The Bidirectional in Bilingual: Cognitive, Social and Linguistic Effects of and on Third-Age Language Learning
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Individual Differences in Bilingual Experiences in Older Adulthood
2.1. Bilingual Language Usage
2.2. Individual Differences and Language Control
2.3. Individual Differences and The Environment
3. Language Learning as a Tool to Promote Healthy Aging in Older Adults
3.1. Short Summaries of Research Questions and Aims of the Studies
3.2. Participants and Group Composition of the Studies
3.3. Language Constellations
3.4. Teaching Methods and Teaching Intensity
3.5. Assessment of Language Proficiency and Language Learning Rate
3.6. Socio-Affective Measures
3.7. Cognitive Tasks and Cognitive Outcomes
4. Future Directions: Cognitive, Social and Linguistic Effects of and on Third Age Language Learning
4.1. Balancing Language, Cognition and Social Dimensions
4.2. Intensity and Type of the Language Intervention
4.3. The Issue Of Thresholds
4.4. Design and Method
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
MDPI and Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute |
DOAJ and Directory of open access journals |
References
- Amer, T.; Kalender, B.; Hasher, L.; Trehub, S.E.; Wong, Y. Do Older Professional Musicians Have Cognitive Advantages? PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e71630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wan, C.Y.; Schlaug, G. Music Making as a Tool for Promoting Brain Plasticity across the Life Span. Neuroscientist 2010, 16, 566–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Blankevoort, C.G.; Scherder, E.J.A.; Wieling, M.B.; Hortobágyi, T.; Brouwer, W.H.; Geuze, R.H.; van Heuvelen, M.J.G. Physical Predictors of Cognitive Performance in Healthy Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Analysis. PLoS ONE 2013, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swain, M.; Lapkin, S.; Deters, P. Exploring the Effect of Languaging Activities on Cognitive Functioning: The Case of an Older Adult in a Long-Term Care Facility. Act. Adapt. Aging 2013, 37, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pascual-Leone, A.; Amedi, A.; Fregni, F.; Merabet, L.B. The plastic human brain cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2005, 28, 377–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schlegel, A.A.; Rudelson, J.J.; Tse, P.U. White Matter Structure Changes as Adults Learn a Second Language. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012, 24, 1664–1670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Craik, F.I.M.; Bialystok, E.; Freedman, M. Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology 2010, 75, 1726–1729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Alladi, S.; Bak, T.H.; Duggirala, V.; Surampudi, B.; Shailaja, M.; Shukla, A.K.; Chaudhuri, J.R.; Kaul, S. Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology 2013, 81, 1938–1944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woumans, E.; Santens, P.; Sieben, A.; Versijpt, J.; Stevens, M.; Duyck, W. Bilingualism delays clinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2015, 18, 568–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mukadam, N.; Sommerlad, A.; Livingston, G. The Relationship of Bilingualism Compared to Monolingualism to the Risk of Cognitive Decline or Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2017, 58, 45–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, K.R.; Sawi, O. Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 962. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hilchey, M.D.; Klein, R.M. Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2011, 18, 625–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Houtzager, N. Bilingual Advantages in Middle-Aged and Elderly Populations. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Pot, A.; Keijzer, M.; de Bot, K.; Pot, A.; Keijzer, M.; de Bot, K. Intensity of Multilingual Language Use Predicts Cognitive Performance in Some Multilingual Older Adults. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, E.; Abutalebi, J.; Bak, T.H.; Burke, D.M.; Kroll, J.F. Aging in two languages: Implications for public health. Ageing Res. Rev. 2016, 27, 56–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Luk, G.; Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.; Grady, C. Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 16808–16813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Estanga, A.; Ecay-Torres, M.; Ibañez, A.; Izagirre, A.; Villanua, J.; Garcia-Sebastian, M.; Iglesias Gaspar, M.T.; Otaegui-Arrazola, A.; Iriondo, A.; Clerigue, M.; et al. Beneficial effect of bilingualism on Alzheimer’s disease CSF biomarkers and cognition. Neurobiol. Aging 2017, 50, 144–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bak, T.H.; Robertson, I. Biology enters the scene—A new perspective on bilingualism, cognition, and dementia. Neurobiol. Aging 2017, 50, iii–iv. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van den Noort, M.; Struys, E.; Bosch, P.; Jaswetz, L.; Perriard, B.; Yeo, S.; Barisch, P.; Vermeire, K.; Lee, S.H.; Lim, S. Does the Bilingual Advantage in Cognitive Control Exist and If So, What Are Its Modulating Factors? A Systematic Review. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Bruin, A. Not All Bilinguals Are the Same: A Call for More Detailed Assessments and Descriptions of Bilingual Experiences. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosjean, F. Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain Lang. 1989, 36, 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lehtonen, M.; Soveri, A.; Laine, A.; Järvenpää, J.; de Bruin, A.; Antfolk, J. Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 2018, 144, 394–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laine, M.; Lehtonen, M. Cognitive consequences of bilingualism: Where to go from here? Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 33, 1205–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E.; Sullivan, M.D. Growing Old with Two Languages; Volume 53, Studies in Bilingualism; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands; Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Bonfieni, M. The Bilingual Continuum: Mutual Effects of Language and Cognition. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Abutalebi, J.; Clahsen, H. Bilingualism, cognition, and aging. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2015, 18, 1–2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macnamara, B.N.; Conway, A.R.A. Novel evidence in support of the bilingual advantage: Influences of task demands and experience on cognitive control and working memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2014, 21, 520–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kavé, G.; Eyal, N.; Shorek, A.; Cohen-Mansfield, J. Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychol. Aging 2008, 23, 70–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ihle, A.; Oris, M.; Fagot, D.; Kliegel, M. The relation of the number of languages spoken to performance in different cognitive abilities in old age. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2016, 38, 1103–1114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeLuca, V.; Rothman, J.; Bialystok, E.; Pliatsikas, C. Redefining bilingualism as a spectrum of experiences that differentially affects brain structure and function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 7565–7574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Prat, C.S.; Just, M.A. Exploring the Neural Dynamics Underpinning Individual Differences in Sentence Comprehension. Cereb. Cortex 2011, 21, 1747–1760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stocco, A.; Prat, C.S. Bilingualism trains specific brain circuits involved in flexible rule selection and application. Brain Lang. 2014, 137, 50–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fricke, M.; Zirnstein, M.; Navarro-Torres, C.; Kroll, J.F. Bilingualism reveals fundamental variation in language processing. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, D.W.; Abutalebi, J. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 515–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Timmer, K.; Christoffels, I.K.; Costa, A. On the flexibility of bilingual language control: The effect of language context. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 22, 555–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Li, P.; Legault, J.; Litcofsky, K.A. Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex 2014, 58, 301–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grosjean, F. Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 1998, 1, 131–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sekerina, I.A.; Spradlin, L.; Valian, V. (Eds.) Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and Insights; Number Volume 57 in Studies in Bilingualism; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands; Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Ihle, A.; Oris, M.; Fagot, D.; Maggiori, C.; Kliegel, M. The association of educational attainment, cognitive level of job, and leisure activities during the course of adulthood with cognitive performance in old age: The role of openness to experience. Int. Psychogeriatrics 2016, 28, 733–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ihle, A.; Oris, M.; Sauter, J.; Spini, D.; Rimmele, U.; Maurer, J.; Kliegel, M. The relation of low cognitive abilities to low well-being in old age is attenuated in individuals with greater cognitive reserve and greater social capital accumulated over the life course. Aging Ment. Health 2018, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bugos, J.A.; Perlstein, W.M.; McCrae, C.S.; Brophy, T.S.; Bedenbaugh, P.H. Individualized Piano Instruction enhances executive functioning and working memory in older adults. Aging Ment. Health 2007, 11, 464–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mackey, A.; Sachs, R. Older Learners in SLA Research: A First Look at Working Memory, Feedback, and L2 Development: Older Learners in SLA Research. Lang. Learn. 2012, 62, 704–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ware, C.; Damnee, S.; Djabelkhir, L.; Cristancho, V.; Wu, Y.H.; Benovici, J.; Pino, M.; Rigaud, A.S. Maintaining Cognitive Functioning in Healthy Seniors with a Technology-Based Foreign Language Program: A Pilot Feasibility Study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2017, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kliesch, M.; Giroud, N.; Pfenninger, S.E.; Meyer, M. Research on Second Language Acquisition in Old Adulthood: What We Have and What We Need. In Third Age Learners of Foreign Languages; Gabryś-Barker, D., Ed.; Blue Ridge Summit; Multilingual Matters: Bristol, UK, 2017; pp. 48–75. [Google Scholar]
- Pfenninger, S.E.; Polz, S. Foreign language learning in the third age: A pilot feasibility study on cognitive, socio-affective and linguistic drivers and benefits in relation to previous bilingualism of the learner. J. Eur. Second. Lang. Assoc. 2018, 2, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bak, T.H.; Long, M.R.; Vega-Mendoza, M.; Sorace, A. Novelty, Challenge, and Practice: The Impact of Intensive Language Learning on Attentional Functions. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0153485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cox, J.G. Explicit instruction, bilingualism, and the older adult learner. Stud. Second. Lang. Acquis. 2017, 39, 29–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramos, S.; Fernández García, Y.; Antón, E.; Casaponsa, A.; Duñabeitia, J.A. Does learning a language in the elderly enhance switching ability? J. Neurolinguistics 2017, 43, 39–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Berggren, R.; Nilsson, J.; Brehmer, Y.; Schmiedek, F.; Lövdén, M. No evidence that foreign language learning in old age improves cognitive function: A randomized controlled study. Lang. Learn. Older Age 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gómez, D.R. Critical geragogy and foreign language learning: An exploratory application. Educ. Gerontol. 2016, 42, 136–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartsuiker, R.J. Why it is pointless to ask under which specific circumstances the bilingual advantage occurs. Cortex J. Devoted Study Nerv. Syst. Behav. 2015, 73, 336–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bice, K.; Kroll, J.F. English only? Monolinguals in linguistically diverse contexts have an edge in language learning. Brain Lang. 2019, 196, 104644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pliatsikas, C. Understanding structural plasticity in the bilingual brain: The Dynamic Restructuring Model. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2019, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marchman, V.A.; Bates, E. Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. J. Child Lang. 1994, 21, 339–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keijzer, M.; Bot, K. Unlearning and relearning of languages from childhood to late adulthood. In The Cambridge Handbook of Bilingualism; De Houwer, A., Ortega, L., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2019; pp. 267–286. [Google Scholar]
- Forlenza, O.V.; Valiengo, L.; Stella, F. Mood disorders in the elderly: Prevalence, functional impact, and management challenges. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2016, 12, 2105–2114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Study | Participants | Groups | Study Aims and Scope |
---|---|---|---|
n, (Gender), Age in Years | |||
Mackey & Sachs (2012) | 9 (4 m./5 f.) 65–89 (mean: 72) | no control group | Mackey & Sachs investigated whether verbal working memory, and phonological short-term memory predict improvement in L2 question formation of Spanish L1 older adults with different lengths of residency in the USA. The older adults participated English question formation in five training sessions spread over five weeks with a train ed L1 speaker and with a focus on interaction and feedback. |
Bak et al. (2016) | 77 (31 m./46 f.) 18–78 (mean: 49.2) | language learning group, active control group, passive control group | Bak et al. investigated how a 1-week intensive foreign language course in Scottish Gaelic, consisting of a total of 19.5 h of language teaching, changed the performance in auditory tests of attentional inhibition. They compared their participants across age groups (young adults, middle-aged, and older adults), and also compared the experimental group to active and passive control groups. In addition, they followed half of their experimental group and re-tested them nine months after the intensive language course with the same cognitive task battery. |
Cox (2017) | 43 (16 m./27 f.) 60–82 (mean: 68.87) | mono-/bilingual with/without explicit instruction | Cox investigated the influence of late Spanish bilingualism and explicit grammar instruction on the acquisition of basic morpho-syntax of Latin in English L1 speakers. The older adults underwent a short computer-based training of basic Latin in two sessions followed by post-test Latin assessments. |
Ramos et al. (2017) | 43 (22 m./21 f.) 60–80 (mean: 68.3) | language learning group, passive control group | Ramos et al. investigated the influence of a long-term (eight month) class-taught language course of Basque on the cognitive ability of switching in Spanish monolingual older adults. |
Ware et al. (2017) | 14 (5 m./9 f.) 63–90 (mean: 75.42) | no control group | Ware et al. focused on the effect of a 4-month foreign language course of English on the general cognitive state and well-being of French older adults with different proficiency levels of English. |
Kliesch et al. (2018) | 10 (6 m./4 f.) 65–74 (mean: 68.2) | no control group | Kliesch et al.’s pilot study examined which cognitive abilities and other factors (such as motivation, or time spent on self-study) best predict the learning rate of a foreign language in older adults. Their subjects were monolingual speakers of Austrian German and they underwent an intensive class-taught course of English. |
Pfenninger & Polz (2018) | 12 (4 m./ 8 f.) 63–89 (mean: 71.83) | mono-/bilingual | Pfenninger & Polz’s pilot study looked into the effects of an intensive English course on the abilities of inhibition, concentration and overall well-being of Austrian older adults. Furthermore, this study compared German monolinguals to sequential Slovenian-German bilinguals in order to investigate the effect of prior bilingualism. |
Berggren et al. (2018) | 160 (60 m./100 f.) 65–75 (mean: 69.35) | language learning group and active control group | Berggren et al. conducted a large-scale study investigating the influence of an 11-week foreign language course of Italian on general cognitive abilities of monolingual Swedish older adults. The study included an active control group undergoing yoga relaxation classes. |
Study | Language Training Method | Training Duration |
---|---|---|
Mackey & Sachs (2012) | communication sessions with trained L1 speakers | non-intensive & short |
(5 sessions during 5 weeks) | ||
total: estimated <10 h | ||
Bak et al. (2016) | classes taught by trained teachers | intensive & short |
total: 19.5 h (in 1 week) | ||
Cox (2017) | computer-based learning of | non-intensive & short |
basic language features | (2 sessions within a week) | |
total: estimated <10 h | ||
Ramos et al. (2017) | classes taught by trained teachers | semi-intensive & long |
(3.5 h/week for 8 months) | ||
total: estimated 110 h | ||
Ware et al. (2017) | course with teacher & technology supported | semi-intensive & long |
(2 h/week for 4 months) | ||
total: 16 h | ||
Kliesch et al. (2018) | classes taught by trained teachers | intensive & semi-long |
(4 h/day for 3 weeks) | ||
total: 60 h | ||
Pfenninger & Polz (2018) | classes taught by trained teachers | semi-intensive & short |
(6 h/week for 4 weeks) | ||
total: 24 h | ||
Berggren et al. (2018) | classes taught by trained teachers | semi-intensive & semi-long |
(5 h/week for 11 weeks) | ||
total: 55 h |
Study | Cognitive Tests | Language Training as Cognitive Boost? |
---|---|---|
Mackey & Sachs (2012) | verbal working memory (listening-span (LS) task) & phonological short-term memory (PSTM, non-word recall test) | not tested |
Bak et al. (2016) | Test of Everyday Attention different aspects of attention | yes, with significance (only for Elevator task with Reversal) |
Cox (2017) | Digit-Symbol Coding Task (processing speed under low cognitive demands) | not investigated |
Ramos et al. (2017) | Colour-Shape Switching Task | not significant |
Ware et al. (2017) | French version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), brief cognitive test of global cognitive functioning in older adults | not significant |
Kliesch et al. (2018) | a battery of nine standardized tests such as Stroop Task, Eriksen Flanker Task or Reading Span Task in order to assess the cognitive skills of inhibition, shifting, working memory, delayed recall, and verbal fluency | not tested |
Pfenninger & Polz (2018) | Stroop Task (verbal & non-verbal inhibition skills), concentration test for geriatric patients (Alters-Konzentrations-Test A-K-T, Gatterer, 1989; attention & concentration) | partially yes (Stroop task), with significance |
Berggren et al. (2018) | test battery of 10 items: 2 tests of spatial intelligence (Ravens matrices (Raven, 1960) & WASI-II Matrix Task (Wechsler, 1999)); 3 tests of verbal intelligence (Analogies, Syllogisms, and Verbal Inference; Ekstrom et al., 1976); 2 tests of working memory (Numerical updating, n-back); 3 tests of long-term associative memory & item memory using different types of stimuli (word-word, face-name, picture-picture) | not significant |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Pot, A.; Porkert, J.; Keijzer, M. The Bidirectional in Bilingual: Cognitive, Social and Linguistic Effects of and on Third-Age Language Learning. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 98. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9090098
Pot A, Porkert J, Keijzer M. The Bidirectional in Bilingual: Cognitive, Social and Linguistic Effects of and on Third-Age Language Learning. Behavioral Sciences. 2019; 9(9):98. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9090098
Chicago/Turabian StylePot, Anna, Joanna Porkert, and Merel Keijzer. 2019. "The Bidirectional in Bilingual: Cognitive, Social and Linguistic Effects of and on Third-Age Language Learning" Behavioral Sciences 9, no. 9: 98. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9090098
APA StylePot, A., Porkert, J., & Keijzer, M. (2019). The Bidirectional in Bilingual: Cognitive, Social and Linguistic Effects of and on Third-Age Language Learning. Behavioral Sciences, 9(9), 98. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9090098