A Global Survey on the Perception of Conservationists Regarding Animal Consciousness
Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Data Preparation
2.2. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Consciousness Perception Index
3.2. Factors That Influence the Perception Index
3.3. Perceptions of Respondents on Predefined Species
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Aristotle. On the Soul. Book II, Part 2; Smith, J.A., Translator; The Internet Classics Archive, 1994–2009; Available online: http://classics.mit.edu/Aristotle/soul.2.ii.html (accessed on 23 December 2024).
- Descartes, R. Discours de La Méthode. In Commentaire Historique Par Étienne Gilson; Vrin: Paris, France, 1987; ISBN 978-2-7116-0180-6. [Google Scholar]
- Franco, N.H. Animal Experiments in Biomedical Research: A Historical Perspective. Animals 2013, 3, 238–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hanlon, A.; Magalhães-Sant’Ana, M. Zoocentrism. In Encyclopedia of Global Bioethics; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2014; pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, I.J.H. The Changing Concept of Animal Sentience. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006, 100, 11–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Low, P.; Panksepp, J.; Reiss, D.; Edelman, D.; Van Swinderen, B.; Koch, C. The Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness. In Proceedings of the Francis Crick Memorial Conference, Cambridge, UK, 7 July 2012; Volume 7. [Google Scholar]
- The New York Declaration on Animal Consciousness. Available online: https://sites.google.com/nyu.edu/nydeclaration/declaration (accessed on 27 May 2024).
- Birch, J.; Schnell, A.K.; Clayton, N.S. Dimensions of Animal Consciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020, 24, 789–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Irwin, L.N.; Chittka, L.; Jablonka, E.; Mallatt, J. Editorial: Comparative Animal Consciousness. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 998421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dawkins, M.S. Animal Welfare with and without Consciousness. J. Zool. 2017, 301, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dawkins, M. Chapter Two. Animal Welfare and the Paradox of Animal Consciousness. Adv. Study Behav. 2015, 47, 5–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dung, L.; Newen, A. Profiles of Animal Consciousness: A Species-Sensitive, Two-Tier Account to Quality and Distribution. Cognition 2023, 235, 105409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Singer, P. The Expanding Circle: Ethics, Evolution, and Moral Progress; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Arluke, A.; Sanders, C. Regarding Animals; Animals, Culture, and Society; Temple University Press: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1996; ISBN 978-1-56639-440-6. [Google Scholar]
- Prokop, P.; Zvaríková, M.; Zvarík, M.; Ježová, Z.; Fedor, P. Charismatic Species Should Be Large: The Role of Admiration and Fear. People Nat. 2024, 6, 945–957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nguyen, T.; Malina, R.; Mokas, I.; Papakonstantinou, A.; Polyzos, O.; Vanhove, M.P.M. WASP: The World Archives of Species Perception. Database 2023, 2023, baad003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McMahon, C.R.; Harcourt, R.; Bateson, P.; Hindell, M.A. Animal Welfare and Decision Making in Wildlife Research. Biol. Conserv. 2012, 153, 254–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Draper, C.; Bekoff, M. Animal Welfare and the Importance of Compassionate Conservation—A Comment on McMahon et al. (2012). Biol. Conserv. 2013, 158, 422–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McMahon, C.R.; Harcourt, R.; Bateson, P.; Hindell, M.A. Animal Welfare and Conservation, the Debate We Must Have: A Response to Draper and Bekoff (2012). Biol. Conserv. 2013, 158, 424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McMahon, S.A.; Wigham, E. ‘All Ears’: A Questionnaire of 1516 Owner Perceptions of the Mental Abilities of Pet Rabbits, Subsequent Resource Provision, and the Effect on Welfare. Animals 2020, 10, 1730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bekoff, M. Cognitive Ethology and the Treatment of Non-Human Animals: How Matters of Mind Inform Matters of Welfare. Anim. Welf. 1994, 3, 75–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kılıç, İ.; Bozkurt, Z. The Relationship between Farmers’ Perceptions and Animal Welfare Standards in Sheep Farms. Asian-Australas. J. Anim. Sci. 2013, 26, 1329–1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tamioso, P.R.; Rucinque, D.S.; Miele, M.; Boissy, A.; Molento, C.F.M. Perception of Animal Sentience by Brazilian and French Citizens: The Case of Sheep Welfare and Sentience. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0200425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgs, M.J.; Bipin, S.; Cassaday, H.J. Man’s Best Friends: Attitudes towards the Use of Different Kinds of Animal Depend on Belief in Different Species’ Mental Capacities and Purpose of Use. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2020, 7, 191162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Knight, S.; Vrij, A.; Cherryman, J.; Nunkoosing, K. Attitudes towards Animal Use and Belief in Animal Mind. Anthrozoös 2004, 17, 43–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hawkins, R.D.; Williams, J.M. Children’s Beliefs about Animal Minds (Child-BAM): Associations with Positive and Negative Child–Animal Interactions. Anthrozoös 2016, 29, 503–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morris, P.; Knight, S.; Lesley, S. Belief in Animal Mind: Does Familiarity with Animals Influence Beliefs about Animal Emotions? Soc. Anim. 2012, 20, 211–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ceríaco, L.M. Human Attitudes towards Herpetofauna: The Influence of Folklore and Negative Values on the Conservation of Amphibians and Reptiles in Portugal. J. Ethnobiol. Ethnomedicine 2012, 8, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Landová, E.; Marešová, J.; Šimková, O.; Cikánová, V.; Frynta, D. Human Responses to Live Snakes and Their Photographs: Evaluation of Beauty and Fear of the King Snakes. J. Environ. Psychol. 2012, 32, 69–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herzog, H.A.; Betchart, N.S.; Pittman, R.B. Gender, Sex Role Orientation, and Attitudes toward Animals. Anthrozoös 1991, 4, 184–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Driscoll, J.W. Attitudes Toward Animal Use. Anthrozoös 1992, 5, 32–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kellert, S.R.; Berry, J.K. Attitudes, Knowledge, and Behaviors toward Wildlife as Affected by Gender. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 1973–2006 1987, 15, 363–371. [Google Scholar]
- Gauthier, J.; Kluge, A.G.; Rowe, T. Amniote Phylogeny and the Importance of Fossils. Cladistics 1988, 4, 105–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2023. [Google Scholar]
- Kleiber, C.; Zeileis, A. Applied Econometrics with R; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2008; Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=AER (accessed on 4 April 2024).
- Venables, W.N.; Ripley, B.D. Modern Applied Statistics with S, 4th ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2002; Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=MASS (accessed on 6 April 2024).
- Aarnio, J.; Aaltola, E. “Has an Ugly Caw”: The Moral Implications of How Hunting Organizations Depict Nonhuman Animals. Anthrozoös 2024, 37, 37–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reading, R.P.; Miller, B.J.; Kellert, S.R. Values and Attitudes Toward Prairie Dogs. Anthrozoös 1999, 12, 43–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eddy, T.J.; Gallup, G.G.; Povinelli, D.J. Attribution of Cognitive States to Animals: Anthropomorphism in Comparative Perspective. J. Soc. Issues 1993, 49, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harrison, M.A.; Hall, A.E. Anthropomorphism, Empathy, and Perceived Communicative Ability Vary with Phylogenetic Relatedness to Humans. J. Soc. Evol. Cult. Psychol. 2010, 4, 34–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pepperberg, I.M. The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Ezcurra, M.D.; Scheyer, T.M.; Butler, R.J. The Origin and Early Evolution of Sauria: Reassessing the Permian Saurian Fossil Record and the Timing of the Crocodile-Lizard Divergence. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e89165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Müller, J.J.A.; Massen, J.J.M.; Bugnyar, T.; Osvath, M. Ravens Remember the Nature of a Single Reciprocal Interaction Sequence over 2 Days and Even after a Month. Anim. Behav. 2017, 128, 69–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pika, S.; Sima, M.J.; Blum, C.R.; Herrmann, E.; Mundry, R. Ravens Parallel Great Apes in Physical and Social Cognitive Skills. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 20617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amodio, P.; Boeckle, M.; Schnell, A.K.; Ostojíc, L.; Fiorito, G.; Clayton, N.S. Grow Smart and Die Young: Why Did Cephalopods Evolve Intelligence? Trends Ecol. Evol. 2019, 34, 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chittka, L. Bee Cognition. Curr. Biol. 2017, 27, R1049–R1053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Odling-Smee, J.; Laland, K.N. Ecological Inheritance and Cultural Inheritance: What Are They and How Do They Differ? Biol. Theory 2011, 6, 220–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunnthorsdottir, A. Physical Attractiveness of an Animal Species as a Decision Factor for Its Preservation. Anthrozoös 2001, 14, 204–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Serpell, J.A. Factors Influencing Human Attitudes to Animals and Their Welfare. Anim. Welf. Potters Bar Then Wheathampstead 2004, 13, S145–S152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Güntürkün, O.; Koenen, C.; Iovine, F.; Garland, A.; Pusch, R. The Neuroscience of Perceptual Categorization in Pigeons: A Mechanistic Hypothesis. Learn Behav 2018, 46, 229–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arterberry, M.E.; Bornstein, M.H. Infant Perceptual and Conceptual Categorization: The Roles of Static and Dynamic Stimulus Attributes. Cognition 2002, 86, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, J.; Wermter, S.; Sun, F.; Zhang, C.; Engel, A.K.; Röder, B.; Fu, X.; Xue, G. Editorial: Cross-Modal Learning: Adaptivity, Prediction and Interaction. Front. Neurorobot. 2022, 16, 889911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhang, L.-Z.; Zhang, S.-W.; Wang, Z.-L.; Yan, W.-Y.; Zeng, Z.-J. Cross-Modal Interaction between Visual and Olfactory Learning in Apis Cerana. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2014, 200, 899–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laska, M. Olfactory Discrimination Learning in an Outbred and an Inbred Strain of Mice. Chem. Sens. 2015, 40, 489–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kurt, S.; Ehret, G. Auditory Discrimination Learning and Knowledge Transfer in Mice Depends on Task Difficulty. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 8481–8485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treviño, M.; Oviedo, T.; Jendritza, P.; Li, S.-B.; Köhr, G.; De Marco, R.J. Controlled Variations in Stimulus Similarity during Learning Determine Visual Discrimination Capacity in Freely Moving Mice. Sci. Rep. 2013, 3, 1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meagher, R.K.; Strazhnik, E.; Von Keyserlingk, M.A.G.; Weary, D.M. Assessing the Motivation to Learn in Cattle. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 6847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schumacher, J.W.; McCann, M.K.; Maximov, K.J.; Fitzpatrick, D. Selective Enhancement of Neural Coding in V1 Underlies Fine-Discrimination Learning in Tree Shrew. Curr. Biol. 2022, 32, 3245–3260.e5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cohen, Y.; Putrino, D.; Wilson, D.A. Dynamic Cortical Lateralization during Olfactory Discrimination Learning. J. Physiol. 2015, 593, 1701–1714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gibbons, M.; Versace, E.; Crump, A.; Baran, B.; Chittka, L. Motivational Trade-Offs and Modulation of Nociception in Bumblebees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2022, 119, e2205821119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adcock, S.J.J.; Tucker, C.B. Injury Alters Motivational Trade-Offs in Calves during the Healing Period. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 6888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Palagi, E.; Burghardt, G.M.; Smuts, B.; Cordoni, G.; Dall’Olio, S.; Fouts, H.N.; Řeháková-Petrů, M.; Siviy, S.M.; Pellis, S.M. Rough-and-tumble Play as a Window on Animal Communication. Biol. Rev. 2016, 91, 311–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roelofs, S.; Boleij, H.; Nordquist, R.E.; Van Der Staay, F.J. Making Decisions under Ambiguity: Judgment Bias Tasks for Assessing Emotional State in Animals. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bateson, M.; Nettle, D. Development of a Cognitive Bias Methodology for Measuring Low Mood in Chimpanzees. PeerJ 2015, 3, e998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DePasquale, C.; Sturgill, J.; Braithwaite, V.A. A Standardized Protocol for Preference Testing to Assess Fish Welfare. JoVE 2020, 60674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ortega, L.J.; Stoppa, K.; Güntürkün, O.; Troje, N.F. Limits of Intraocular and Interocular Transfer in Pigeons. Behav. Brain Res. 2008, 193, 69–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stein, B.E.; Stanford, T.R.; Rowland, B.A. Development of Multisensory Integration from the Perspective of the Individual Neuron. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014, 15, 520–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kavcic, V.; Fei, R.; Hu, S.; Doty, R.W. Hemispheric Interaction, Metacontrol, and Mnemonic Processing in Split-Brain Macaques. Behav. Brain Res. 2000, 111, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ünver, E.; Xiao, Q.; Güntürkün, O. Meta-Control in Pigeons (Columba Livia) and the Role of the Commissura Anterior. Symmetry 2019, 11, 124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gori, S.; Agrillo, C.; Dadda, M.; Bisazza, A. Do Fish Perceive Illusory Motion? Sci. Rep. 2014, 4, 6443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newsome, W.T.; Mikami, A.; Wurtz, R.H. Motion Selectivity in Macaque Visual Cortex. III. Psychophysics and Physiology of Apparent Motion. J. Neurophysiol. 1986, 55, 1340–1351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Landis, C.; Hamwi, V. The Effect of Certain Physiological Determinants on the Flicker-Fusion Threshold. J. Appl. Physiol. 1954, 6, 566–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lisney, T.J.; Ekesten, B.; Tauson, R.; Håstad, O.; Ödeen, A. Using Electroretinograms to Assess Flicker Fusion Frequency in Domestic Hens Gallus Gallus Domesticus. Vis. Res. 2012, 62, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benjamin, M.M.; Shaker, M.; Rabbat, M.G. Chapter 5—Assessing Coronary Artery Disease Using Coronary Computed Tomography Angiography. In Cardiovascular and Coronary Artery Imaging; El-Baz, A.S., Suri, J.S., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2022; pp. 129–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dale, R.; Plotnik, J.M. Elephants Know When Their Bodies Are Obstacles to Success in a Novel Transfer Task. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 46309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- DeGrazia, D. Self-Awareness in Animals. In The Philosophy of Animal Minds; Lurz, R.W., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2009; pp. 201–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallup, G.G. Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition. Science 1970, 167, 86–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Suddendorf, T.; Corballis, M.C. The Evolution of Foresight: What Is Mental Time Travel, and Is It Unique to Humans? Behav. Brain Sci. 2007, 30, 299–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cheke, L.G.; Clayton, N.S. Eurasian Jays (Garrulus glandarius) Overcome Their Current Desires to Anticipate Two Distinct Future Needs and Plan for Them Appropriately. Biol. Lett. 2012, 8, 171–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Szabo, B.; Noble, D.W.A.; Whiting, M.J. Context-Specific Response Inhibition and Differential Impact of a Learning Bias in a Lizard. Anim. Cogn. 2019, 22, 317–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vlamings, P.H.J.M.; Hare, B.; Call, J. Reaching around Barriers: The Performance of the Great Apes and 3–5-Year-Old Children. Anim. Cogn. 2010, 13, 273–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barth, J.; Call, J. Tracking the Displacement of Objects: A Series of Tasks with Great Apes (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo pygmaeus) and Young Children (Homo sapiens). J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 2006, 32, 239–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, J.R.; Elsworth, J.D.; Roth, R.H.; Sladek, J.R.; Redmond, D.E. Cognitive and Motor Deficits in the Acquisition of an Object Retrieval/Detour Task in MPTP-Treated Monkeys. Brain 1990, 113, 617–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beran, M.J. Maintenance of Self-Imposed Delay of Gratification by Four Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and an Orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). J. Gen. Psychol. 2002, 129, 49–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anderson, J.R.; Kuroshima, H.; Fujita, K. Delay of Gratification in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) and Squirrel Monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). J. Comp. Psychol. 2010, 124, 205–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stevens, J.R.; Hallinan, E.V.; Hauser, M.D. The Ecology and Evolution of Patience in Two New World Monkeys. Biol. Lett. 2005, 1, 223–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Evans, T.A.; Westergaard, G.C. Self-Control and Tool Use in Tufted Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella). J. Comp. Psychol. 2006, 120, 163–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Evans, T.A.; Beran, M.J. Delay of Gratification and Delay Maintenance by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca Mulatta). J. Gen. Psychol. 2007, 134, 199–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shokur, S.; O’Doherty, J.E.; Winans, J.A.; Bleuler, H.; Lebedev, M.A.; Nicolelis, M.A.L. Expanding the Primate Body Schema in Sensorimotor Cortex by Virtual Touches of an Avatar. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 15121–15126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wada, M.; Takano, K.; Ora, H.; Ide, M.; Kansaku, K. The Rubber Tail Illusion as Evidence of Body Ownership in Mice. J. Neurosci. 2016, 36, 11133–11137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vasconcelos, M. Transitive Inference in Non-Human Animals: An Empirical and Theoretical Analysis. Behav. Process. 2008, 78, 313–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hill, H.; Dietrich, S.; Cadena, A.; Raymond, J.; Cheves, K. More than a Fluke: Lessons Learned from a Failure to Replicate the False Belief Task in Dolphins. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2018, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krupenye, C.; Call, J. Theory of Mind in Animals: Current and Future Directions. WIRES Cogn. Sci. 2019, 10, e1503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kano, F.; Krupenye, C.; Hirata, S.; Tomonaga, M.; Call, J. Great Apes Use Self-Experience to Anticipate an Agent’s Action in a False-Belief Test. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 20904–20909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Connell, S.; Dunbar, R.I.M. A Test for Comprehension of False Belief in Chimpanzees. Evol. Cogn. 2003, 9, 131–140. [Google Scholar]
- Foote, A.L.; Crystal, J.D. Metacognition in the Rat. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 551–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Seed, A.; Byrne, R. Animal Tool-Use. Curr. Biol. 2010, 20, R1032–R1039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Striedter, G.F. Bird Brains and Tool Use: Beyond Instrumental Conditioning. Brain Behav. Evol. 2013, 82, 55–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moye, T.B.; Rudy, J.W. Ontogenesis of Trace Conditioning in Young Rats: Dissociation of Associative and Memory Processes. Dev. Psychobiol. 1987, 20, 405–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bublitz, A.; Weinhold, S.R.; Strobel, S.; Dehnhardt, G.; Hanke, F.D. Reconsideration of Serial Visual Reversal Learning in Octopus (Octopus vulgaris) from a Methodological Perspective. Front. Physiol. 2017, 8, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buttelmann, D.; Carpenter, M.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. Enculturated Chimpanzees Imitate Rationally. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, F31–F38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bates, L.A.; Byrne, R.W. Imitation: What Animal Imitation Tells Us about Animal Cognition. WIRES Cogn. Sci. 2010, 1, 685–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galizio, M.; Bruce, K.E. Abstraction, Multiple Exemplar Training and the Search for Derived Stimulus Relations in Animals. Perspect. Behav. Sci. 2018, 41, 45–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin-Malivel, J.; Fagot, J. Cross-Modal Integration and Conceptual Categorization in Baboons. Behav. Brain Res. 2001, 122, 209–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Labash, A.; Aru, J.; Matiisen, T.; Tampuu, A.; Vicente, R. Perspective Taking in Deep Reinforcement Learning Agents. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huber, L.; Lonardo, L. Canine Perspective-Taking. Anim. Cogn. 2023, 26, 275–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Yakhlef, V.; Magalhães-Sant’Ana, M.; Pereira, A.L.; Azevedo, A. A Global Survey on the Perception of Conservationists Regarding Animal Consciousness. Animals 2025, 15, 341. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15030341
Yakhlef V, Magalhães-Sant’Ana M, Pereira AL, Azevedo A. A Global Survey on the Perception of Conservationists Regarding Animal Consciousness. Animals. 2025; 15(3):341. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15030341
Chicago/Turabian StyleYakhlef, Valentine, Manuel Magalhães-Sant’Ana, Ana Luísa Pereira, and Alexandre Azevedo. 2025. "A Global Survey on the Perception of Conservationists Regarding Animal Consciousness" Animals 15, no. 3: 341. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15030341
APA StyleYakhlef, V., Magalhães-Sant’Ana, M., Pereira, A. L., & Azevedo, A. (2025). A Global Survey on the Perception of Conservationists Regarding Animal Consciousness. Animals, 15(3), 341. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15030341