Next Article in Journal
Interaction between Sovereign Quanto Credit Default Swap Spreads and Currency Options
Next Article in Special Issue
The Effects of Geopolitical Risk on Foreign Direct Investment in a Transition Economy: Evidence from Vietnam
Previous Article in Journal
Family Ownership, Corporate Governance Quality and Tax Avoidance: Evidence from an Emerging Market—The Case of Jordan
Previous Article in Special Issue
Impact of Financial Factors on the Economic Cycle Dynamics in Selected European Countries
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Consideration of Future Consequences, Prior Gain or Loss, Personal Risk Profile, and Justification Affect Risk–Payoff Preferences

Department of Accounting, University of Northern Iowa, Cedar Falls, IA 50614-0127, USA
J. Risk Financial Manag. 2024, 17(2), 83; https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm17020083
Submission received: 3 November 2023 / Revised: 28 December 2023 / Accepted: 4 January 2024 / Published: 18 February 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Financial Markets, Financial Volatility and Beyond (Volume III))

Abstract

:
This study examines (1) how risk–payoff preference can be affected by differences in consideration of future consequences (CFC), prior gain/loss, and personal risk profile, and (2) whether one’s risk–payoff preference may vary with justification prompts. Using an experimental design with 366 undergraduate business students, participants are tasked to make risk–payoff choices in two scenarios, with the combined risk–payoff outcomes serving as the dependent variable. In addition, participants are assessed on their personal risk profiles and also complete the 14-item CFC scale to gauge the propensity to take into account future consequences of their behaviors. Findings show that one who scores low (high) in CFC will prefer lower (higher) risk and payoff. Further, for an individual who scores high in CFC and has a prior gain (loss), he/she will be more inclined to prefer lower (higher) risk and payoff, though this effect is moderated by one’s risk profile. Finally, justification prompts help to reduce one’s propensity toward high risk–payoff, irrespective of prior gain/loss and risk profile considerations. With regard to consumers’ financial choices, particularly in a volatile economic environment, the findings here indicate that prompting for strategic justifications before making decisions can help lower one’s overall propensity toward high risk–payoff choices.

1. Introduction

In this study, consideration of future consequences (CFC) is defined as the importance a person assigns to the immediate versus delayed consequences of their actions (Strathman et al. 1994). In predicting how individual differences in CFC may impact one’s financial decision-making, Joireman et al. (2005) showed that individuals high in CFC are more inclined to use a hypothetical windfall in a fiscally responsible fashion, like exhibiting greater promptness in paying down one’s credit card debt. Howlett et al. (2008) similarly found that consumers who display a higher propensity to consider the future consequences of their behaviors (i.e., higher CFC) will be more likely to make decisions and evaluations that will maximize their future financial well-being. In matters concerning personal finance, one with a higher level of CFC reports a higher likelihood of contributing to a 401 K plan, though this is moderated by one’s self-regulatory state and the level of financial knowledge.
Prior literature concerning aspiration level has also shown that investment choices are largely based on loss aversion, with individuals typically being risk-averse over gains, but risk-seeking over losses (see Payne et al. 1980, 1981). The economic theory of endowment effect posits an asymmetry between gains and losses, whereby loss is considered more aversive than gain of the same size (Kahneman and Tversky 1979). With outcome dispersion or variance often used as a proxy for risk, a greater (lower) risk can portend a lower (higher) probability of an event occurring (see Libby and Fishburn 1977; Payne 2005). Prior research has shown that using lower- (versus higher-) level construal to represent information about near (versus distant) events in the future increases one’s preference for a higher (lower) probability of a lower (higher) expected payoff (Joireman and Balliet 2004; Sagristano et al. 2002).
Finally, Tetlock (1983, 1985) has found that prompting a more effortful process to justify one’s decision choices could attenuate the tendency of making fundamental attribution errors. Applied to one’s risk–payoff preference and conditional on one’s CFC, prior gain/loss, and risk profile considerations, this study hereforth examines if strategic justification prompts could provide an effective prescription toward reducing one’s propensity for making high-risk choices.
Using an experimental design with 366 undergraduate business students, participants are tasked to make risk–payoff choices in two scenarios, with the combined risk–payoff outcomes serving as the dependent variable. In addition, participants are assessed on their personal risk profiles and also complete the 14-item CFC scale to gauge the propensity to take into account future consequences of their behaviors. The findings of this study show that one who scores low (high) in CFC will prefer a lower (higher) risk and payoff. For an individual who scores high in CFC and has a prior gain (loss), he/she will be more inclined to prefer a lower (higher) risk and payoff. In examining the moderation effect arising from one’s risk profile, results show that one with high CFC and a prior loss will demonstrate a preference for higher risk regardless of their personal risk profile. On the other hand, one with high CFC and a prior gain will choose the less risky approach if they were risk avoiders, versus the more risky approach if they were risk seekers. Finally, justification prompts help to reduce one’s propensity toward high risk and payoff, irrespective of prior gain/loss and risk profile considerations.
Prior studies in financial statement risk assessment have provided theoretical insights by using both decision theory and behavioral variables in explaining investors’ decision judgments, as well as investors’ biases toward the different labeling and disclosures used in financial instruments (e.g., Koonce et al. 2005a, 2005b). This experimental study makes at least three incremental contributions to risk–payoff decision research. First, from a methodological standpoint, prior research in the areas of risk tolerance and risk-taking have predominantly used archival data from surveys, illuminating the importance of household demographics, individuals’ life domains, and portfolio structures as well as a composite of subjective and objective financial knowledge in determining one’s financial risk tolerance level or risk-taking propensity (see Grable and Rabbani 2014; Heo et al. 2021; Irandoust 2017; Noman et al. 2023; Rabbani et al. 2021; Schooley and Worden 2016). Experimental studies examining this research stream have also primarily looked at various individual effects, such as the effects of inducing differing time pressures or stressors on risk-taking preferences/behaviors (see, e.g., Cahlikova and Cingl 2017; Kirchler et al. 2017; Kocher et al. 2013). This study uses an experimental setting to further examine a multitude of important constructs on respondents’ risk preference decisions.
Second, it extends the findings of earlier studies by introducing the important concept of CFC. Many of our life decisions involve a temporal element that hinges on our ability to look past the immediate and into the future. Contrary to the piecemeal approaches of prior studies in studying various individual concepts, this study consolidates and examines the dynamics of how one’s CFC, prior gain/loss, and risk profile may interact with justification requirement to affect risk–payoff judgments. Thus, incorporating CFC within the broader financial decision-making framework can serve as a springboard for more future research involving a temporal dimension. Third, on a practical level, findings of this study indicate that, notwithstanding one’s CFC, prior gain/loss, and risk profile, prompting for crucial justifications before making decisions can effectively be used to evoke a more cognitively vigilant mental processing mode, thereby helping to mitigate one’s propensity toward making highly divergent, and often risky, choices. Overall, one could take heed of the findings from this study and gain a better understanding of the mechanism shaping one’s decision-making processes. From a personal financial planning perspective, this can hopefully help steer investors toward making more reasoned financial choices, with lower overall risks, particularly in a volatile economic environment when investors are more prone to make emotion-driven decisions.
The remainder of this paper is organized into five sections. The next section provides the theoretical background to the study. This is followed by the hypotheses development, research design, analyses, limitations and future research, and finally, the conclusion and implications.

2. Theoretical Background

2.1. Consideration of Future Consequences

Prior research has demonstrated that CFC’s immediate-versus-future orientation is a stable personality characteristic with a significant influence on one’s domain-specific behavioral outcomes (Murphy et al. 2020; Zimbardo and Boyd 1999). As defined by Strathman et al. (1994), individual differences in CFC reflect “the extent to which people consider the potential distant outcomes of their current behaviors and the extent to which they are influenced by these potential outcomes.” Past research has shown an inverse relationship between CFC and temporal discounting, in that one who scores lower in CFC will prefer a smaller but more certain outcome amount, while in comparison, a person who scores higher in CFC will prefer a larger (i.e., higher expected utility) but less certain payoff (Joireman and Balliet 2004; Sagristano et al. 2002).
Related, construal level theory (CLT) proposes that temporal distance changes one’s responses to future events by changing the way one mentally represents those events (Liberman and Trope 1998; Trope and Liberman 2000). One will use lower- (higher-)level construal to represent information about nearer (more distant) events in the future. As CLT proposes, in the context of making decision choices, if one thinks of the probability of (immediate) winning as subordinate to (distant) payoffs, then information regarding the probability of winning will be more prominent in construing near-future than distant-future decision choices, whereas information regarding payoffs will be more prominent in construing distant-future than near-future decision choices (Sagristano et al. 2002).
Inducing the near- versus distant-future concepts of CLT to predict how individual differences in CFC may impact one’s financial decision-making, Joireman et al. (2005) showed that individuals high in CFC are more inclined to use a hypothetical windfall in a fiscally responsible fashion, like exhibiting greater promptness in paying down one’s credit card debt. Howlett et al. (2008) further found that consumers who display a higher propensity to consider the future consequences of their behaviors (i.e., higher CFC) will be more likely to make decisions and evaluations that will maximize their future financial well-being. In matters concerning personal finance, one with a higher level of CFC reports a higher likelihood of contributing to a 401 K plan, though this is moderated by one’s self-regulatory state and the level of financial knowledge.

2.2. Prior Gain/Loss and Risk

The phenomenon of mere ownership bias effect is one whereby an object will be regarded more favorably by an owner than by a non-owner (Hoorens et al. 1990). From the mere ownership effect comes the endowment effect phenomenon, the latter defined as an increment in value that accrues to an object when it becomes part of a person’s endowment due to ownership (see Heider 1958). As put forth subsequently by Kahneman and Tversky (1979), in economic theory terms, the endowment effect is thought to occur because of the asymmetry between gains and losses, with a loss viewed as more aversive than a comparable gain (Kahneman et al. 1990). This endowment-driven, loss aversion impact on one’s preference decisions could change depending on whether one is in a position with a prior gain or loss. Such tendency follows from the tenets of individuals being risk-averse over gains but risk-seeking over losses (Kahneman and Tversky 1979). Intuitively, this could impact one’s preference, such that a prior gain (loss) position will evoke a lower (greater) propensity to take on risky investment decisions. When combined with prior findings related to CFC, one’s prior gain or loss should also be considered when tasked to make decision choices.
Several recent research studies have examined the topics of financial risk tolerance (FRT) and risk-taking behavior. Grable and Rabbani (2014) had earlier constructed a risk-tolerance portrait for individuals near retirement, to accurately characterize risk attitudes across seven domains. Focusing on the financial crisis event in 2008, Noman et al. (2023) examined a 2009–2018 data set and found that investors’ FRT was associated more with their subjective than their objective financial knowledge, though the relative influence of subjective knowledge increased as the survey periods progress further away from 2008. Schooley and Worden (2016) also used a 2007–2009 panel data set to examine changes in perceived and realized risk tolerance after the financial crisis. They found that households who perceived less risk tolerance (particularly those whose wealth had decreased) were more likely to have reduced their portfolio risk. Single, less educated, self-employed, or unemployed households were also most susceptible to portfolio reduction during this period, and thus may need financial advice to help prevent them from selling too quickly and potentially missing out on gains as the market recovers.
Separately, Rabbani et al. (2021) used a January 2018 to January 2019 data set to compare one’s FRT pre- to post- 3 October 2018 (i.e., the date of market high for the year 2018). They found a decrease in FRT levels post-October, with the most acute decrease evident with younger and less financially experienced respondents. There was no significant decrease in FRT post-October though, for those who have worked with financial advisors and counselors. Focusing from the COVID perspective by analyzing a data set from April 2019 to July 2020, Heo et al. (2021) found a general decrease in FRT as a result of the pandemic.
On one’s risk-taking propensity, Irandoust (2017) investigated, in the context of Sweden, the effects of factors that may influence the probabilities of one being risk-averse or risk-seeking. Results from questionnaire responses conducted in Sweden during May 2015 showed that one’s willingness to take financial risks depended on a multitude of portfolio and personal (i.e., gender, age, education, etc.) factors. Some previous studies have also examined the impact of inducing acute stressors or varying time pressures on risk-taking behaviors, finding inconsistent risk preferences toward gains and losses in response to differing conditions (Cahlikova and Cingl 2017; Kirchler et al. 2017; Kocher et al. 2013).
The normative mean-variance model as posited by Sharpe (1970) is centered on the value of an investment with its expected return (whereby more is preferred to less) and standard deviation (whereby less is preferred to more). In terms of one trying to reach an aspiration (i.e., success) level, the corresponding aim of loss aversion is often emphasized (Payne et al. 1980, 1981). Loss aversion, by definition, emphasizes a greater weighting attached to a negative event (Kahneman et al. 1990). Insofar as availability heuristics is concerned, people often base their predictions of the rate of occurrence within a population on the ease with which an example can be brought to mind (Tversky and Kahneman 1973). In the context of making decision choices, one will thus be more inclined to choose that option with a greater presence of positive outcomes, assuming equal actual expected outcome values for the different options.
Prior studies regarding decisions under uncertainty in finance and risk management have also postulated the importance of risk, in that an investor’s emphasis is on avoiding the prospect of not meeting some target rate of return (Mao 1970). Payne (2005) conceptualized risk from the perspective of the overall probability of success or positive outcome in obtaining a certain level of return, thus the focus is on both increasing the overall probability of a gain as well as decreasing the overall probability of a loss. In effect, the higher (lower) the risk, the lower (higher) the probability of a favorable outcome, or conversely, the higher (lower) the probability of an unfavorable outcome. Edwards (1954) similarly showed in his study that the research subjects prefer low probabilities of losing large amounts of money to high probabilities of losing small amounts. Striving to consistently stay above one’s aspiration (success) level, Siegel (1957) provided further evidence in this regard.
Earlier studies have so far largely identified the risk construct into two main groups: (1) as mentioned earlier, those conceiving risk as the failure to obtain a certain level of returns, and (2) those conceiving risk as relating to the total dispersion of the outcomes (i.e., measured in terms of the whole distribution variance) (Libby and Fishburn 1977). Since prior evidence has largely suggested that risk is more related to probability and the amount of loss (hence adhering to such decision rules as ‘minimize possible loss’ or ‘maximize possible gain’) (e.g., Slovic 1972), this study varies the level of risk (i.e., with an inverse relationship to the probability of success or a favorable outcome) and examines one’s risky investment decision choices given a prior gain (as compared to a loss or neutral) condition. Hence, controlling for one’s CFC, this study examines whether one’s prior gain/loss and personal risk profile can impact their risk–payoff choices.

2.3. Justification

Justification strategies in investment, shopping, or health-related decisions provide various rich contexts to examine judgment accountability (e.g., Huber et al. 2009; Johnson and Kaplan 1991; Park and Hill 2018; Schippers and Rus 2021; Tetlock 1983), whereby the process of explaining one’s decision to support a chosen option over another could evoke a more cognitively vigilant mental processing mode. One’s fundamental attribution error tendency could also be attenuated when meaningful accountability is introduced (Tetlock 1985; Lerner and Tetlock 1999). In essence, by being mindful and deliberate, in spending enough time to consider as many factors as possible before making a decision, this thought process could potentially reduce the risk of ending up with information-processing failures, thereby minimizing any divergence in one’s decision preference across differing endowment effects.
The main impetus for incorporating strategic justification requirements as an experimental treatment in this study is to examine if this will lead to a reduction in one’s propensity toward making high-risk choices. Using a more effortful process to document one’s formal decision rules will hopefully point to a more effective prescription toward reducing biased and risky choices previously observed. Hence, based on one’s prior gain or loss and personal risk profile, this study further examines if and how a more in-depth cognitive reasoning process impacting one’s choice preference can be evoked through the imposition of a justification requirement.

3. Hypotheses Development

Prior focus on the overall probability of success stemming from aspiration level is evident from studies by Payne and Braunstein (1971), Payne et al. (1980, 1981), and Payne (2005). Results indicate that subjects prefer the option with a higher overall probability of a positive outcome or gain, ceteris paribus. Insofar as the endowment effect is concerned, an increment in value is evident as a result of a person’s endowment due to ownership. This arises primarily because of the asymmetry between gains and losses, with a loss generally viewed as more aversive than a comparable gain (Kahneman and Tversky 1979). In this study, the objective is to examine whether one with a prior gain position can accentuate such an endowment-driven, loss-aversion phenomenon, while a prior loss position can conversely mollify such an endowment-driven, loss-aversion phenomenon. Thus, in adherence to prior findings related to the endowment effect, with a prior gain (loss), one will intuitively have greater (lower) loss- and risk-aversion tendencies, hence will be more inclined to prefer decisions with lower (higher) risk.
Individuals with lower CFC will logically prefer a more certain but smaller expected payoff, while a person higher in CFC will comparatively prefer a less certain but larger expected payoff. This study thus addresses the gap in the existing literature by further examining how one’s CFC could induce differing temporal distance perceptions in adherence to the near- versus distant-future construal, which when considered in conjunction with one’s prior gain or loss position, will impact one’s decision choices. Prior research established that one who scores low in CFC is less affected by their prior gain or loss, and will be more inclined to direct resources away from the options maximizing long-term future benefits toward those options maximizing more certain short-term interests, in effect seeking the less risky (i.e., more certain) option (Joireman and Balliet 2004; Joireman et al. 2005). On the other hand, for those with high CFC, the endowment effect with a longer-term future benefit will likely take precedence (depending on whether one has a prior gain or loss), thus resulting in a stronger response to a loss than to a corresponding gain. Subject to “risk-seeking in the losses” and possibly moderated by one’s personal risk profile, this study also further builds on prior postulation and examines one’s risk–payoff choice preferences, depending on whether one has a prior gain or loss in a high CFC setting. Hypotheses One and Two are henceforth formally stated as:
H1. 
One who scores low (high) in CFC will be more inclined to prefer decisions with lower (higher) risk (i.e., higher (lower) probability) and lower (higher) payoff.
H2a. 
One with a prior gain (loss) and who scores high in CFC will be inclined to prefer decisions with lower (higher) risk and lower (higher) payoff.
H2b. 
The effect of prior gain/loss and CFC on risk–payoff preference is moderated by one’s personal risk profile.
Tetlock (1983) first discussed the construct of accountability in terms of justification, defining it as the social pressure to justify one’s decisions to others. Simonson and Nye (1992) found that, depending on the decision context, accountability can end up improving, degrading, or not changing one’s performance. In enhancing one’s self-critical attention to the judgment process as well as evoking a greater cognizance of the information provided, this study further examines if imposition of a justification requirement will lead to a less biased choice in a high CFC setting, potentially enabling one to pivot toward a decision with lower risk–payoff dynamics. As one documents the formal decision rules to follow, a more de-biased overall outcome should logically eventuate. Hypothesis Three, in imposing a strategic justification requirement, is henceforth formally stated as:
H3. 
For one who scores high in CFC, justification prompts will help to reduce the propensity toward high risk–payoff.

4. Research Design

The objective of this study is to examine the difference in risk–payoff preference between one with a prior gain vis-a-vis one with a prior loss. The experimental design is 3 (prior gain, prior loss, and neutral as control) × 2 (no justification versus justification requirement) randomized between-subject, with 370 undergraduate business students (recruited over two semesters) from a public university in the U.S., participating in exchange for extra course credits in an undergraduate accounting course. To improve participation, besides being awarded the extra course credits, each participant was entered into a cash lottery draw of USD 25 for each class of participants. Participants in the “justification requirement” condition provided their opinions on the specific reasons they have adopted in regard to their choices. An open-ended response format was used for participants who were prompted to provide justification for their choices. All participants were assessed on their CFC and personal risk profiles after they had indicated their choices. Four participants were removed from the final usable sample for leaving right after the experimental study began or for having not spent any time to reason (when prompted to justify),1 thereby rendering their subsequent responses invalid with the substantially missing data responses (final sample: n = 366, 45% male, 55% female, median age = 20.6).
Part of the experimental instrument regarding the risk choice options was adapted from Sagristano et al. (2002), Payne (2005), and Payne et al. (1980). It started by narrating a synopsis whereby the participant is currently assumed to be in a position of net gain, neutral, or net loss. Recently, the participant became aware of a prospectus regarding a new investment fund. Each participant is to assume that this new investment fund comes with two options (Choice A or B). To prevent participants’ selection from being confounded by order effects, two within-subject scenarios were provided. In Scenario 1, Choice A has a lower risk (i.e., higher probability) of a lower expected payoff as compared to Choice B. Conversely, in Scenario 2, Choice A has a higher risk (i.e., lower probability) of a higher expected payoff as compared to Choice B. Each participant is then tasked to choose whether to invest in Choice A or B for both scenarios. A coding of “1” is used when a participant chooses lower risk and lower payoff, and “2” when one chooses higher risk and higher payoff, hence providing a possible range of 2 (lowest) to 4 (highest) in summing the combined choices made for the two scenarios.2 This risk score (i.e., the risk variance spread preference value) will form the dependent variable in our analyses. A copy of the case is found in Appendix A.
All participants completed the adapted 14-item CFC scale (see Joireman et al. 2012, 2006; Strathman et al. 1994), as shown in Appendix B. The 14-item CFC scale contains general statements regarding a person’s propensity to take into account future consequences of their behavior. Participants were asked to indicate whether each of the fourteen statements was characteristic of themselves by circling the appropriate number on the scale, ranging from 1 (extremely uncharacteristic) to 7 (extremely characteristic). Uni-dimensionality of items in the composite CFC measurement model was affirmed using factor analysis to ascertain reliability (SRMR = 0.030, RMSEA = 0.045, CFI = 0.995, TLI = 0.993).3 Upon completion of the experiment, each participant answered a post-experimental debriefing questionnaire, eliciting demographic information for use in subsequent covariate testing.

5. Analyses

Analyses of covariates (ANCOVA) ascertain that the following factors exert no qualitative influence on the final results: gender, age, class level, major, GPA, work experience, current investments, personal and family net worth, as well as perceived investment sophistication level (all p > 0.100). All participants of the final usable sample completed the adapted 14-item CFC scale (modified from Strathman et al. 1994). Prior research has shown that CFC is a reliable, stable, and valid construct with wide-ranging behavioral implications. In effect, individuals with low (high) CFC attach a high (low) degree of importance to the immediate consequences of their behaviors, and a low (high) importance to the delayed consequences of their behaviors (Joireman et al. 2006). As illustrated in Table 1, the Cronbach alpha value based on standardized items is 0.877 (p = 0.000, DF = 91). Table 2 also shows the correlation matrix of the 14 items used in the CFC scale administration (with significant p values), hence affirming the reliability of the CFC items used.
To test for Hypothesis 1, an independent sample t-test was performed to compare the group statistics between those with high versus low CFC (by dividing their responses using the CFC scale’s midway point).4 Evident from Table 3 and Table 4, those with low CFC show a significantly lower risk (i.e., higher probability) preference as compared to those with high CFC (low CFC at mean = 2.26 as compared to high CFC at mean 2.83, significantly different at p = 0.000).5 In effect, the results here indicate that those with low (high) CFC prefer near- (distant-) future preferences for higher (lower) probability, albeit a lower (higher) risk–payoff, thus affirming Hypothesis 1.
To test for Hypotheses 2, variance tests of between-subject effects were performed on those with high CFC values. Table 5 illustrates the descriptive statistics of the different factors (personal risk profile,6 prior gain/loss, justification requirement, and one’s CFC position). Table 6 shows that, among the group with high CFC, those who are risk-seekers will uniformly adopt a more risky preference (i.e., higher risk scores), vis-a-vis that for risk avoiders.
Also shown in Table 6, for those without justification requirement as well as irrespective of the risk profile, with a prior gain (loss), one who scores high in CFC will be significantly more (less) inclined to prefer decisions with a lower risk (mean of 2.94 versus 3.80, p < 0.010), thus affirming Hypothesis 2a. Further, in examining the possible moderation effects arising from one’s personal risk profile, for participants with a prior loss and without justification prompt, those with high CFC will show a greater propensity to prefer a choice entailing a higher risk regardless of their personal risk profile (mean for risk seekers = 3.82 versus mean for risk avoiders = 3.77, at p > 0.100). On the other hand, with a prior gain and without justification prompt, participants with high CFC will choose the less risky approach if they are risk avoiders (mean = 2.06) versus the more risky approach if they are risk seekers (mean = 3.81, at p < 0.010). This moderating effect of one’s prior gain/loss position with their personal risk profile is also graphically evident from Figure 1, thus affirming Hypothesis 2b.7
Finally, in affirming Hypothesis 3, the findings show that when those with high CFC were prompted to provide strategic justifications, an overall reduction in one’s propensity toward high risk–payoff is evident, irrespective of prior gain/loss and risk profile (p < 0.010). Additional analyses also show that, when those with high CFC were prompted to provide their justifications, there was no significant difference in choice preference for risk avoiders with a prior gain versus loss (mean for gain group = 2.02 versus mean for loss group = 2.03, at p > 0.100). However, under the same conditions, there is a significant difference in choice preference for risk seekers with a prior gain versus loss (mean for the gain group = 2.25 versus mean for the loss group = 3.00, at p < 0.010). This effect is statistically illustrated by the interaction effect of one’s prior gain/loss position with the imposition of a justification requirement (p = 0.000) in Table 7, and is also graphically evident from Figure 2. Overall, Table 7 shows that insofar as one’s prior gain/loss position, imposition of a justification requirement, as well as personal risk profile are concerned, each exerts main and 2-way interaction effects on the dependent variable (all at p = 0.000, and with adjusted R-squared = 0.690 affirming a good-fitting model).8 A summary of all the findings is shown in Table 8.

6. Limitations and Future Research

The results of this study should be interpreted in light of its contextual limitations. In the name of parsimony, no mention is made in this study to the magnitude of the prior gain or loss amounts nor regarding the timeline for the existing as well as that of the intended future investments. The time value of money doctrine may affect how one will allocate their wealth. Also, in both experimental scenarios, participants were prompted to choose only one of the two profit options provided, while not provided with any loss option nor with the choice of prorating fractional allocations. Further, since this study was only conducted with an undergraduate business participant pool, with no decision-based monetary incentive at stake, findings may be biased. Future research could potentially explore the generalizability and real-world relevance of these findings across different demographic, cultural, and geographic settings.9 Similarly, one can extend this study toward a corporate setting, examining the motivations that may drive managerial decision-making processes. Last but not least, in conducting a laboratory experiment, participants were provided with the probability of achieving the various outcomes in each choice option, ex ante would not be feasible in reality.

7. Conclusions and Implications

Overall, in affirming the three hypotheses, one’s decision choice preference is shown to be dependent upon CFC inclination, prior gain or loss, as well as personal risk profile. Nonetheless, with justification requirements, there is an overall reduction in one’s propensity for making high risk–payoff choices.
In terms of theoretical implications, this study complements prior research of Howlett et al. (2008), Koonce et al. (2005a), as well as Koonce et al. (2005b) by examining choice preference effects of varied CFC profiles in conjunction with other important variables like one’s personal risk profile, prior gain or loss, as well as the crucial imposition of a justification requirement. Juxtaposing the construct, consideration of future consequences, with the variables examined in prior research studies, findings here allude to the boundary conditions particularly for individuals with high CFC, whereby only those with a prior gain will manifest endowment effect tendencies. This would, in turn, result in one’s aversion toward committing to a higher-risk decision. On the contrary, one with a prior loss would gravitate toward making a higher-risk decision. Further, when a strategic justification requirement is imposed, an overall reduction in one’s propensity toward high risk–payoff is evident. Additional analyses also show that the personal risk profile will moderate the effects when a justification requirement is imposed, resulting in a de-biased effect for risk avoiders, while exacerbating the biased effect for risk seekers.
The research findings here hold practical implications, particularly in a volatile economic environment, whereby, with increased pressure, the use of heuristics for investment decision-making may become more prevalent. In matters concerning personal finance, investors should remain leery of potentially misleading and sometimes fraudulent claims from investment scheme promoters during bad economic times, a period when an investor’s investment portfolio and psyche could typically take a hit. Many unfortunate human situations unfold in circumstances where people take desperate gambles and potentially turn manageable failures into even greater disasters. Classic quote from the famed Warren Buffett sums it up, ‘When the tide goes out, you learn who’s been swimming naked.’ Capitalizing on investors’ weakened morale and cognitive biases as a result of suffering a loss in their existing investment portfolios, marketers will often take advantage of the situation to push investment products that promise greater potential gain (while obfuscating the potential loss), thus entailing a greater level of risk. Despite the observed pitfalls, results from this study’s justification prompt exercise show that investors can reduce their propensity for making high risk–payoff choices by exercising greater due diligence through formal, justifying reasoning in strategic areas when rationalizing one’s decisions. This mechanism seems to cause risk avoiders to uniformly seek lower risk–payoff choices. On the other hand, for risk seekers, with a justification requirement, those with a prior gain (loss) will now be inclined to make a lower- (higher-) risk decision, although the overall risk level undertaken by either risk profile is still significantly decreased as compared to that without the justification requirement.
Ultimately, a conscious effort to strategically justify should uniformly help lower risk in varying degrees, thereby leading to a more reasoned and rational mechanism in decision-making, irrespective of one’s CFC, prior gain/loss, and risk profile. From a public interest perspective in personal financial planning, when tasked to make risky decision choices, one could take heed of the findings from this study and strive to conduct a more efficacious deliberation as part of due diligence. Particularly in a volatile economic environment, when investors are more prone to make emotion-driven decisions, this study will hopefully result in an overall greater understanding of the mechanism shaping one’s decision-making processes.

Funding

The author acknowledges financial and research support from Washington State University and the University of Northern Iowa.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article are available on request from the author.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Case Background
Gain condition: You have so far made a gain (resulting in a net positive sum to date).
Neutral condition: You have so far not made a gain or loss (resulting in a breakeven to date).
Loss condition: You have so far made a loss (resulting in a net negative sum to date).
Instruction
Recently, you received a prospectus regarding a new investment fund. With some money on hand to invest, you are now deciding whether to invest in CHOICE A or CHOICE B (i.e., two possibilities), assuming you know the probabilities of the payoffs for each choice.
For each of the two scenarios below, probabilities of earning the payoffs are listed.
No justification requirement condition:
For each scenario, choose (by circling) only ONE choice you would put your money in.
Scenario 1:
CHOICE A: 60% probability of gaining USD 3000
CHOICE B: 40% probability of gaining USD 4500
Scenario 2: (to be reverse coded)
CHOICE A: 40% probability of gaining USD 12,000, 20% probability of gaining USD 11,000
CHOICE B: 70% probability of gaining USD 6000, 70% probability of gaining USD 4000
Justification requirement condition:
Provide your justification within the box by listing down your reasons, as you assess your choice A or B selection in each of the two scenarios below. After assessing, for each scenario, choose (by circling) only ONE choice you would put your money in.
 
 
 
 
 
Scenario 1:
CHOICE A: 60% probability of gaining USD 3000
CHOICE B: 40% probability of gaining USD 4500
Scenario 2: (to be reverse coded)
CHOICE A: 40% probability of gaining USD 12,000, 20% probability of gaining USD 11,000
CHOICE B: 70% probability of gaining USD 6000, 70% probability of gaining USD 4000
For both No Justification requirement and Justification requirement conditions:
All participants are assessed their Consideration of Future Consequences and Personal Risk Profiles after they have circled their choices for both scenarios.

Appendix B

Assessing consideration of future consequences (CFC)
For each of the statements below, indicate whether or not the statement is characteristic of you based on the 1–7 scale shown below. Please write your answer in the LEFT SPACE provided.
1------------------2------------------3----------4----------5--------------6-------------7-------------
ExtremelyModeratelySlightlyNeutralSlightlyModeratelyExtremely
UncharacteristicUncharacteristicUncharacteristicCharacteristicCharacteristicCharacteristic
____ 1.I consider how things might be in the future, and try to influence those things with my day-to-day behavior.
____ 2.Often, I engage in behaviors to achieve outcomes that may not produce results for many years.
____ 3. RI only act to satisfy immediate concerns, figuring the future will take care of itself.
____ 4. RMy behavior is only influenced by the immediate and short-term outcomes of my actions.
____ 5. RMy convenience is a big factor in the decisions I make or the actions I take.
____ 6. I am willing to sacrifice my immediate happiness or well-being in order to achieve future outcomes.
____ 7.I think it is important to take warnings about negative outcomes seriously even if the negative outcome will not occur for many years.
____ 8.I think it is more important to perform a behavior with important distant consequences than a behavior with less important immediate consequences.
____ 9. RI generally ignore warnings about possible future problems because I think the problems will be resolved before they reach crisis level.
____ 10. RI think that sacrificing now is usually unnecessary since future outcomes can be dealt with at a later time.
____ 11. RI only act to satisfy immediate concerns, figuring that I will take care of future problems that may occur at a later date.
____ 12. RIt is more important to me to take care of my day-to-day work that has immediate outcomes than my work that has distant outcomes.
____ 13. When I make a decision, I think about how it might affect me in the future.
____ 14. My behavior is generally influenced by future consequences.
R: Reverse-scored items.

Notes

1
This is affirmed through a time clock monitored by a third-party researcher during the experimental study. In addition, there is no significant difference in the amount of time spent on both tasks by the participants from both conditions (p > 0.100). Justification responses received for both tasks were also quite uniform in length.
2
In analyzing the participants’ responses, Scenario 2 was reverse-coded. Lower (higher) combined scores for the responses from both scenarios thus indicate a greater affinity for lower (higher) risk and payoff.
3
Vilar et al. (2022) and Joireman et al. (2012) have alternatively suggested a two-factor solution (CFC-Future and CFC-Immediate) to the CFC scale. Dichotomizing into two factors, factor analyses similarly affirmed adequate internal reliabilities for both factors and a sufficiently robust model fit.
4
A greater number of participants responded in the upper half of the CFC scale (i.e., n = 339 for those with averages above ‘4′ on the CFC scale), as compared to those who responded in the lower half of the CFC scale (i.e., n = 27 for those with averages below ‘4′ on the CFC scale). Since ‘4′ on the 7-point CFC scale denotes a neutral position, and with the upper half denoting characteristic and the lower half denoting uncharacteristic, this study dichotomizes the high versus low CFC group at the midway point of the CFC scale.
5
Mann–Whitney U-testing was performed as a nonparametric alternative, with the results remaining qualitatively unchanged (p > 0.100).
6
Respondents were asked to self-assess their personal risk profile on a 0 (unwilling to take risks) to 10 (fully prepared to take risks) scale with the following question: “How do you see yourself in financial matters: Are you a person who is fully prepared to take risks or do you try to avoid taking risks?” (adapted from Dohmen et al. (2005)). The ‘risk avoider’ versus the ‘risk seeker’ group was differentiated using the midway point of 5 on the scale. Additional testing for the small number of respondents scoring ‘5′ shows that their subsequent responses were much closer to those of risk avoiders (p < 0.050), hence were also counted as risk avoiders. The results of the study remain qualitatively unchanged when this middle group is removed from the test sample (p > 0.100).
7
Results for risk and payoff are both directionally consistent; Untabulated analyses based on responses from those with low CFC showed that this group exhibits a uniformly greater inclination to prefer decision choices with a lower risk (i.e., higher probability) of a payoff, irrespective of differences in risk profiles as well as prior gain or loss position (all p > 0.100).
8
Untabulated analyses for those with low CFC show that the earlier observed preference for a lower risk variance spread remains unchanged regardless of whether a justification requirement is imposed or not (p > 0.100).
9
The sample and the general population data may differ in various dimensions. For instance, according to the U.S. Census Bureau projections’ data for 2022, the percentage of the population in the 18–24 age group is 9.4%, the percentage of the male population is 49.6%, and the percentage of persons 18–24 years of age with a Bachelors’ degree is 11.4% among that age group (see https://www.census.gov (accessed on 27 December 2023)).

References

  1. Cahlikova, Jana, and Lubomir Cingl. 2017. Risk preferences under acute stress. Experimental Economics 20: 209–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Dohmen, Thomas J., Armin Falk, David Huffman, Uwe Sunde, Jürgen Schupp, and Gert G. Wagner. 2005. Individual Risk Attitudes: New Evidence from a Large, Representative, Experimentally-Validated Survey. Discussion Paper No. 1730. Bonn: Institute for the Study of Labor. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Edwards, Ward. 1954. The theory of decision making. Psychological Bulletin 51: 380–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Grable, John E., and Abed Rabbani. 2014. Risk tolerance across life domains: Evidence from a sample of older adults. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 25: 174–83. [Google Scholar]
  5. Heider, Fritz. 1958. The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New York: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Heo, Wookjae, Abed Rabbani, and John E. Grable. 2021. An evaluation of the effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on the risk tolerance of financial decision makers. Finance Research Letters 41: 101842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Hoorens, Vera, Jozef M. Nuttin Jr., Ildikó Erdélyi Herman, and Ubolwanna Pavakanun. 1990. Mastery pleasure versus mere ownership; A quasi-experimental cross-cultural and cross-alphabetical test of the name letter effect. European Journal of Social Psychology 20: 181–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Howlett, Elizabeth, Jeremy Kees, and Elyria Kemp. 2008. The role of self-regulation, future orientation, and financial knowledge in long-term financial decisions. The Journal of Consumer Affairs 42: 223–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Huber, Oswald, Arlette S. Bär, and Odilo W. Huber. 2009. Justification pressure in risky decision making: Search for risk defusing operators. Acta Psychologica 130: 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Irandoust, Manuchehr. 2017. Factors associated with financial risk tolerance based on proportional odds model: Evidence from Sweden. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 28: 155–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Johnson, Van E., and Steven E. Kaplan. 1991. Experimental evidence on the effects of accountability on auditor judgments. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 10: 96–107. [Google Scholar]
  12. Joireman, Jeff, Alan Strathman, and Daniel Balliet. 2006. Considering future consequences: An integrative model. In Judgments over Time: The Interplay of Thoughts, Feelings, and Behaviors. Edited by Lawrence Sanna and Edward Chang. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 88–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Joireman, Jeff, and Daniel Balliet. 2004. Preference for certain but smaller vs less certain but larger outcomes as a function of individual differences in the consideration of future consequences. Unpublished Data. [Google Scholar]
  14. Joireman, Jeff, David E. Sprott, and Eric R. Spangenberg. 2005. Fiscal responsibility and the consideration of future consequences. Personality and Individual Differences 39: 1159–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Joireman, Jeff, Monte Shaffer, Daniel Balliet, and Alan Strathman. 2012. Promotion orientation explains why future-oriented people exercise and eat healthy: Evidence from the two-factor consideration of future consequences-14 scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 38: 1272–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. 1979. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47: 263–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Kahneman, Daniel, Jack L. Knetsch, and Richard H. Thaler. 1990. Experimental tests of the endowment effect and the Coase theorem. Journal of Political Economy 98: 1325–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kirchler, Michael, David Andersson, Caroline Bonn, Magnus Johannesson, Erik Sorenson, Matthias Stefan, Gustav Tinghog, and Daniel Vastfjall. 2017. The effects of fast and slow decisions on risk taking. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 54: 37–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Kocher, Martin, Julius Pahlke, and Stefan Trautmann. 2013. Tempus fugit: Time pressure in risky decisions. Management Science 59: 2380–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Koonce, Lisa, Marlys Lipe, and Mary McAnally. 2005a. Judging the risk of financial instruments: Problems and potential remedies. The Accounting Review 80: 871–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Koonce, Lisa, Mary McAnally, and Molly Mercer. 2005b. How do investors judge the risk of financial items? The Accounting Review 80: 221–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Lerner, Jennifer. S., and Philip. E. Tetlock. 1999. Accounting for the effects of accountability. Psychological Bulletin 125: 255–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Libby, Robert, and Peter. C. Fishburn. 1977. Behavioral models of risk taking in business decisions: A survey and evaluation. Journal of Accounting Research 15: 272–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Liberman, Nira, and Yaacov Trope. 1998. The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75: 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Mao, James C. T. 1970. Survey of capital budgeting: Theory and practice. Journal of Finance 25: 349–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Murphy, Lisa, Elmer Cadogan, and Samantha Dockray. 2020. The consideration of future consequences: Evidence for domain specificity across five life domains. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 46: 663–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Noman, Abdullah, Lanlan Chu, and Mohammad Rahman. 2023. Subjective and objective financial knowledge and their associations with financial risk tolerance. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 34: 219–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Park, Jisook, and W. Trey Hill. 2018. Exploring the role of justification and cognitive effort exertion on post-purchase regret in online shopping. Computers in Human Behavior 83: 235–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Payne, John W. 2005. It is whether you win or lose: The importance of the overall probabilities of winning or losing in risky choice. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 30: 5–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Payne, John W., and Myron L. Braunstein. 1971. Preferences among gambles with equal underlying distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 87: 13–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Payne, John W., Dan J. Laughhunn, and Roy Crum. 1980. Translation of gambles and aspiration level effects in risky choice behavior. Management Science 26: 1039–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Payne, John W., Dan J. Laughhunn, and Roy Crum. 1981. Further tests of aspiration level effects in risky choice behavior. Management Science 27: 953–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Rabbani, Abed, John Grable, Barbara O’Neill, Frances Lawrence, and Zheying Yao. 2021. Financial risk tolerance before and after a stock market shock: Testing the recency bias hypothesis. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 32: 294–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Schippers, Michaela, and Diana Rus. 2021. Optimizing decision-making processes in times of COVID-19: Using reflexivity to counteract information-processing failures. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 650525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Sagristano, Michael, Yaacov Trope, and Nira Liberman. 2002. Time-dependent gambling: Odds now, money later. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 131: 364–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Schooley, Diane, and Debra Worden. 2016. Perceived and realized risk tolerance: Changes during the 2008 financial crisis. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 27: 265–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Sharpe, William. 1970. Portfolio Theory and Capital Markets. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  38. Siegel, Sidney. 1957. Level of aspiration and decision making. Psychological Review 64: 253–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Simonson, Itamar, and Peter Nye. 1992. The effect of accountability on susceptibility to decision errors. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 51: 416–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Slovic, Paul. 1972. Psychological study of human judgment: Implications for investment decision making. Journal of Finance 27: 779–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Strathman, Alan, Faith Gleicher, David Boninger, and Scott Edwards. 1994. The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66: 742–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Tetlock, Philip E. 1983. Accountability and complexity of thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45: 74–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Tetlock, Philip E. 1985. Accountability: A social check on the fundamental attribution error. Social Psychology Quarterly 48: 227–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Trope, Yaacov, and Nira Liberman. 2000. Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79: 876–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. 1973. Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology 5: 207–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Vilar, Roosevelt, Taciano Milfont, Rafaella Araújo, Gabriel Coelho, Ana Soares, and Valdiney Gouveia. 2022. Consideration of future consequences (CFC): Validation and proposition of an ultra-short scale. Current Psychology 41: 3428–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Zimbardo, Philip, and John Boyd. 1999. Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual- differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77: 1271–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Estimated marginal means of decision choice for one with high CFC.
Figure 1. Estimated marginal means of decision choice for one with high CFC.
Jrfm 17 00083 g001
Figure 2. Estimated marginal means of decision choice for one with high CFC.
Figure 2. Estimated marginal means of decision choice for one with high CFC.
Jrfm 17 00083 g002
Table 1. KMO and Bartlett’s test.
Table 1. KMO and Bartlett’s test.
Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy.0.877
Bartlett’s test of sphericity:Approx. chi-square1.666 × 103
Df91
Sig.0.000
Table 2. Inter-item correlation matrix among consideration of future consequences (CFC) items a.
Table 2. Inter-item correlation matrix among consideration of future consequences (CFC) items a.
C1C2C3C4C5C6C7C8C9C10C11C12C13C14
C11.00.3170.3720.3440.1100.3320.2450.1860.3020.3260.4030.2960.5270.552
C20.3171.00.2330.2370.2170.2980.0930.1900.1320.2190.3640.2970.2670.335
C30.3720.2331.0000.6840.2430.2800.1830.1550.2930.3610.6040.3650.3350.396
C40.3440.2370.6841.0000.2870.2440.1910.1480.3120.3130.5550.3520.3410.386
C50.1100.2170.2430.2871.0000.042−0.009−0.0110.1900.2410.2920.2530.0390.073
C60.3320.2980.2800.2440.0421.000.3560.2000.1310.2960.3890.2470.3290.388
C70.2450.0930.1830.191−0.0090.3561.000.2390.3980.3640.2720.1490.3700.313
C80.1860.1900.1550.148−0.0110.2000.2391.000.1760.1990.2190.0610.2300.238
C90.3020.1320.2930.3120.1900.1310.3980.1761.0000.4690.4070.2960.3660.355
C100.3260.2190.3610.3130.2410.2960.3640.1990.4691.0000.5050.3100.3660.330
C110.4030.3640.6040.5550.2920.3890.2720.2190.4070.5051.0000.4690.3940.425
C120.2960.2970.3650.3520.2530.2470.1490.0610.2960.3100.4691.0000.2940.294
C130.5270.2670.3350.3410.0390.3290.3700.2300.3660.3660.3940.2941.000.646
C140.5520.3350.3960.3860.0730.3880.3130.2380.3550.3300.4250.2940.6461.000
Sig. (1-tailed)C1 0.0000.0000.0000.0180.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
C20.000 0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0380.0000.0060.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
C30.0000.000 0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0020.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
C40.0000.0000.000 0.0000.0000.0000.0020.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
C50.0180.0000.0000.000 0.2110.4320.4150.0000.0000.0000.0000.2300.083
C60.0000.0000.0000.0000.211 0.0000.0000.0060.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
C70.0000.0380.0000.0000.4320.000 0.0000.0000.0000.0000.0020.0000.000
C80.0000.0000.0020.0020.4150.0000.000 0.0000.0000.0000.1220.0000.000
C90.0000.0060.0000.0000.0000.0060.0000.000 0.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
C100.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000 0.0000.0000.0000.000
C110.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000 0.0000.0000.000
C120.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0020.1220.0000.0000.000 0.0000.000
C130.0000.0000.0000.0000.2300.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000 0.000
C140.0000.0000.0000.0000.0830.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.0000.000
Note: a Determinant = 0.009.
Table 3. Test of difference between high and low CFC.
Table 3. Test of difference between high and low CFC.
Group Statistics
CFCnMeanStd. DeviationStd. Error Mean
DVLow272.260.5260.101
High3392.830.8540.046
Note: DV dependent variable: risk variance spread preference (2: low to 4: high).
Table 4. Independent samples test.
Table 4. Independent samples test.
Levene’s Test for Equality of
Variances
t-Test for Equality of Means
FSig.tdfSig. (2-Tailed)Mean DifferenceStd. Error Difference95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
LowerUpper
DVEqual variances assumed22.3090.000−3.3933640.001−0.5670.167−0.895−0.238
Equal variances not assumed −5.09137.9610.000−0.5670.111−0.792−0.341
Table 5. Between-subject factors.
Table 5. Between-subject factors.
DescriptionTypen
RiskSeeker185
Avoider181
PriorGain151
Neutral68
Loss147
JustificationNo177
Yes189
CFCLow27
High339
Table 6. Descriptive statistics for high CFC responses.
Table 6. Descriptive statistics for high CFC responses.
Prior Gain/LossJustificationRisk ProfileMeanStd. Deviationn
GainNoSeeker3.810.39730
Avoider2.060.24630
Total2.940.94160
YesSeeker2.250.61541
Avoider2.020.15241
Total2.140.46282
TotalSeeker2.910.94171
Avoider2.040.19771
Total2.480.807142
NeutralNoSeeker3.760.43715
Avoider2.500.51614
Total3.150.79529
YesSeeker3.050.40516
Avoider2.120.34214
Total2.630.59830
TotalSeeker3.390.54931
Avoider2.310.47128
Total2.880.74459
LossNoSeeker3.820.38538
Avoider3.770.42337
Total3.800.40375
YesSeeker3.000.25031
Avoider2.030.17132
Total2.510.53363
TotalSeeker3.450.52869
Avoider2.970.93669
Total3.210.796138
TotalNoSeeker3.810.39582
Avoider2.920.88782
Total3.370.816164
YesSeeker2.670.61089
Avoider2.040.20486
Total2.360.553175
TotalSeeker3.220.771171
Avoider2.470.771168
Total2.850.856339
Notes: Dependent variable: risk variance spread preference (2: low to 4: high).
Table 7. Tests of between-subject effects for high CFC responses.
Table 7. Tests of between-subject effects for high CFC responses.
SourceType III Sum of SquaresdfMean SquareFSig.
Corrected model186.385 a823.298102.6240.000
Intercept2573.01312573.0131.133 × 1040.000
PriorGL (prior gain/loss position)28.226214.11362.1650.000
Justification61.741161.741271.9610.000
RiskProfile52.518152.518231.3330.000
PriorGL ×RiskProfile5.43322.71711.9660.000
PriorGL × justification7.64223.82116.8310.000
Error74.913300.227
Total3234339
Corrected total267.432338
Note: a R-squared = 0.697 (adjusted R-squared = 0.690). Dependent variable: risk variance spread preference (2: Low to 4: high).
Table 8. Summary of findings.
Table 8. Summary of findings.
CFCPrior Gain/LossRisk Avoider/SeekerJustificationRisk–Payoff PreferenceTests
Low LowH1 √
High High
HighGain NoLowerH2a √
HighLoss NoHigher
HighGainAvoiderNoLowerH2b √
HighGainSeekerNoHigher
HighLossAvoiderNoHigher
HighLossSeekerNoHigher
HighBoth Gain and lossAvoiderYesLowerH3 √
HighGainSeekerYesLowerOverall lower
HighLossSeekerYesHigher
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lee, W.E. How Consideration of Future Consequences, Prior Gain or Loss, Personal Risk Profile, and Justification Affect Risk–Payoff Preferences. J. Risk Financial Manag. 2024, 17, 83. https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm17020083

AMA Style

Lee WE. How Consideration of Future Consequences, Prior Gain or Loss, Personal Risk Profile, and Justification Affect Risk–Payoff Preferences. Journal of Risk and Financial Management. 2024; 17(2):83. https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm17020083

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lee, W. Eric. 2024. "How Consideration of Future Consequences, Prior Gain or Loss, Personal Risk Profile, and Justification Affect Risk–Payoff Preferences" Journal of Risk and Financial Management 17, no. 2: 83. https://doi.org/10.3390/jrfm17020083

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop