse presentation for each ITI tested. The MN activity reductions by the repeated pulse presentation were different across the ITIs used. There was a higher percentage reduction in MN activity for the ITI of 5–15 s (maximum reduction of 11.14%) compared to the ITI of 10–15 s (maximum of 8.31%) and 20–25 s (3.69%).
Brainsci 14 00502 g004
Figure 5. Effect of the manipulation of GABA factor in amygdala and VP over the %PPI and their activity in a trial of PPI. For the GABA factor between 0.00 and 1.00, there was an inhibition of the unit under test (simulating the effect of a GABAergic agonist) and for values between 1.00 and 2.00, there was a hyperactivation of the unit (simulating the effect of a GABAergic antagonist). This test was conducted by varying the GABA factor in the amygdala (Equations (A11) and (A10) in the Appendix A) and in VP (Equation (A21) in the Appendix A)) from 0.00 to 2.00 in steps of 0.10 between simulations. We simulated trials of prepulse + pulse with pulse intensity of 60 dB above background, prepulse intensity of 25 dB, ISI of 80 ms and duration of prepulse and pulse of 30 ms. (A) The horizontal axis indicates the value of the GABA factor in the VP. The vertical axis indicates the value of the GABA factor in the amygdala. The horizontal line at 1.00 for the GABA factor in the amygdala corresponds to the manipulation of only VP activity. The vertical line at 1.00 for the GABA factor in VP corresponds to the manipulation of only amygdala activity. The results are shown as difference in %PPI from control (GABA factor at 1.00 in both amygdala and VP). Negative values for %PPI indicate less inhibition compared to control, while positive values indicate more inhibition. The inhibition and hyperactivation of amygdala and the inhibition of VP cause a reduction in the %PPI obtained in the simulation. The combination of amygdala inhibition and VP hyperactivation, amygdala hyperactivation, and VP inhibition or amygdala hyperactivation and VP hyperactivation caused a further decrease in the %PPI compared to the manipulations of amygdala alone or VP alone. Interestingly, the inactivation of both amygdala and VP attenuated the reduction in %PPI caused by the inhibition of amygdala alone or VP alone. (B) Activities in the excitatory (Amygdala) and inhibitory (Amygdala I.) subpopulations in amygdala for five different values of GABA factor in amygdala. (C) VP activity for five different values of GABA factor in VP.
Figure 5. Effect of the manipulation of GABA factor in amygdala and VP over the %PPI and their activity in a trial of PPI. For the GABA factor between 0.00 and 1.00, there was an inhibition of the unit under test (simulating the effect of a GABAergic agonist) and for values between 1.00 and 2.00, there was a hyperactivation of the unit (simulating the effect of a GABAergic antagonist). This test was conducted by varying the GABA factor in the amygdala (Equations (A11) and (A10) in the Appendix A) and in VP (Equation (A21) in the Appendix A)) from 0.00 to 2.00 in steps of 0.10 between simulations. We simulated trials of prepulse + pulse with pulse intensity of 60 dB above background, prepulse intensity of 25 dB, ISI of 80 ms and duration of prepulse and pulse of 30 ms. (A) The horizontal axis indicates the value of the GABA factor in the VP. The vertical axis indicates the value of the GABA factor in the amygdala. The horizontal line at 1.00 for the GABA factor in the amygdala corresponds to the manipulation of only VP activity. The vertical line at 1.00 for the GABA factor in VP corresponds to the manipulation of only amygdala activity. The results are shown as difference in %PPI from control (GABA factor at 1.00 in both amygdala and VP). Negative values for %PPI indicate less inhibition compared to control, while positive values indicate more inhibition. The inhibition and hyperactivation of amygdala and the inhibition of VP cause a reduction in the %PPI obtained in the simulation. The combination of amygdala inhibition and VP hyperactivation, amygdala hyperactivation, and VP inhibition or amygdala hyperactivation and VP hyperactivation caused a further decrease in the %PPI compared to the manipulations of amygdala alone or VP alone. Interestingly, the inactivation of both amygdala and VP attenuated the reduction in %PPI caused by the inhibition of amygdala alone or VP alone. (B) Activities in the excitatory (Amygdala) and inhibitory (Amygdala I.) subpopulations in amygdala for five different values of GABA factor in amygdala. (C) VP activity for five different values of GABA factor in VP.
Brainsci 14 00502 g005
Figure 6. Simulation of PPI sessions under amygdala and/or VP inhibition. The test session was composed by 74 stimuli presentations including pulse (60 dB above background), prepulse (15, 20 and 25 dB above background), prepulse + pulse, and background. The ITI was set to 5–15 s, the ISI of 80 ms, and duration of pulse and prepulse of 30 ms. Columns represent the groups: VP group (GABA factor at 0.20 in VP), the Amygdala group (GABA factor at 0.20 in amygdala), Amygdala-VP group (GABA factor at 0.20 in both VP and amygdala), and Control group (GABA factor at 1.00 in both units). (A) ASR as m e a n ± S E M . The ASR amplitude for the pulse was higher compared to the ASR amplitude for all the prepulse intensities. The Amygdala group showed higher ASR for PP15 + P60 and PP20 + P60 than all other groups and ASR for PP30 + P60 higher than the Control and Amygdala-VP group. (B) %PPI as m e a n ± S E M . The %PPI of the Amygdala group was lower than the control condition. There was no difference between the Amygdala-VP group compared to the control condition.
Figure 6. Simulation of PPI sessions under amygdala and/or VP inhibition. The test session was composed by 74 stimuli presentations including pulse (60 dB above background), prepulse (15, 20 and 25 dB above background), prepulse + pulse, and background. The ITI was set to 5–15 s, the ISI of 80 ms, and duration of pulse and prepulse of 30 ms. Columns represent the groups: VP group (GABA factor at 0.20 in VP), the Amygdala group (GABA factor at 0.20 in amygdala), Amygdala-VP group (GABA factor at 0.20 in both VP and amygdala), and Control group (GABA factor at 1.00 in both units). (A) ASR as m e a n ± S E M . The ASR amplitude for the pulse was higher compared to the ASR amplitude for all the prepulse intensities. The Amygdala group showed higher ASR for PP15 + P60 and PP20 + P60 than all other groups and ASR for PP30 + P60 higher than the Control and Amygdala-VP group. (B) %PPI as m e a n ± S E M . The %PPI of the Amygdala group was lower than the control condition. There was no difference between the Amygdala-VP group compared to the control condition.
Brainsci 14 00502 g006
Figure 7. Simulation of DA factor manipulation on a PPI trial. The trials simulated had a pulse intensity of 60 dB, prepulse intensity of 25 dB, ISI of 80 ms and duration of stimuli of 30 ms. When 1.00 D A < 0.00 , it was simulated the effect of a dopaminergic antagonist, while for 0.00 < D A 1.00 it was simulated the effect of an agonist. The control condition is defined as the DA factor at 0.00. The purple line indicates the tests with manipulation of both dopaminergic receptor types. The blue line represents the tests with manipulation of only D 1 receptors. The orange line indicates the tests with manipulation of DA factor only in D 2 receptors. (A) Effect of manipulation of DA factor in all the dopaminergic receptors in the model (systemic test), in amygdala, and in NAc. With exception for the D1 receptor in NAc, there was a reduction in the %PPI with the increase of DA factor compared to control. The increase of DA factor in D1 receptors in NAc caused a slight increase in the %PPI compared to control. In all cases, decreasing the DA factor led to an increase in the %PPI compared to control. (B) Amygdala activity for three values of DA factor (−0.6, 0.2 and 1.0) in D 1 , D 2 or both receptors in amygdala. Dashed line represents the control condition (DA factor = 0.0). The decrease in DA factor led to a reduction in amygdala activity, while the increase in DA factor caused an increase in the activity. (C) Activity of inhibitory subpopulation in amygdala for three values of DA factor (−0.6, 0.2 and 1.0) in D 1 , D 2 or both receptors in amygdala. Dashed line represents the control condition (DA factor = 0.0). The decrease in DA factor in amygdala led to an activity increase in the inhibitory subpopulation in amygdala. The increase in DA factor caused a slight decrease in the activity of the amygdala inhibitory subpopulation. (D) NAc activity for three values of DA factor (−0.6, 0.2, and 1.0) in D 1 , D 2 , or both receptors in NAc. Dashed line represents the control condition (DA factor = 0.0). There was a decrease in NAc activity by decreasing the DA factor in D 1 , D 2 , or both receptors. For a DA factor at 0.2, there was a slight increase in NAc activity. In contrast, for a high DA factor (at 1.0), there was a reduction in NAc activity. (E) VP activity for three values of DA factor (−0.6, 0.2 and 1.0) in D 1 , D 2 , or both receptors in NAc. Dashed line represents the control condition (DA factor = 0.0). For the DA factor at 1.0 of D 2 or both receptors in NAc there was an increase in VP activity compared to control.
Figure 7. Simulation of DA factor manipulation on a PPI trial. The trials simulated had a pulse intensity of 60 dB, prepulse intensity of 25 dB, ISI of 80 ms and duration of stimuli of 30 ms. When 1.00 D A < 0.00 , it was simulated the effect of a dopaminergic antagonist, while for 0.00 < D A 1.00 it was simulated the effect of an agonist. The control condition is defined as the DA factor at 0.00. The purple line indicates the tests with manipulation of both dopaminergic receptor types. The blue line represents the tests with manipulation of only D 1 receptors. The orange line indicates the tests with manipulation of DA factor only in D 2 receptors. (A) Effect of manipulation of DA factor in all the dopaminergic receptors in the model (systemic test), in amygdala, and in NAc. With exception for the D1 receptor in NAc, there was a reduction in the %PPI with the increase of DA factor compared to control. The increase of DA factor in D1 receptors in NAc caused a slight increase in the %PPI compared to control. In all cases, decreasing the DA factor led to an increase in the %PPI compared to control. (B) Amygdala activity for three values of DA factor (−0.6, 0.2 and 1.0) in D 1 , D 2 or both receptors in amygdala. Dashed line represents the control condition (DA factor = 0.0). The decrease in DA factor led to a reduction in amygdala activity, while the increase in DA factor caused an increase in the activity. (C) Activity of inhibitory subpopulation in amygdala for three values of DA factor (−0.6, 0.2 and 1.0) in D 1 , D 2 or both receptors in amygdala. Dashed line represents the control condition (DA factor = 0.0). The decrease in DA factor in amygdala led to an activity increase in the inhibitory subpopulation in amygdala. The increase in DA factor caused a slight decrease in the activity of the amygdala inhibitory subpopulation. (D) NAc activity for three values of DA factor (−0.6, 0.2, and 1.0) in D 1 , D 2 , or both receptors in NAc. Dashed line represents the control condition (DA factor = 0.0). There was a decrease in NAc activity by decreasing the DA factor in D 1 , D 2 , or both receptors. For a DA factor at 0.2, there was a slight increase in NAc activity. In contrast, for a high DA factor (at 1.0), there was a reduction in NAc activity. (E) VP activity for three values of DA factor (−0.6, 0.2 and 1.0) in D 1 , D 2 , or both receptors in NAc. Dashed line represents the control condition (DA factor = 0.0). For the DA factor at 1.0 of D 2 or both receptors in NAc there was an increase in VP activity compared to control.
Brainsci 14 00502 g007
Figure 8. Simulation of the increased activity of D 1 and D 2 receptors. The test session was composed of 74 trials among pulse (60 dB above background), prepulse (15, 20, and 25 dB above background), prepulse + pulse, and background. The ITI was set to 5–15 s, the ISI of 80 ms, and duration of pulse and prepulse of 30 ms. The groups were as follows: Systemic group (DA factor at 0.50 in both D 1 and D 2 receptors and in all receptors of every unit that received the dopaminergic input), Amygdala group (DA factor at 0.50 in both D 1 and D 2 receptors in amygdala), NAc group (DA factor at 0.50 in both D 1 and D 2 receptors in NAc), and Control group (DA factor at 0.00 in all receptors). (A) ASR as m e a n ± S E M . The ASR amplitude for the pulse was higher than all ASR amplitude for any prepulse intensities. The systemic group also showed a higher ASR for all prepulse + pulse trials compared to Control, Amygdala, and NAc groups. (B) %PPI as m e a n ± S E M . The %PPIs in Systemic and NAc were lower than the control condition for all prepulse intensities. Except for the PP15, the Amygdala showed lower %PPI than the Control condition. The amygdala and NAc groups showed higher %PPI than the Systemic group for all prepulse intensities.
Figure 8. Simulation of the increased activity of D 1 and D 2 receptors. The test session was composed of 74 trials among pulse (60 dB above background), prepulse (15, 20, and 25 dB above background), prepulse + pulse, and background. The ITI was set to 5–15 s, the ISI of 80 ms, and duration of pulse and prepulse of 30 ms. The groups were as follows: Systemic group (DA factor at 0.50 in both D 1 and D 2 receptors and in all receptors of every unit that received the dopaminergic input), Amygdala group (DA factor at 0.50 in both D 1 and D 2 receptors in amygdala), NAc group (DA factor at 0.50 in both D 1 and D 2 receptors in NAc), and Control group (DA factor at 0.00 in all receptors). (A) ASR as m e a n ± S E M . The ASR amplitude for the pulse was higher than all ASR amplitude for any prepulse intensities. The systemic group also showed a higher ASR for all prepulse + pulse trials compared to Control, Amygdala, and NAc groups. (B) %PPI as m e a n ± S E M . The %PPIs in Systemic and NAc were lower than the control condition for all prepulse intensities. Except for the PP15, the Amygdala showed lower %PPI than the Control condition. The amygdala and NAc groups showed higher %PPI than the Systemic group for all prepulse intensities.
Brainsci 14 00502 g008
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Bezerra, T.O.; Roque, A.C.; Salum, C. A Computational Model for the Simulation of Prepulse Inhibition and Its Modulation by Cortical and Subcortical Units. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 502. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14050502

AMA Style

Bezerra TO, Roque AC, Salum C. A Computational Model for the Simulation of Prepulse Inhibition and Its Modulation by Cortical and Subcortical Units. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(5):502. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14050502

Chicago/Turabian Style

Bezerra, Thiago Ohno, Antonio C. Roque, and Cristiane Salum. 2024. "A Computational Model for the Simulation of Prepulse Inhibition and Its Modulation by Cortical and Subcortical Units" Brain Sciences 14, no. 5: 502. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14050502

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop