Toward a New Approach to Investigate the Role of Working Memory in Stereotype Threat Effects
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Stereotype Threat and Working Memory in Young Adults
2.1. Working Memory as a Mediator of Stereotype Threat Effects
2.2. Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity as a Moderator of Stereotype Threat Effects
2.3. The Impact of Stereotype Threat Effects on Working Memory Task Performance
2.4. Summary of Findings and Critical Assessment of WM Measures Used in Young Adult Studies
3. Stereotype Threat and Working Memory in Older Adults
3.1. The Role of Working Memory in Age-Based Stereotype Threat: Contradictory Results
3.2. Methodological Differences That May Account for Conflicting Results in Older Adult Studies
4. Main Explanatory Theories of Stereotype Threat Effects
4.1. The Integrated Process Model of Stereotype Threat
4.2. The Regulatory Focus Model of Stereotype Threat
4.3. Confrontation between the Two Models
5. Investigating the Role of Attentional Resources in Working Memory in Stereotype Threat Effects
5.1. What Are Attentional Resources in Working Memory?
5.2. Toward a Precise Assessment of Attentional Resources in Working Memory
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Kite, M.E.; Deaux, K.; Haines, E.L. Gender Stereotypes. In Psychology of Women: A Handbook of Issues and Theories, 2nd ed.; Women’s Psychology; Praeger Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group: Westport, CT, USA, 2008; pp. 205–236. ISBN 978-0-275-99162-3. [Google Scholar]
- Brigham, J.C. Ethnic Stereotypes. Psychol. Bull. 1971, 76, 15–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kite, M.E.; Stockdale, G.D.; Whitley, B.E.; Johnson, B.T. Attitudes Toward Younger and Older Adults: An Updated Meta-Analytic Review. J. Soc. Issues 2005, 61, 241–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spencer, S.J.; Logel, C.; Davies, P.G. Stereotype Threat. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2016, 67, 415–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Steele, C.M. A Threat in the Air: How Stereotypes Shape Intellectual Identity and Performance. Am. Psychol. 1997, 52, 613–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Steele, C.M.; Aronson, J. Stereotype Threat and the Intellectual Test Performance of African Americans. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 69, 797–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schmader, T.; Johns, M.; Forbes, C. An Integrated Process Model of Stereotype Threat Effects on Performance. Psychol. Rev. 2008, 115, 336–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baddeley, A. Working Memory, Thought, and Action; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007; ISBN 978-0-19-852801-2. [Google Scholar]
- Barrouillet, P.; Camos, V. Working Memory: Loss and Reconstruction, 1st ed.; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2014; ISBN 978-1-315-75585-4. [Google Scholar]
- Cowan, N. Working Memory Capacity; Working Memory Capacity; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. ix, 246. ISBN 1-84169-097-X. [Google Scholar]
- Jarrold, C.; Towse, J.N. Individual Differences in Working Memory. Neuroscience 2006, 139, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daneman, M.; Carpenter, P.A. Individual Differences in Working Memory and Reading. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1980, 19, 450–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engle, R.W.; Kane, M.J.; Tuholski, S.W. Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and What They Tell Us about Controlled Attention, General Fluid Intelligence, and Functions of the Prefrontal Cortex. In Models of Working Memory; Miyake, A., Shah, P., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999; pp. 102–134. ISBN 978-0-521-58721-1. [Google Scholar]
- Johns, M.; Inzlicht, M.; Schmader, T. Stereotype Threat and Executive Resource Depletion: Examining the Influence of Emotion Regulation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2008, 137, 691–705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rydell, R.J.; McConnell, A.R.; Beilock, S.L. Multiple Social Identities and Stereotype Threat: Imbalance, Accessibility, and Working Memory. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 96, 949–966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmader, T.; Johns, M. Converging Evidence That Stereotype Threat Reduces Working Memory Capacity. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 440–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Loo, K.J.; Rydell, R.J. On the Experience of Feeling Powerful: Perceived Power Moderates the Effect of Stereotype Threat on Women’s Math Performance. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 39, 387–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mazerolle, M.; Régner, I.; Morisset, P.; Rigalleau, F.; Huguet, P. Stereotype Threat Strengthens Automatic Recall and Undermines Controlled Processes in Older Adults. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 723–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hess, T.M.; Hinson, J.T.; Hodges, E.A. Moderators of and Mechanisms Underlying Stereotype Threat Effects on Older Adults’ Memory Performance. Exp. Aging Res. 2009, 35, 153–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Popham, L.E.; Hess, T.M. Age Differences in the Underlying Mechanisms of Stereotype Threat Effects. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2015, 70, 223–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barber, S.J.; Mather, M. Stereotype Threat Can Both Enhance and Impair Older Adults’ Memory. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 2522–2529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barber, S.J. An Examination of Age-Based Stereotype Threat About Cognitive Decline: Implications for Stereotype-Threat Research and Theory Development. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 12, 62–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quinn, D.M.; Spencer, S.J. The Interference of Stereotype Threat with Women’s Generation of Mathematical Problem-Solving Strategies. J. Soc. Issues 2001, 57, 55–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, M.L.; Engle, R.W. Is Working Memory Capacity Task Dependent? J. Mem. Lang. 1989, 28, 127–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forbes, C.E.; Schmader, T. Retraining Attitudes and Stereotypes to Affect Motivation and Cognitive Capacity under Stereotype Threat. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 99, 740–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Régner, I.; Smeding, A.; Gimmig, D.; Thinus-Blanc, C.; Monteil, J.-M.; Huguet, P. Individual Differences in Working Memory Moderate Stereotype-Threat Effects. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 1646–1648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chan, N.H.W.; Rosenthal, H.E.S. Working Memory Moderates Stereotype Threat Effects for Adolescents in Hong Kong. Rev. Int. Psychol. Soc. 2014, 27, 103–118. [Google Scholar]
- Hutchison, K.A.; Smith, J.L.; Ferris, A. Goals Can Be Threatened to Extinction: Using the Stroop Task to Clarify Working Memory Depletion Under Stereotype Threat. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2013, 4, 74–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stroop, J.R. Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmader, T.; Forbes, C.E.; Zhang, S.; Berry Mendes, W. A Metacognitive Perspective on the Cognitive Deficits Experienced in Intellectually Threatening Environments. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 35, 584–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Allen, B.; Friedman, B.H. Threatening the Heart and Mind of Gender Stereotypes: Can Imagined Contact Influence the Physiology of Stereotype Threat?: Threatening the Heart and Mind. Psychophysiology 2016, 53, 105–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tine, M.; Gotlieb, R. Gender-, Race-, and Income-Based Stereotype Threat: The Effects of Multiple Stigmatized Aspects of Identity on Math Performance and Working Memory Function. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 2013, 16, 353–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McKown, C.; Strambler, M.J. Developmental Antecedents and Social and Academic Consequences of Stereotype-Consciousness in Middle Childhood. Child Dev. 2009, 80, 1643–1659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guardabassi, V.; Tomasetto, C. Does Weight Stigma Reduce Working Memory? Evidence of Stereotype Threat Susceptibility in Adults with Obesity. Int. J. Obes. 2018, 42, 1500–1507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guardabassi, V.; Tomasetto, C. Weight Status or Weight Stigma? Obesity Stereotypes—Not Excess Weight—Reduce Working Memory in School-Aged Children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2020, 189, 104706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blaine, H.; Sullivan, K.A.; Edmed, S.L. The Effect of Varied Test Instructions on Neuropsychological Performance Following Mild Traumatic Brain Injury: An Investigation of “Diagnosis Threat”. J. Neurotrauma 2013, 30, 1405–1414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kit, K.A.; Mateer, C.A.; Tuokko, H.A.; Spencer-Rodgers, J. Influence of Negative Stereotypes and Beliefs on Neuropsychological Test Performance in a Traumatic Brain Injury Population. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2014, 20, 157–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Suhr, J.A.; Gunstad, J. Further Exploration of the Effect of “Diagnosis Threat” on Cognitive Performance in Individuals with Mild Head Injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2005, 11, 23–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Beilock, S.L.; Rydell, R.J.; McConnell, A.R. Stereotype Threat and Working Memory: Mechanisms, Alleviation, and Spillover. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2007, 136, 256–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Baddeley, A. Working Memory. Science 1992, 255, 556–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Draheim, C.; Harrison, T.L.; Embretson, S.E.; Engle, R.W. What Item Response Theory Can Tell Us about the Complex Span Tasks. Psychol. Assess. 2018, 30, 116–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lamont, R.A.; Swift, H.J.; Abrams, D. A Review and Meta-Analysis of Age-Based Stereotype Threat: Negative Stereotypes, Not Facts, Do the Damage. Psychol. Aging 2015, 30, 180–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Armstrong, B.; Gallant, S.N.; Li, L.; Patel, K.; Wong, B.I. Stereotype Threat Effects on Older Adults’ Episodic and Working Memory: A Meta-Analysis. Gerontologist 2017, 57, S193–S205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Desrichard, O.; Köpetz, C. A Threat in the Elder: The Impact of Task-Instructions, Self-Efficacy and Performance Expectations on Memory Performance in the Elderly. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 35, 537–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chasteen, A.L.; Bhattacharyya, S.; Horhota, M.; Tam, R.; Hasher, L. How Feelings of Stereotype Threat Influence Older Adults’ Memory Performance. Exp. Aging Res. 2005, 31, 235–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mazerolle, M.; Régner, I.; Rigalleau, F.; Huguet, P. Stereotype Threat Alters the Subjective Experience of Memory. Exp. Psychol. 2015, 62, 395–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Follenfant, A.; Atzeni, T. True Performance: Reducing Stereotype Threat Effect in Older Adults’ Clinical Assessment of Memory. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2020, 27, 935–948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fresson, M.; Dardenne, B.; Geurten, M.; Meulemans, T. The Effect of Stereotype Threat on Older People’s Clinical Cognitive Outcomes: Investigating the Moderating Role of Dementia Worry. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2017, 31, 1306–1328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mazerolle, M.; Régner, I.; Barber, S.J.; Paccalin, M.; Miazola, A.-C.; Huguet, P.; Rigalleau, F. Negative Aging Stereotypes Impair Performance on Brief Cognitive Tests Used to Screen for Predementia. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2016, 72, 932–936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Barber, S.J. The Applied Implications of Age-Based Stereotype Threat for Older Adults. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2020, 9, 274–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ylikoski, R.; Ylikoski, A.; Keskivaara, P.; Tilvis, R.; Sulkava, R.; Erkinjuntti, T. Heterogeneity of Congnitive Profiles in Aging: Successful Aging, Normal Aging, and Individuals at Risks for Cognitive Decline. Eur. J. Neurol. 1999, 6, 645–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baghel, M.S.; Singh, P.; Srivas, S.; Thakur, M.K. Cognitive Changes with Aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. India Sect. B Biol. Sci. 2019, 89, 765–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wong, J.T.; Gallo, D.A. Activating Aging Stereotypes Increases Source Recollection Confusions in Older Adults: Effect at Encoding but Not Retrieval. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2019, 74, 633–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swift, H.J.; Abrams, D.; Marques, S. Threat or Boost? Social Comparison Affects Older People’s Performance Differently Depending on Task Domain. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2013, 68, 23–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lambert, A.E.; Watson, J.M.; Stefanucci, J.K.; Ward, N.; Bakdash, J.Z.; Strayer, D.L. Stereotype Threat Impairs Older Adult Driving: Stereotype Threat Impairs Older Adult Driving. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2016, 30, 22–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abrams, D.; Eller, A.; Bryant, J. An Age Apart: The Effects of Intergenerational Contact and Stereotype Threat on Performance and Intergroup Bias. Psychol. Aging 2006, 21, 691–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nicolas, P.; Lemaire, P.; Régner, I. When and How Stereotype Threat Influences Older Adults’ Arithmetic Performance: Insight from a Strategy Approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2020, 149, 343–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engle, R.W. Working Memory Capacity as Executive Attention. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 11, 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kane, M.J.; Conway, A.R.A.; Hambrick, D.Z.; Engle, R.W. Variation in Working Memory Capacity as Variation in Executive Attention and Control. In Variation in Working Memory; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 21–46. ISBN 0-19-516863-1. [Google Scholar]
- Seibt, B.; Förster, J. Stereotype Threat and Performance: How Self-Stereotypes Influence Processing by Inducing Regulatory Foci. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 87, 38–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Higgins, E.T. Beyond Pleasure and Pain. Am. Psychol. 1997, 52, 1280–1300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgins, E.T. Regulatory Fit. In Handbook of Motivation Science; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 356–372. ISBN 1-59385-568-0. [Google Scholar]
- Keller, J.; Bless, H. Regulatory Fit and Cognitive Performance: The Interactive Effect of Chronic and Situationally Induced Self-Regulatory Mechanisms on Test Performance. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 36, 393–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Motyka, S.; Grewal, D.; Puccinelli, N.M.; Roggeveen, A.L.; Avnet, T.; Daryanto, A.; de Ruyter, K.; Wetzels, M. Regulatory Fit: A Meta-analytic Synthesis. J. Consum. Psychol. 2014, 24, 394–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shah, J.; Higgins, T.; Friedman, R.S. Performance Incentives and Means: How Regulatory Focus Influences Goal Attainment. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 74, 285–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grimm, L.R.; Markman, A.B.; Maddox, W.T.; Baldwin, G.C. Stereotype Threat Reinterpreted as a Regulatory Mismatch. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 96, 288–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barber, S.J.; Mather, M. Stereotype Threat Can Reduce Older Adults’ Memory Errors. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2013, 66, 1888–1895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barber, S.J.; Mather, M.; Gatz, M. How Stereotype Threat Affects Healthy Older Adults’ Performance on Clinical Assessments of Cognitive Decline: The Key Role of Regulatory Fit. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2015, 70, 891–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wong, J.T.; Gallo, D.A. Stereotype Threat Reduces False Recognition When Older Adults Are Forewarned. Memory 2016, 24, 650–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aaker, J.L.; Lee, A.Y. Understanding Regulatory Fit. J. Mark. Res. 2006, 43, 15–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Avnet, T.; Laufer, D.; Higgins, E.T. Are All Experiences of Fit Created Equal? Two Paths to Persuasion. J. Consum. Psychol. 2013, 23, 301–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hong, J.; Lee, A.Y. Be Fit and Be Strong: Mastering Self-Regulation through Regulatory Fit. J. Consum. Res. 2008, 34, 682–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Inzlicht, M.; Kang, S.K. Stereotype Threat Spillover: How Coping with Threats to Social Identity Affects Aggression, Eating, Decision Making, and Attention. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 99, 467–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jamieson, J.P.; Harkins, S.G. Mere Effort and Stereotype Threat Performance Effects. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 93, 544–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Worthy, D.A.; Markman, A.B.; Maddox, W.T. What Is Pressure? Evidence for Social Pressure as a Type of Regulatory Focus. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2009, 16, 344–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lemaire, P.; Brun, F.; Régner, I. Negative Aging Stereotypes Disrupt Both the Selection and Execution of Strategies in Older Adults. Gerontology 2018, 64, 373–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Working Memory: State of the Science; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2021; pp. xvi, 437. ISBN 9780198842286.
- Barrouillet, P.; Bernardin, S.; Camos, V. Time Constraints and Resource Sharing in Adults’ Working Memory Spans. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2004, 133, 83–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barrouillet, P.; Bernardin, S.; Portrat, S.; Vergauwe, E.; Camos, V. Time and Cognitive Load in Working Memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2007, 33, 570–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barrouillet, P.; De Paepe, A.; Langerock, N. Time Causes Forgetting from Working Memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2012, 19, 87–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Vergauwe, E.; Barrouillet, P.; Camos, V. Visual and Spatial Working Memory Are Not That Dissociated after All: A Time-Based Resource-Sharing Account. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2009, 35, 1012–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Vergauwe, E.; Barrouillet, P.; Camos, V. Do Mental Processes Share a Domain-General Resource? Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 384–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Jordano, M.L.; Touron, D.R. Stereotype Threat as a Trigger of Mind-Wandering in Older Adults. Psychol. Aging 2017, 32, 307–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jordano, M.L.; Touron, D.R. Priming Performance-Related Concerns Induces Task-Related Mind-Wandering. Conscious. Cogn. 2017, 55, 126–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cadinu, M.; Maass, A.; Rosabianca, A.; Kiesner, J. Why Do Women Underperform under Stereotype Threat?: Evidence for the Role of Negative Thinking. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 572–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mrazek, M.D.; Chin, J.M.; Schmader, T.; Hartson, K.A.; Smallwood, J.; Schooler, J.W. Threatened to Distraction: Mind-Wandering as a Consequence of Stereotype Threat. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 47, 1243–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lu, A.; Feng, Y.; Yu, Z.; Tian, H.; Hong, X.; Zheng, D. Anxiety and Mind Wandering as Independent Consequences of Stereotype Threat. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 2015, 43, 537–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schuster, C.; Martiny, S.E.; Schmader, T. Distracted by the Unthought—Suppression and Reappraisal of Mind Wandering under Stereotype Threat. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0122207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Banks, J.B.; Boals, A. Understanding the Role of Mind Wandering in Stress-Related Working Memory Impairments. Cogn. Emot. 2017, 31, 1023–1030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kam, J.W.Y.; Handy, T.C. Differential Recruitment of Executive Resources during Mind Wandering. Conscious. Cogn. 2014, 26, 51–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Unsworth, N.; Robison, M.K. The Influence of Lapses of Attention on Working Memory Capacity. Mem. Cognit. 2016, 44, 188–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wraga, M.; Helt, M.; Jacobs, E.; Sullivan, K. Neural Basis of Stereotype-Induced Shifts in Women’s Mental Rotation Performance. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2007, 2, 12–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Forbes, C.E.; Leitner, J.B. Stereotype Threat Engenders Neural Attentional Bias toward Negative Feedback to Undermine Performance. Biol. Psychol. 2014, 102, 98–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barber, S.J.; Seliger, J.; Yeh, N.; Tan, S.C. Stereotype Threat Reduces the Positivity of Older Adults’ Recall. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2019, 74, 585–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Piroelle, M.; Abadie, M.; Régner, I. Toward a New Approach to Investigate the Role of Working Memory in Stereotype Threat Effects. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1647. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121647
Piroelle M, Abadie M, Régner I. Toward a New Approach to Investigate the Role of Working Memory in Stereotype Threat Effects. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(12):1647. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121647
Chicago/Turabian StylePiroelle, Margaux, Marlène Abadie, and Isabelle Régner. 2022. "Toward a New Approach to Investigate the Role of Working Memory in Stereotype Threat Effects" Brain Sciences 12, no. 12: 1647. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121647
APA StylePiroelle, M., Abadie, M., & Régner, I. (2022). Toward a New Approach to Investigate the Role of Working Memory in Stereotype Threat Effects. Brain Sciences, 12(12), 1647. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12121647