Next Article in Journal
Patterns of Equine Small Strongyle Species Infection after Ivermectin Intervention in Thailand: Egg Reappearance Period and Nemabiome Metabarcoding Approach
Next Article in Special Issue
Two Separate Brain Networks for Predicting Trainability and Tracking Training-Related Plasticity in Working Dogs
Previous Article in Journal
Comparative Genome Analysis of Two Streptococcus suis Serotype 8 Strains Identifies Two New Virulence-Associated Genes
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Dog Soundscape: Recurrence, Emotional Impact, Acoustics, and Implications for Dog Observations and Dog–Human Interactions
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Role of Life History and Familiarity in Performance of Working and Non-Working Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) in a Point-Following Task

by
Josephine M. McCartney
1 and
David A. Leavens
2,*
1
School of Life Sciences, University of Sussex, Falmer BN1 9QG, UK
2
School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer BN1 9QH, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2024, 14(4), 573; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14040573
Submission received: 18 January 2024 / Revised: 4 February 2024 / Accepted: 7 February 2024 / Published: 8 February 2024

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

We wondered whether dogs that have been subjected to formal training to work with people were better at reading pointing cues than untrained pet dogs. We compared actively working dogs (search-and-rescue, police dogs, assistance dogs, and so on), which were all from working dog breeds (sports dogs, working dogs, herding dogs) with two other groups: (1) pet dogs not from working dog breeds (toy dogs, mixed breeds, and mongrels) and (2) pet dogs from working dog breeds. We found that both trained dogs and pet dogs from working dog breeds performed better than pet dogs from non-working dog breeds, suggesting that breeding does influence performance beyond evolutionary changes attributable to domestication, per se.

Abstract

Domestic dogs are very successful at following human communicative gestures in paradigms such as the object-choice task. Pet dogs also prefer responding to cues given by a familiar cue-giver and this had not been found in working dogs. Therefore, we tested three groups of dogs in the object-choice task (n = 54): the groups were “Actively working” dogs from working dog breeds, pet dogs from “Non-working breeds” and pet dogs from “Working breeds”. We found that “Actively working” and “Working breeds” dog groups outperformed “Non-working breeds” in following a point in the object-choice task. We also found that both “Actively working” and “Working breeds” preferred a familiar cue-giver over an unfamiliar one, in contrast to previous findings. Therefore, we conclude that dogs’ abilities to perform well in the object-choice task is influenced by the selective history of the breed, and this is then increased by life experience and training.

1. Introduction

Domestic dogs are thought to have evolved from wolves around 25,000 to 40,000 years ago [1]. This makes dogs the earliest domesticated animal, predating cattle, pigs, and sheep who are thought to have been domesticated around 10,500 years ago [2]. A recent large-scale comparison of DNA in ancient and modern dogs reveals a complex history of divergence and admixture in the late Pleistocene to early Holocene; according to the authors, these complex migratory dynamics obscure a clear locus of origin for domestic dogs, but nevertheless they suggest that dogs likely evolved, originally, from a single wolf population or, less likely, multiple closely related wolf populations [1].
Dogs are currently the most common household pet, with 470 million worldwide [3], but they have also been used for working purposes since they were first domesticated, from initially helping humans with hunts to now working as search and rescue, police, herding, and assistance dogs. This means that dogs play an important social role and spend a disproportionately large amount of time around humans when compared with other species. Dogs are thought by some to have developed such human-like social skills as understanding the referential intent of deictic gestures (e.g., pointing) via the process of domestication. This was initially hypothesised by Hare and Tomasello [4], who noted the tens of thousands of years that dogs and humans have spent together, which allows substantial opportunity for co-evolutionary processes to take place. The process of domestication has been shown on a small scale by the ”The Silver Fox Experiment” (reviewed by Dugatkin [5]). Researchers selected a group of foxes (Vulpes vulpes) with non-aggressive traits and found that within six generations, the foxes showed signs of a domestication syndrome: including neotenous morphological changes such as floppy ears, and shorter, more rounded snouts, plus behavioural changes such as reduced levels of stress hormones [5], but see [6], for an argument rejecting the characterisation of these foxes as ”domesticated”. These foxes also outperformed non-“domesticated” foxes in following human communicative gestures [7].
The Domestication Hypothesis holds that dogs are born with the ability to understand human communication gestures. This was supported by Riedel and her colleagues [8] who found that dogs at 6, 8, and 16 weeks of age could all follow human communicative cues; because the puppies at 6 weeks were still being raised by their mothers, this suggests an innate ability provided through a history of domestication. Further support for the Domestication Hypothesis is that wolves (Canis lupus), despite having the same ancestors as dogs, do not show the same ability when it comes to understanding human communicative gestures (but see [9] and below). Ujfalussy and their colleagues [10] compared hand-raised and mother-raised dogs to hand-raised wolves—both groups of dogs performed better than the wolves despite having similar socialisation. Similarly, Lazzaroni and her colleagues [11] compared pet dogs, free-ranging dogs, and research dogs to hand-raised wolves (the latter two groups housed and raised in a research facility), finding that dogs approached humans more for food and a cuddle (i.e., food was not the only motivator); wolves approached only for food and were slower to do so, despite being raised in the same environment as some of the dogs tested. Lampe, Bräuer, Kaminski, and Virányi [12] compared similarly raised wolves and dogs living in packs to pet dogs and found all groups of dogs outperformed wolves on a variety of cognitive tasks. They hypothesised that pet dogs rely more on humans to make decisions as opposed to their wild counterparts, therefore, domestication has allowed them to develop an understanding of non-verbal human communicative gestures to make this easier. Dogs are willing to wait longer for rewards than wolves [13].
A common test of animals’ abilities to follow human cues is the object-choice task. In these studies, an experimenter hides a reward inside an opaque container and the subject is then presented with the container with the reward and one or more empty containers. The subject can only choose one container and will receive the reward if they choose the correct box. To indicate the correct container, a communicative gesture is used. The gesture is most commonly a type of point, however, gazing cues are also used. Anderson, Sallaberry, and Barbier [14] were the among the first to use the object-choice task with non-human primates. They found that capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus) could follow the cue only when pointing was used and failed on a gazing-only condition. Shortly later, Miklósi, Polgárdi, Topál, and Csányi [15] used the object-choice task on the first set of dog subjects; they found that dogs were able to reliably follow a point to find the rewarded container. Domestic dogs have also been found to follow human cues even when the cue is counterproductive, as some dogs chose empty cups, over cups with rewards [16]. Dogs have very powerful olfactory senses, and they will use this sense to find rewards in object-choice tasks [17]; however, when human cues are provided, dogs have been reported to privilege these cues over their own senses [18,19].
Dogs are not the only domestic animal to exhibit this trait as when tested, both dogs and cats (Felis catus), chose a baited container over an empty one if no cue was given but would choose the container, they saw a human approach, indicating that they are influenced by human actions [20]. However, even though dogs may show trust in humans and will initially follow counterproductive cues, they have been found to be less likely to follow a point once a human has shown to be misleading [21].
It is frequently claimed that dogs out-perform apes in paradigms such as the object-choice task, and this has been attributed to their different selective histories [22,23], but, as noted by Clark, Elsherif, and Leavens [9,24,25,26,27,28], to date, dogs and non-human primates have not been matched in testing contexts, task preparation, life history stage, or either quality or quantity of experience with humans. Therefore, as Udell and her colleagues noted [28] (p. 717), “[d]ogs’ sensitivity to human gestures may not be entirely explained by phylogenetic variables; rather, the interactions between genetic, developmental and experiential variables must be considered”.
Numerous studies have reported facilitating effects of training on dogs’ performances in a variety of object-choice tasks, demonstrating, among other things, that dogs can rapidly adapt to novel pointing postures [28,29]. McKinley and Sambrook [30] compared actively working gundogs to pet dogs and found superior performance by the highly trained dogs. Interestingly, they found, on average, gundogs of traditional gundog breeds outperformed gundogs from non-gundog breeds, this may suggest that the traditional breeds are aided by a genetic predisposition to perform better. Wobber, Hare, Koler-Matznick, Wrangham, and Tomasello [31] reported that working breeds are more skilled at using pointing gestures than non-working breeds. They also found that primitive breeds with little human contact and not bred to be pets or to perform working roles, such as the New Guinea Singing Dog, were able to follow human cues but were outperformed by pet dogs on the object-choice task. Udell and her colleagues [32] compared point-following performance on hunting, herding, and livestock-guarding dog breeds, using Airedale Terriers, Border Collies, and Anatolian Shepherds, respectively, reporting that Border Collies and Airedale Terriers performed above chance level when following a momentary distal point, and that, after training, Anatolian Shepherds were also able to perform above chance level, which is not typical of the breed. Thus, life history factors seem to matter in dogs’ performances in human-canine interactions, and we sought to examine the broad effects of training by comparing actively working dogs with dogs of both non-working breeds and working breeds (see Table 1 notes for definitions).
The types of interaction dogs have with a cue-giver is a potential factor in dogs’ performances in formal assessments. For example, dogs prefer receiving petting to vocal praise and do not distinguish between owners and strangers during these activities [33]. A similar finding was reported by Hegedüs, Bálint, Miklósi, and Pongrácz [34], who asked owners to encourage their dogs to select the correct box when an experimenter was pointing to incorrect one; they found that owners had no effect and suggested this was due to the experimenter calling the dog’s name and therefore, the experimenter and dog were engaged in communication. Thus, the effects of familiarity of interactant seem to be moderated by situational factors, such as the quality of interaction.
We were interested in whether actively working dogs were advantaged in the use of human cues by the cue-giver’s familiarity (owner/trainer vs. experimenter) and breed/working classification. We therefore compared an actively working group of dogs with pet dogs from working-dog breeds and pet dogs not from working dog breeds on an object-choice task, in which the owner/trainer or the experimenter provided cues to toy locations. We reasoned that if actively working dogs displayed an advantage over pet dogs from working dog breeds, that this would implicate a facilitatory effect of experience over breeding, and that if both of these groups of dogs outperformed pet dogs from non-working breeds, this would implicate an influence of breeding on performance. Finally, we elected to use familiar and unfamiliar toys in this study, for two reasons: (a) to avoid the potentially distracting effects of food, e.g., Ref. [35] and (b) because of the relative ease in manipulating toy familiarity, compared with food familiarity, during pandemic lockdown conditions.
We set out to answer three questions: (1) Are there systematic differences between dogs of different life histories in performance in the object-choice task? (2) Are there systematic differences between dogs of different life histories in their responses to familiar and unfamiliar cue-givers in the object-choice task? (3) Are there systematic differences between dogs of different life histories in their responses to familiar and unfamiliar toys in the object-choice task?
Table 1. List of breeds, age, sex, and working role for each subject.
Table 1. List of breeds, age, sex, and working role for each subject.
Subj. IDBreedSexAge (M)Working Role
Actively Working a
A1Jack Russell Terrier X SpanielM36Search and Rescue
A2English Springer SpanielF96Search and Rescue
A3Scottish TerrierF23Therapy dog
A4German ShepherdF12General purpose police dog
A5LabradorF13General purpose police dog
A6LabradorF7General purpose police dog
A7Cocker SpanielM84Active drug dog
A8Springer SpanielM36Active explosives dog
A9Working Cocker SpanielM72Beating and agility
A10Springer SpanielM37Victim recovery
A11Labrador X Springer SpanielM10Assistance dog
A12CockapooF36Assistance dog
A13LabradorM108Assistance dog
A14German ShepherdF8General purpose police dog
A15Working Cocker SpanielM120Agility
A16Golden RetrieverF120Agility
A17Working Cocker SpanielF36Agility
A18Border Collie X Welsh SheepdogF96Scent work
A19Working Cocker SpanielM60Agility
Non-working b
B1PugF24N/A
B2Unknown mixM72N/A
B3French Bulldog x PugF36N/A
B4Unknown mixF36N/A
B5MalteseF65N/A
B6Unknown mixM12N/A
B7Unknown mixF7N/A
B8Cavalier King Charles SpanielF120N/A
B9Shih Tzu XM8N/A
B10Shih Tzu XF72N/A
B11Unknown mixM144N/A
B12Shih TzuM96N/A
B13ChihuahuaF12N/A
B14Toy Spaniel X Toy PoodleM11N/A
B15Bichon Frise X Shih TzuF96N/A
B16Unknown mixF96N/A
Working breeds c
C1Cocker Spaniel X Jack RussellM12N/A
C3CockapooF60N/A
C4WhippetF10N/A
C5Show Cocker SpanielF36N/A
C6English Springer SpanielM36N/A
C7Working Cocker SpanielM48N/A
C9Bull LurcherM16N/A
C10Shar-pei X Cane CorsoM24N/A
C11LabradoodleF6N/A
C12CockapooM36N/A
C13Rottweiler X German ShepherdM81N/A
C14CockapooF96N/A
C15Cane CorsoF16N/A
C16DachshundM72N/A
C17Border CollieM11N/A
C18Staffordshire Bull TerrierM9N/A
C19Hungarian VizslaM108N/A
C20Hungarian VizslaF22N/A
C21DachshundF8N/A
Notes: a Actively Working breeds include dogs that are both actively working in the roles indicated and belong to one of the breeds identified by the American Kennel Club [36] as Working, Sporting, or Herding. b Non-working dog breeds include pet dogs that are not actively working and belonging to breeds identified by the American Kennel Club (2021) as Toy, Non-working, Miscellaneous, or in some cases Unknown. c Working breeds include pet dogs that have not been trained for a working role, but belong to breeds identified by the American Kennel Club (2021) as Working, Sporting, Herding, or Hound.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 56 domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) served as subjects in this study. The subjects were divided into three groups, these were “Actively working”, “Non-working breeds”, and “Working breeds”. For the statistical analyses, 54 out of 56 dogs tested were analysed. Two dogs were excluded from the “Working breed” group (one male and one female) due to experimenter error: an unequal number of points were displayed to the familiar and unfamiliar items. This left the “Working breed” group with 19 subjects to be analysed (see Table 1 and notes for elaboration of dog categorisations).
The “Actively Working” dogs were breeds recognised by the American Kennel Club (AKC; [36]), and these were dogs which had been trained for a specific purpose. There were 19 subjects in this group, this included 9 males, 10 females with the age ranging from 7 months to 10 years (mean = 3.75 years).
“Non-working breeds” were dogs who were kept only as pets but not breeds belonging to any of the working groups. These breeds belonged to the toy dog group as defined by the AKC [36]; this group also contained (a) mixed breed dogs, where at least one of the dogs’ parents was from the toy group and the other of unknown breed, and (b) dogs of unknown breed. There were 16 subjects in this group, this included 6 males, 10 females with the age ranging from 7 months to 12 years (mean = 4.72 years).
“Working breed” dogs were also kept only as pets and included dog breeds from the herding, hound, sporting, non-sporting, terrier, and working group as defined by the AKC. This group also included mixed breeds when both breeds were from working groups. These dogs had not been trained to work a specific role. There were 19 subjects in this group which included 12 males, 9 females with an age range of 6 months to 9 years (mean = 3.10 years).
All dogs used in this study were over 6 months of age and had lived with their owners for a minimum of 2 months. For the full list of breeds, see Table 1. No pre-training was given to the dogs, and food rewards were not used. The participants were given a bag of dog treats for taking part in the experiment after the event.

2.2. Materials

The dogs were tested in Pevensey Dog Laboratory (2.5 m in length and 4.55 m in width), to ensure standardisation in the testing context (see Figure 1). The subjects were filmed on a Sony HDR-PJ410 Handycam video camera that was set up prior to the subjects entering the room. A questionnaire was used to assess the subjects’ social experience, age, lifestyle habits, and training level to see if this affected the results (see Appendix A). This involved questions such as: On a scale of 1 to 10 how well does your dog follow commands?

2.3. Design

A repeated measures design was used, with two independent variables: the familiarity of the cue giver (familiar, unfamiliar) and the familiarity of a toy (familiar, unfamiliar). The first author served as the unfamiliar cue-giver and the dog owner served as the familiar cue-giver. We asked the owners to bring in a familiar item for the dogs (i.e., favourite tennis balls/toys). The unfamiliar items were a novel stuffed toy or tennis ball. The dependent variable was whether the subject (a) chose the correct referent, (b) chose the incorrect referent, or (c) made no choice. Each subject took part in all conditions of this study.

2.4. Procedure

The dogs were allowed off lead in the laboratory, with the owner and experimenter present to allow the dog to feel comfortable in the room. To ensure the dogs were not under stress, they were required to willingly approach the experimenter and eat from their hand. If the dogs apparently did not want to do this, then they would not take part in the experiment (following the protocol in Ref. [37]). However, no subjects were excluded on this basis. Once the dog met the inclusion criteria, the study began. Both the owner and the experimenter served as cue-givers, in counterbalanced order across the dogs. The cue-giver (either owner or experimenter) guided the dog to the starting position, which was marked on the floor of the laboratory. The human not acting as the cue-giver stood silently in a central position behind the dog and held the dog in place until the cue-giver arrived in the correct position. Commands such as ”sit” and ”stay” were used to keep the dog in the starting position. Once the dog and cue-giver were in their starting positions, ostensive cues such as calling the dogs’ name to attract their attention were used. The cue-giver pointed to one of two items (see Figure 1, Figure 2 and Figure 3); this was either an item that was familiar to the dog, or an item that was unfamiliar.
Each dog received eight trials, four with the owner as the cue giver and four with the experimenter as the cue-giver, so that each cue-giver pointed to each object once. The order in which the cues were given (left or right) were counterbalanced to reduce a response pattern bias forming, as well as the order in which the familiar and non-familiar cue giver gave the cue. All subjects received the same type of cue, the cue-giver used an ipsilateral static point, whereby the pointing hand on the same side as the intended target was in place before the participant viewed the cue and remained so until the participant chose one of the items. The cue giver kept their head directed centrally, and body position facing forward, and the cue-giver looked at the dog whilst giving the cue, to indicate the cue is meant for the dog.
If the dog did not make a choice or approached one of the two items after 1 min this was classed as a no-choice trial (as seen in similar protocols: [29,37]). A successful trial was when the dog chose the correct referent by touching it, retrieving it, or coming within 10 cm of it. An unsuccessful trial was when the subjects approached the incorrect item. During the experiment, our plan was to exclude a dog if they exhibited urination, defecation, destructive behaviour, or excessive vocalisation (these are signs of anxiety), however, no dog was excluded on these grounds.

2.5. Spontaneous Variations on the Form of the Pointing Cue

During the initial coding, we noticed that some owners/trainers did not point with their index fingers, as had been displayed by the experimenter, and this non-canonical pointing seemed to be more frequent in the trained working dog group; therefore we added a code for conformity with an index-finger point to determine the distribution of these points among the groups.

2.6. Statistical Analyses

Tests of normality showed the performance data were not normally distributed and so we elected to test our statistical hypotheses with nonparametric statistics. Kruskal–Wallis H tests were used to compare the three groups on the dependent variable and Mann–Whitney U tests were used for the pairwise comparisons between groups. Spearman’s rho was used to assess the relationship between overall performance and the results of the survey, and Cochran Q tests were used to measure the frequency of no-choice trials, and Wilcoxon signed ranks tests were used to compare two related samples.

2.7. Interobserver Reliability

All eight trials from eight subjects were randomly selected from the corpus and independently coded for performance by a naïve coder, who was blind to the hypotheses under test; this represents ~15% of the corpus. Reliability for dogs’ performances was Cohen’s kappa = 0.74, indicating excellent inter-observer reliability.

2.8. Ethical Note

All subjects were volunteered by their owners and were recruited through posts on social media. The dogs remained with their owners throughout this study, and the owners gave written consent for their dog to partake and to be filmed prior to this study; they were also reminded that should they wish to they could withdraw their participation at any time and leave. This study was conducted under ethical approval from the Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body (AWERB) of the University of Sussex.

2.9. Risk Assessment

This study took place during the COVID-19 pandemic, therefore we needed to ensure the safety of the participants during testing. All participants wore a mask and remained a social distance of two metres when possible. The laboratory was sanitised between each participant and spare gloves, masks, and antibacterial hand gel were available. The experimenter was tested regularly for COVID-19 and all participants were invited to watch a video detailing the university’s COVID procedure.

3. Results

3.1. Sex and Age Distributions

A chi-squared analysis showed that sex ratio was not significantly different across the three groups, X2 (2, n = 54) = 1.45, p = 0.48. A Kruskal–Wallis test showed that the three groups of dogs were not significantly different in median age (H = 2.04, df = 2, p = 0.36). The median ages for the three groups were 36.0 months (IQR = 83) for the “Actively working”, 50.5 months (IQR = 84) for “Non-working dogs”, and 24.0 months (IQR = 49) for the “Working breeds”. These results show us that there were no obvious confounds with sex or age between the three groups.

3.2. Order and Side Effects

We did not find an effect of cue-giver order on overall performance, (Mann–Whitney U = 304.50, df = 1 p = 0.30); thus, we found no evidence that whether the experimenter or owner first served as cue-giver had any influence on results. There were six potential side orders in which the cue-givers could point, and we found no effect of the order of side of pointing (Kruskal–Wallis H = 5.09, df = 5, p = 0.41); this means that we found no evidence of side biases or trial order effects on performance. There were also six potential target orders in which the cue-givers could point, we found no effect of the order to which the familiar and non-familiar toys were pointed at (Kruskal–Wallis H = 2.45, df = 5, p = 0.78; see Table 2 for full list of order possibilities). These results show us that the trial order and the side pointed to did not seem to influence overall performance.

3.3. Age, Sex, and Neuter Status

All three groups of dogs were just as likely to be neutered, X2 (2, n = 54) = 2.50, p = 0.29. We found no relationship between age and performance (Spearman’s rs = 0.05, n = 54, p = 0.71). In addition, we found no relationship between dogs’ sex and performance (Mann–Whitney U = 342.50, df = 2, p = 0.70). Therefore, we do not further consider these variables.

3.4. Performance across Groups

There was a significant difference in overall performance between the three groups of dogs (Kruskal–Wallis H = 8.90, df = 2, p < 0.01). To look at this in closer detail for each group, we found that “Actively Working” dogs performed better than “Non-working breeds” (Mann–Whitney U = 64.50, df = 2, p < 0.001). “Working breeds” performed better than “Non-working breeds” (Mann–Whitney U = 94.50, df = 2, p = 0.05), and we found no significant difference in performance between “Actively working” dogs and “Working breeds” (Mann–Whitney U = 150.00, df = 2, p = 0.36). Figure 4 shows the overall performance for each group. These results show us that “Actively working” and “Working breeds” performed better overall than “Non-working breeds”.

3.5. Survey Results

We then moved on to analyse the survey questions which were given to the owners prior to the experiment. We found no correlation between the degree to which the owners say their dogs get on with other dogs and overall performance (Spearman’s rs = -0.10, n = 54, p = 0.46). There was no significant correlation between the degree to which owners say their dogs play with toys and overall performance (Spearman’s rs = 0.14, n = 54, p = 0.31). We did, however, find a significant positive correlation between the degree to which owners say their dogs are well trained with overall performance (Spearman’s rs = 0.38, n = 54, p < 0.001), as shown in Figure 5. In addition, we found a significant positive correlation between the degree to which the dogs follow commands and overall performance (Spearman’s rs = 0.39, n = 54, p < 0.001), as shown in Figure 6. Finally, we found a significant positive correlation between the degree to which owners say their dogs are food motivated between and the degree to which the dogs follow commands and overall performance (Spearman’s rs = 0.30, n = 54, p < 0.05), as shown in Figure 7.

3.6. Familiarity of Cue-Giver

We found that the familiarity of the cue-giver had a significant effect on overall performance (Wilcoxon Z = −3.35, p < 0.001). When we looked at this between groups, we found that familiarity of the cue giver had a significant effect on performance for “Actively working” dogs (Wilcoxon Z = −1.97, df = 1, p < 0.05) and “Working breeds” (Z = −2.02, df = 1, p < 0.05) but not “Non-working breeds” (Z = −1.81, df = 1, p = 0.07). This means that both “Actively working” and “Working breeds” responded significantly better to a familiar cue-giver than an unfamiliar one, this is shown in Figure 8.

3.7. Familiarity of Toy

We found that the familiarity of the toy did not have a significant effect on overall performance (Wilcoxon Z = −0.73, df = 1 p = 0.46). Familiarity of the toy also had no significant effect on overall performance for “Actively working” dogs (Wilcoxon Z = −0.48, df = 1, p = 0.63), “Working breeds” (Wilcoxon Z = −1.10, df = 1, p = 0.27) and “Non-working breeds” (Wilcoxon Z = −0.73, df = 1, p = 0.46). Thus, we found no evidence of any influence of the familiarity of the toy on performance for any group.

3.8. No-Choice Trials

We categorised each of the eight trials for every dog as either correct choice or not, and then we looked to see whether there was any systematic change in this measure over the course of the eight trials. We found that overall performance did not change over time (Cochran’s Q (7) = 6.85, p = 0.45). However, when we categorised each of the eight trials for every dog as either a choice was made (whether correct or not) or no choice was made, we found that the groups differed significantly in the number of no-choice trials (Kruskal–Wallis H = 13.75, df = 2, p < 0.001). To look at this in more detail we compared “Actively working” dogs to “Non-working breeds” and found that “Non-working breeds” had significantly more no choice responses (Mann–Whitney U = 57.00, df = 2, p < 0.001). We then compared “Non-working breeds” to “Working breeds” and again found that “Non-working breeds” had significantly more no choice responses Mann–Whitney U = 92.50, df = 2, p < 0.05). We found no significant difference between “Actively working” and “Working breeds” (Mann–Whitney U = 147.50, df = 2, p = 0.34). This is shown in Figure 9.
We then looked at whether no-choice trials were changing over time for each group, we did this by categorising each of the eight trials for every dog in each group as either a choice was made (whether correct or not) or no choice was made, and then we looked to see whether there was any systematic change in this measure over the course of the eight trials. We found no significant effect for “Actively working” dogs (Cochran’s Q (7) = 11.42, p = 0.12). This means the number of no-choice trials did not increase over time for this group, as there were only 3 no-choice trials out of a possible 152. We found no significant effect for “Non-working breeds” (Cochran’s Q (7) = 12.42, p = 0.08). This means the number of no-choice trials also did not increase over time; however, this group had the highest number of no choice responses with 38 out of a possible 152. Finally, we found no significant effect for “Working breeds” (Cochran’s Q (7) = 6.21, p = 0.52). This means the number of no-choice trials did not significantly increase over time, and this group had 19 no-choice trials out of a possible 152. These results show that the number of no-choice trials remained broadly steady throughout this study for each group.

4. Discussion

Our results show that actively working dogs and working breeds perform better than non-working breeds on the object-choice task. These findings are consistent with those of McKinley and Sambrook [30] and Wobber and her colleagues [31], who found that actively working dogs perform better than pet dogs. These findings are also consistent with those of Udell and her colleagues [32], who found that traditional working breeds perform better in the object-choice task. Similarly to McKinley and Sambrook [30], we compared working breed dogs kept as pets to non-working breed dogs kept as pets and found that working breeds perform better; however, unlike McKinley and Sambrook, we did not find a statistically significant difference between actively working dogs and working breeds kept as pets. Therefore, our findings are consistent with the idea of a breeding-related advantage for working dogs, although it should be acknowledged that we did not equate the pet working dogs and the pet dogs from working dog breeds on the quality or quantity of their pre-experimental interactions with their owners. Because there was no statistical difference in performance between the “Actively working” and “Working breed” groups, and both of these groups outperformed the pet dogs from non-working dog breeds, we conclude that breeding may play a role in performance in this task environment, given our sampling sizes and sampling environment. However, as emphasised by an anonymous reviewer, social experiences with humans are also clearly relevant to performance in understanding human communicative cues (e.g., [28,37,38]).
We also found that the familiarity of the cue giver has a significant effect on overall performance, such that actively working and working breed dogs respond better to cues given by their owners than by a stranger. However, no such effect was found in non-working breeds kept as pets. These findings are only partially congruent with those of Cunningham and Ramos [39], who reported that all the dogs they tested preferred their owners over a stranger; however, the breed sampling in their study differed from ours, as all dogs in their pet and highly trained categories were of working breeds, with no non-working breeds used.
The present results also indicate that previous studies conducted on dogs without their owners present may not have reflected the dogs’ true abilities to follow human cues, as suggested by Burani, Barnard, Wells, Pelosi, and Valsecchi [40]. Our results also support the findings of previous studies that have found that dogs largely react better to their owners than a stranger [17,41]. However, our results are not consistent with those of Scandurra et al. [42] and Lazarowski et al. [43], who found that actively working dogs responded just as well to a stranger as they did to their owners. In fact, we found the opposite results: in our study, non-working breed dogs kept as pets responded to strangers and owners equally. The difference in findings in this case may lie in the types of actively working dogs tested. Scandurra et al. [42] exclusively used water rescue dogs and Lazarowski and her colleagues [43] only used detection dogs in their studies; these are both types of jobs which require the dog to act on their own and not rely on a human to guide them; whereas, in our study, we used a wide range of dogs which included detection and search and rescue dogs, but also included assistance and therapy dogs who rely much more on owner interaction for their working role. We also wanted to find out whether the familiarity of the object being pointed to would affect the performance of the dogs. However, we found no significant effect of referent familiarity for all three groups on overall performance.
We then moved on to analyse the results of the survey given to the owners prior to the experiment. We found no significant correlation between the degree to which owners said their dogs get on with other dogs or the frequency the dogs play with toys and overall performance. However, we did find significant positive correlations between the degree to which owners say their dogs are well-trained and performance in this object-choice task, and we found significant positive correlations between the degree to which owners say their dogs follow commands and performance in this object-choice task. This suggests that dogs who are perceived by their owners to be well-trained do perform better on the object-choice task as these dogs may have received more training and could be perceiving the point as a command to be followed. Interestingly, we found a positive correlation between the degree to which dogs were food motivated and overall performance; because we did not use food rewards in this study, this was a surprising result. However, because most dogs are trained with food rewards, those who are more food motivated may have expected a treat once they followed the point.
The survey was measured on a 10-point scale for most of the questions and an ordinal scale for others (Yes/Sometimes/Usually/No). Because the owners were answering the survey to characterise their dogs, it could have been subjected to a variety of response biases. For example, the question “How well trained would you say your dogs is?” could be interpreted in a number of different ways pertaining to the dog’s general demeanor. Still, several items did positively correlate with the dogs’ actual performance, suggesting that owners do have a fairly good grasp of their dogs’ performance potential.
We then examined the no-choice trials in terms of frequency and changes over time. We compared the overall performance for each group and found they did differ significantly, by group classification, in the number of no-choice trials. We found that “Non-working breeds” had significantly more no-choice trials than “Actively working” and “Working breeds”, and that “Actively working” and “Working breeds” did not differ from each other significantly in the number of no-choice trials. This shows that “Actively working” and “Working breeds” are more motivated to participate in the point-following task. When we look at the exact number of no-choice trials, we can see that “Actively working” dogs nearly always made a choice, with only three no-choice trials out of 152 total trials. Therefore, we can conclude again that “Actively working” and “Working breeds” may benefit from a genetic predisposition to not only perform better in the object-choice task but to be more motivated than non-working breeds.
We wanted to see if no-choice trials were increasing over time, to indicate whether the dogs were either losing interest over time (i.e., if these no-choice trials were increasing) or, alternatively, if the dogs were learning what to do (i.e., if the no-choice trials decreased over time). However, we found no significant effect of the passage of time; all three groups remained motivated at seemingly constant levels throughout the experiment.
This study has a number of limitations. First, this study took place during the COVID-19 pandemic, which means that all participants and the experimenter were required to wear masks for the duration on the study. Because the masks cover the lower half of the face this may have been confusing to the dogs who are not used to their owners interacting with them in this way. However, because this study took place in the United Kingdom, in May and June of 2021, it seems likely that the dogs will have had some interaction with people wearing masks, previously. However, it would be interesting to complete this study again with no masks used, when it is safe to do so, to see if the same effects are found. Second, we are grateful to an anonymous reviewer for pointing out that dogs of non-working breeds tend to be smaller than working dog breeds, and therefore our specific findings of poorer performance by non-working dog breeds is potentially confounded with body size, which we did not measure—the reviewer pointed us towards McGreevy et al. [44] for further information about the effects of size on performance in dogs. Third, we thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing out that dogs’ individual preferences for the toys were not assessed—theoretically, this means that, at the group level, rather than failing to demonstrate that dogs displayed no apparent bias for or against the toys on the basis of their familiarity, it could be the case that relatively equal numbers of dogs displayed preferences for the familiar and unfamiliar toys, respectively, with results cancelled out at the group level. Finally, our sample size is relatively modest, which suggests that one possible explanation for some of our findings that diverge from other similar studies might be attributable to sampling error.

5. Conclusions and Future Research

Therefore, based on the findings of our study, we conclude that breed differences may play a role in dogs’ responses to communicative cues. The findings of this study do not align completely with that of the Domestication Hypothesis, insofar as a strong version of that model predicts no breed differences. Our findings suggest that a dog’s ability to perform well in the object-choice task may be influenced more by the genetics of the breed than the genetics of the species, and that this is then likely amplified by life experience and training, as suggested by Sommese, Nováková, Šebková, and Bartoš [45]) in a different experimental context (impossible task), and, in object-choice tasks by many others (e.g., [28,37,42]).
In future, these findings could be built on further with longitudinal designs, by testing non-working breeds without any training, and then training them specifically on the task to see if performance improves. This could possibly be used to see if non-working breeds could be trained for working roles. It would also be interesting to use shelter dogs, of working and non-working breeds to see whether having a stable home environment affects a dog’s ability to be trained for working roles, because, currently, working roles are performed by mainly pedigree breed dogs who are bred specifically for their individual role. If studies can find that non-working breed dogs can be trained to perform these roles just as well, it could possibly alleviate the number of dogs in shelters without homes.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, J.M.M. and D.A.L.; methodology, D.A.L. and J.M.M.; validation, J.M.M. and D.A.L.; formal analysis, J.M.M. and D.A.L.; investigation, J.M.M.; resources, J.M.M. and D.A.L.; data curation, D.A.L.; writing—original draft preparation, J.M.M.; writing—review and editing, D.A.L.; visualisation, J.M.M. and D.A.L.; supervision, D.A.L.; project administration, D.A.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the School of Life Sciences, University of Sussex, master’s project support bursary to J.M.M.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Animal Welfare and Review Body of the University of Sussex (ARG-25, 22 April 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from the owners of all dogs involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

Data contained within this article.

Acknowledgments

We thank Lucy Ratcliffe for interobserver reliability coding. We thank Daniel Hyndman and Martha Casey for technical support. We are extremely grateful to the dogs and their owners for their participation in this study. This research was conducted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for Master of Research in Animal Behaviour, School of Life Sciences, University of Sussex. We are indebted to five anonymous reviewers who reviewed earlier versions of this manuscript; the manuscript has been significantly improved thanks to their generous scholarship and attention to detail.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

  • Survey
  • Owner name:
  • Dog name:
  • Breed:
  • Age:
  • Sex:
  • Neutered? (Y/N)
  • Working dog? If yes, as what?
  • Time spent working (Years):
  • Adopted? If yes, at what age?
  • Time spent at shelter (if known) in years:
  • How well trained would you say your dogs is? Scale 1 (not at all)–10 (extremely well)
  • How well does your dog follow commands? Scale 1 (not at all)–10 (extremely well)
  • Is your dog highly motivated by food? Scale 1 (not at all)–10 (extremely)
  • Does your dog get along with other dogs? Yes/Usually/Sometimes/No
  • Does your dog play with toys regularly? Yes/Usually/Sometimes/No

References

  1. Bergström, A.; Frantz, L.; Schmid, R.; Ersmark, E.; Lebrasseur, O.; Girdland-Flink, L.; Lin, A.T.; Storå, J.; Sjögren, K.-G.; Anthony, D.; et al. Origins and genetic legacy of prehistoric dogs. Science 2020, 370, 557–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Zeder, M.A. Domestication and early agriculture in the Mediterranean Basin: Origins, diffusion, and impact. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 11597–11604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bredford, E. Number of Dogs and Cats Kept as Pets Worldwide in 2018. Statista. 2020. Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/1044386/dog-and-cat-pet-population-worldwide/ (accessed on 31 August 2021).
  4. Hare, B.; Tomasello, M. Human-like social skills in dogs? TRENDS Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 439–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Dugatkin, L.A. The silver fox domestication experiment. Resonance 2020, 25, 987–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Miklósi, Á. Lovely tame foxes that nobody domesticated. Curr. Biol. 2017, 27, R626–R627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Hare, B.; Plyusnina, I.; Ignacio, N.; Schepina, O.; Stepika, A.; Wrangham, R.; Trut, L. Social cognitive evolution in captive foxes is a correlated by-product of experimental domestication. Curr. Biol. 2005, 15, 226–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Riedel, J.; Schumann, K.; Kaminski, J.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. The early ontogeny of human–dog communication. Anim. Behav. 2008, 75, 1003–1014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Udell, M.A.R.; Dorey, N.R.; Wynne, C.D.L. Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues. Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 1767–1773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ujfalussy, D.J.; Virányi, Z.; Gácsi, M.; Faragó, T.; Pogány, Á.; Bereczky, B.M.; Miklósi, Á.; Kubinyi, E. Comparing the tractability of young hand-raised wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 14678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Lazzaroni, M.; Range, F.; Backes, J.; Portele, K.; Scheck, K.; Marshall-Pescini, S. The effect of domestication and experience on the social interaction of dogs and wolves with a human companion. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Lampe, M.; Bräuer, J.; Kaminski, J.; Virányi, Z. The effects of domestication and ontogeny on cognition in dogs and wolves. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 11690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Range, F.; Brucks, D.; Virányi, Z. Dogs wait longer for better rewards than wolves in a delay of gratification task: But why? Anim. Cogn. 2020, 23, 443–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Anderson, J.; Sallaberry, P.; Barbier, H. Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by capuchin monkey. Anim. Behav. 1995, 49, 201–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Miklósi, Á.; Polgárdi, R.; Topál, J.; Csányi, V. Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Anim. Cogn. 1998, 1, 113–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Dwyer, C.; Cole, M. Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) tend to follow repeated deceptive human cues even when food is visible. Learn. Behav. 2018, 46, 442–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Elgier, A.M.; Jakovcevic, A.; Barrera, G.; Mustaca, A.E.; Bentosela, M. Communication between domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans: Dogs are good learners. Behav. Process. 2009, 81, 402–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Marshall-Pescini, S.; Passalacqua, C.; Petrazzini, M.; Valsecchi, P.; Prato-Previde, E. Do dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) make counterproductive choices because they are sensitive to human ostensive cues? PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e35437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Plourde, V.; Fiset, S. Pointing gestures modulate domestic dogs’ search behaviour for hidden objects in a spatial rotation problem. Learn. Motiv. 2013, 44, 282–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Chijiiwa, H.; Takagi, S.; Arahori, M.; Hori, Y.; Saito, A.; Kuroshima, H.; Fujita, K. Dogs and cats prioritize human action: Choosing a now-empty instead of a still-baited container. Anim. Cogn. 2021, 24, 16–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Takaoka, A.; Maeda, T.; Hori, Y.; Fujita, K. Do dogs follow behavioral cues from an unreliable human? Anim. Cogn. 2015, 18, 475–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Bräuer, J.; Kaminski, J.; Riedel, J.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Making inferences about the location of hidden food: Social dog, causal ape. J. Comp. Psychol. 2006, 120, 38–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Kirchhofer, K.; Zimmermann, F.; Kaminski, J.; Tomasello, M. Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointing. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e30913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Clark, H.; Elsherif, M.; Leavens, D.A. Ontogeny vs. phylogeny in Primate/Canid comparisons: A meta-analysis of the object choice task. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2019, 105, 178–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Clark, H.; Leavens, D.A. Testing dogs in ape-like conditions: The effect of a barrier on dogs’ performance on the object choice task. Anim. Cogn. 2019, 22, 1063–1072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Clark, H.; Leavens, D.A. The performance of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) on two versions of the Object Choice Task. Anim. Cogn. 2021, 24, 1087–1098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Hopkins, W.D.; Russell, J.L.; McIntyre, J.; Leavens, D.A. Are chimpanzees really so poor at understanding imperative pointing? Some new data and an alternative view of canine and ape social cognition. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e79338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Udell, M.A.R.; Dorey, N.R.; Wynne, C.D.L. The performance of stray dogs (Canis familiaris) living in a shelter on human-guided object-choice tasks. Anim. Behav. 2010, 79, 717–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lazarowski, L.; Dorman, D.C. A comparison of pet and purpose-bred research dog (Canis familiaris) performance on human-guided object-choice tasks. Behav. Process. 2015, 110, 60–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. McKinley, J.; Sambrook, T. Use of human-given cues by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and horses (Equus caballus). Anim. Cogn. 2000, 3, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Wobber, V.; Hare, B.; Koler-Matznick, J.; Wrangham, R.; Tomasello, M. Breed differences in domestic dogs’ (Canis familiaris) comprehension of human communicative signals. Interact. Stud. 2009, 10, 206–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Udell, M.A.R.; Ewald, M.; Dorey, N.R.; Wynne, C.D.L. Exploring breed differences in dogs (Canis familiaris): Does exaggeration or inhibition of predatory response predict performance on human-guided tasks? Anim. Behav. 2014, 89, 99–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Feuerbacher, E.N.; Wynne, C.D.L. Shut up and pet me! Domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer petting to vocal praise in concurrent and single-alternative choice procedures. Behav. Process. 2015, 110, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Hegedüs, D.; Bálint, A.; Miklósi, Á.; Pongrácz, P. Owners fail to influence the choices of dogs in a two-choice, visual pointing task. Behaviour 2013, 150, 427–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kraus, C.; Van Waveren, C.; Huebner, F. Distractible dogs, constant cats? A test of the distraction hypothesis in two domestic species. Anim. Behav. 2014, 93, 173–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. American Kennel Club. Dog Breeds—Types of Dogs. 2021. Available online: https://www.akc.org/dog-breeds/ (accessed on 30 June 2021).
  37. Udell, M.A.R.; Dorey, N.R.; Wynne, C.D.L. What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs’ sensitivity to human actions. Biol. Rev. 2010, 85, 327–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. D’Aniello, B.; Alterisio, A.; Scandurra, A.; Petremolo, E.; Iommelli, M.R.; Aria, M. What’s the point? Golden and Labrador retrievers living in kennels do not understand human pointing gestures. Anim. Cogn. 2017, 20, 777–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Cunningham, C.L.; Ramos, M.F. Effect of training and familiarity on responsiveness to human cues in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 805–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Burani, C.; Barnard, S.; Wells, D.; Pelosi, A.; Valsecchi, P. Using judgment bias test in pet and shelter dogs (Canis familiaris): Methodological and statistical caveats. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0241344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Cook, A.; Arter, J.; Jacobs, L.F. My owner, right or wrong: The effect of familiarity on the domestic dog’s behaviour in a food-choice task. Anim. Cogn. 2014, 17, 461–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Scandurra, A.; Alterisio, A.; Marinelli, L.; Mongillo, P.; Seminc, G.R.; D’Anielloa, B. Effectiveness of verbal and gestural signals and familiarity with signal-senders on the performance of working dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 191, 78–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Lazarowski, L.; Thompkins, A.; Krichbaum, S.; Waggoner, P.; Deshpande, G.; Katz, J.S. Comparing pet and detection dogs (Canis familiaris) on two aspects of social cognition. Learn. Behav. 2020, 48, 432–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. McGreevy, P.D.; Georgevsky, D.; Carrasco, J.; Valenzuela, M.; Duffy, D.L.; Serpell, J.A. Dog behavior co-varies with height, bodyweight and skull shape. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e80529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Sommese, A.; Nováková, K.; Šebková, N.F.; Bartoš, L. A wolfdog point of view on the impossible task paradigm. Anim. Cogn. 2019, 22, 1073–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Schematic of the laboratory set up, and an example of a point towards a familiar item. Drawing not to scale, distances are approximate.
Figure 1. Schematic of the laboratory set up, and an example of a point towards a familiar item. Drawing not to scale, distances are approximate.
Animals 14 00573 g001
Figure 2. Example of trial with an unfamiliar cue-giver to unfamiliar toy.
Figure 2. Example of trial with an unfamiliar cue-giver to unfamiliar toy.
Animals 14 00573 g002
Figure 3. Example of trial with a familiar cue-giver to familiar toy.
Figure 3. Example of trial with a familiar cue-giver to familiar toy.
Animals 14 00573 g003
Figure 4. A boxplot showing the median performances and interquartile ranges (IQR) for each group. * = p < 0.05. ns = not significant.
Figure 4. A boxplot showing the median performances and interquartile ranges (IQR) for each group. * = p < 0.05. ns = not significant.
Animals 14 00573 g004
Figure 5. A scatterplot showing a positive correlation between the degree to which owners say their dogs are well trained and overall performance.
Figure 5. A scatterplot showing a positive correlation between the degree to which owners say their dogs are well trained and overall performance.
Animals 14 00573 g005
Figure 6. A scatterplot showing a positive correlation between the degree to which owners say their dogs follow commands and overall performance.
Figure 6. A scatterplot showing a positive correlation between the degree to which owners say their dogs follow commands and overall performance.
Animals 14 00573 g006
Figure 7. A scatterplot showing a positive correlation between the degree to which owners say their dogs are food motivated and overall performance.
Figure 7. A scatterplot showing a positive correlation between the degree to which owners say their dogs are food motivated and overall performance.
Animals 14 00573 g007
Figure 8. A bar chart showing mean performance for “Actively working”, “Working breeds”, and “Non-working breeds” with familiar and unfamiliar cue-givers. * = p < 0.05. ns = not significant.
Figure 8. A bar chart showing mean performance for “Actively working”, “Working breeds”, and “Non-working breeds” with familiar and unfamiliar cue-givers. * = p < 0.05. ns = not significant.
Animals 14 00573 g008
Figure 9. A histogram showing the percentages of making a choice vs. not making a choice for “Actively working”, “Non-working breeds”, and “Working breeds”.
Figure 9. A histogram showing the percentages of making a choice vs. not making a choice for “Actively working”, “Non-working breeds”, and “Working breeds”.
Animals 14 00573 g009
Table 2. List of all possibilities for cue-giver, side of baiting, and point target order.
Table 2. List of all possibilities for cue-giver, side of baiting, and point target order.
Cue-Giver OrderSide OrderTarget Order
ExperimenterLLRRFFUU
OwnerLRLRFUFU
LRRLFUUF
RRLLUUFF
RLRLUFUF
RLLRUFFU
Note. F = Familiar item, U = Unfamiliar item, L = Left, R = Right.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

McCartney, J.M.; Leavens, D.A. The Role of Life History and Familiarity in Performance of Working and Non-Working Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) in a Point-Following Task. Animals 2024, 14, 573. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14040573

AMA Style

McCartney JM, Leavens DA. The Role of Life History and Familiarity in Performance of Working and Non-Working Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) in a Point-Following Task. Animals. 2024; 14(4):573. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14040573

Chicago/Turabian Style

McCartney, Josephine M., and David A. Leavens. 2024. "The Role of Life History and Familiarity in Performance of Working and Non-Working Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) in a Point-Following Task" Animals 14, no. 4: 573. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14040573

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop