Next Article in Journal
A Comparative Study of Dog- and Cat-Induced Injury on Incidence and Risk Factors among Children
Next Article in Special Issue
Determinants, Health Problems, and Food Insecurity in Urban Areas of the Largest City in Cape Verde
Previous Article in Journal
Dietary Patterns in Relation to General and Central Obesity among Adults in Southwest China
Previous Article in Special Issue
Noise Annoyance in Urban Children: A Cross-Sectional Population-Based Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Opinion

Transforming Life: A Broad View of the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease Concept from an Ecological Justice Perspective

by
Susan L. Prescott
1,2,* and
Alan C. Logan
1,3
1
International Inflammation (in-FLAME) Network, Worldwide Universities Network (WUN), 35 Stirling Hwy, Crawley 6009, Australia
2
School of Paediatrics and Child Health Research, University of Western Australia, P.O. Box D184, Princess Margaret Hospital, Perth 6001, Australia
3
PathLight Synergy, 23679 Calabassas Road, Suite 542, Calabassas, CA 91302, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13(11), 1075; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph13111075
Submission received: 31 August 2016 / Revised: 21 October 2016 / Accepted: 28 October 2016 / Published: 3 November 2016
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Urban Place and Health Equity)

Abstract

:
The influential scientist Rene J. Dubos (1901–1982) conducted groundbreaking studies concerning early-life environmental exposures (e.g., diet, social interactions, commensal microbiota, housing conditions) and adult disease. However, Dubos looked beyond the scientific focus on disease, arguing that “mere survival is not enough”. He defined mental health as fulfilling human potential, and expressed concerns about urbanization occurring in tandem with disappearing access to natural environments (and elements found within them); thus modernity could interfere with health via “missing exposures”. With the advantage of emerging research involving green space, the microbiome, biodiversity and positive psychology, we discuss ecological justice in the dysbiosphere and the forces—financial inequity, voids in public policy, marketing and otherwise—that interfere with the fundamental rights of children to thrive in a healthy urban ecosystem and learn respect for the natural environment. We emphasize health within the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) rubric and suggest that greater focus on positive exposures might uncover mechanisms of resiliency that contribute to maximizing human potential. We will entrain our perspective to socioeconomic disadvantage in developed nations and what we have described as “grey space”; this is a mental as much as a physical environment, a space that serves to insidiously reinforce unhealthy behavior, compromise positive psychological outlook and, ultimately, trans-generational health. It is a dwelling place that cannot be fixed with encephalobiotics or the drug-class known as psychobiotics.

1. Introduction: Mere Survival Is Not Enough

But the quality of the environment cannot be measured only in terms of gross defects such as air, water, or food pollution. Environmental conditions experienced early in life (including the formative months before birth) cause the most profound and lasting changes in man...the maintenance of biological and mental health requires that technological societies provide in some form the biological freedom enjoyed by our Paleolithic ancestors.
Rene Dubos, 1970 [1]
Renowned microbiologist and environmentalist Rene Dubos was a pioneer in the “early origins” concept of health and disease. Throughout the 1960s he and colleagues published multiple studies demonstrating the long-lasting influences of maternal stress, nutritional deficiencies and gut microbial alterations on offspring health within various rodent models. In addition to his classic 1966 paper “Biological Freudianism: lasting effects of early environmental influences” (reprinted in his honor within a more recent issue of the International Journal of Epidemiology) [2], much of this “early origins” bench work is summarized in his book Man Adapting [3].
There is little doubt that Dubos and his colleagues paved mechanistic roads to the contemporary developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) concept. Although Dubos connected detrimental early-life environmental influences to subsequent disease, he was equally concerned with the ways in which the progressive loss of (evolutionary-rooted) environmental exposures could erode health in absentia. Included in more passive or less obvious aspects of what might be “missing” was diminished exposure to natural environments, including unseen aspects such as microbes. He proposed that in humans, socioeconomic status was a likely determinant of the “intensity of the inflammatory response to the so-called ‘normal’ flora” in the context of dietary practices, commensal microbiota and food utilization [3].

2. Roadmap to the Current Review

Here, we expand upon the arguments of Dubos in the context of DOHaD, urbanicity and socioeconomic disadvantage. While the absence of disease allows us to survive, we argue that preventing disease is not the ultimate goal of DOHaD. We emphasize the word health within the DOHaD rubric and suggest that greater focus on positive (beneficial) chemical and non-chemical, biotic and abiotic, family, neighborhood and societal “exposures” might uncover mechanisms of resiliency that go far beyond disease prevention and extend into health. What’s more, we propose that layering positive “exposures” during all critical developmental stages will have a synergistic effect such that the sum of positive pathways to health might be more than that of their individual parts.
In our perspective review, we build upon the speculations of Dubos with the advantage of rapidly-evolving research exploring the role of positive psychology, biodiversity and natural environments on human health, and, of course, the microbiome revolution that continues to underscore the critical role of early-life microbial exposure in long-term health. We highlight the emerging research on positive emotions (rather than exclusively negative emotions typical of the study of depression and/or anxiety) and their associations with immune function, stress physiology and non-communicable disease (NCD) risk [4,5,6]. Further, we explore the rapidly-evolving and no less interconnected realm of nutrition and mental health [7].
Accepting the fundamental truth that there is no health without mental health (defined as the ability to fulfill human potential), we use the vantage of mental health to allow for a vision of equitable urban environments. We will attempt to sew together seemingly disconnected threads of research to make our argument that the interconnectivity of life (biodiversity) and lifestyle (bios), cannot be separated from the causes and consequences of the social policies and practices that either promote or prevent complete mental health. The entire discussion tales place in the context of attaining fundamental rights to achieve one’s potential; equity of the ecosystem.

3. Ecological Justice as a Basic Human Right

In referring to ecology, and our later discussion of “ecological justice”, we do so from a perspective in which ecology (“eco” from Greek roots οἶκος or oikos; house/dwelling place; hence study of the dwelling place) encompasses all lines between internal ecosystems, a single city residence occupied by an individual, through community neighborhoods, and on to surrounding municipal and national dwellings (which house the houses), and outward to our large, universally shared n = 1 residence of Earth. Put simply, the way in which microbes sit on a single intestinal villus might contribute to health is not unrelated to the larger ecosystems that impact its human host. For example, there is much talk of the microbiome, but little discussion of the ways in which the policies and practices in the dwelling place of corporate boardrooms (e.g., profits), or the houses of government (e.g., policy or absence of policy concerning corporate practice), insinuate themselves into microbial ecology.
At the outset we frame this ecological vantage by reminding the reader that according to the United Nations, it is a fundamental right of every child to be supported in the development of “talents and mental and physical abilities to their fullest potential [and] respect for the natural environment” [8]. Thus, the interconnectedness of life and lifestyle cannot be removed from dialogue and action toward achieving and maintaining fundamental rights of childhood development. Any policy or practice that compromises the fulfillment of potential (i.e., health) and the development of respect for the natural environment (i.e., biodiversity) is therefore at direct odds with childhood rights. From our perspective, the microbiome revolution has opened a Pandora’s Box for any remaining attempts to place sole responsibility on individuals or communities for the lifestyles that compromise life.

4. Defining Grey Space and the Green Distinction

We entrain our DOHaD perspective to what we have previously described as urban “grey space“, areas that may include disproportionately higher industrial operations, commercial activity and major transportation routes, with resultant noise stress and excess light at night (LAN). Residential proximity to higher levels of grey space and less equitable access to biodiversity (a product of so-called “green” vegetation-rich and “blue” waterside areas) are an often-overlapping burden in disadvantaged populations (Figure 1). However, our definition of grey space is not based on specific units of analysis, single indicators and/or jargon related to quantitative toxin-based environmental impact assessments. These can obscure the ability to understand the sum of the total reality endured by the socioeconomic status SES disadvantaged [9].
The disproportionately higher presence of bars, liquor stores, convenience stores, fast-food outlets, and tobacco vendors in disadvantaged areas help define the grey aspects of place; however, it is more than mere structural presence. Toxin and remote sensing indicators cannot capture the profit-driven marketing, billboards, sidewalk signage, in-store magnification of unhealthy products and targeted screen media delivery that make grey space an entirely different “mental” environment (Figure 2). As we will argue, these aspects of grey space serve to insidiously reinforce unhealthy behavior, compromise positive psychological outlook and, ultimately, trans-generational health.
Importantly, in our later discussions of green space, natural environments and the potential health benefits of maintaining psychological connections to nature (i.e., nature relatedness), we underscore that ours is not an attempt to privilege or romanticize a notion of a “pure“, “green“ way of living that sits in a neat mirror image of grey space and urbanicity. Undoubtedly, the urban built environment, mixed with green and grey as it may be, can promote or detract from health in ways that are predominated by socioeconomic factors. Ours is not necessarily a “back to nature“ call. However, if we can succeed in our attempts to demonstrate that a more equitable distribution of access to quality natural environments, fresh healthy, minimally-processed foods and the “assets“ of positive psychology are part of ecological justice—capable of narrowing the SES health gap—then perhaps ours is a “forward to nature“ message.

5. Broadening Developmental Origins of Disease Paradigms

DOHaD research has centered upon three potentially-overlapping early-life environmental influences that have been linked to subsequent disease: nutrition (that is, extremes of undernutrition or, conversely, overnutrition via energy dense-nutrient-poor dietary patterns), stress (including social adversity), and the burden of environmental toxicants [10,11,12]. These exposures can act alone or in concert during specific windows of developmental plasticity. Allostatic load (wear-and-tear, cellular damage of physiological dysregulation; e.g., sustained elevations in stress hormones and/or inflammatory immune chemicals) and epigenetic mechanisms (e.g., methylation of the CpG dinucleotide in DNA, acetylation and methylation of histones, and the binding of small non-coding RNAs to DNA) can manifest as disease and dysfunction over the life course.
There has been less focus on the upstream ecological drivers of DOHaD mechanistic pathways. That is, politics and policies. Less discussion of the impact of missing exposures (e.g., biodiversity (microbial and macroecological) and positive emotions discussed later). The focus on “preventing disease“ has obscured the more expansive goal of “promoting health“ in the broadest sense of achieving human potential.
Notwithstanding the importance of specific developmental windows, in our view there are no specific life-course timeframes that determine where developmental “origins“ begin and end; paternal stress may influence sperm microRNA which, in turn, could influence offspring depression and anxiety [13]. Overall, trans-generational research has made it clear that ancestral trauma and stress are relevant to where the line of origin might begin [14,15]. DOHaD is a discussion for all.

6. Mental Health, Societal Health: Avoiding Mother Blame

There has been a consistent reporting of increased psychological distress in most (8/11) global surveys over the last several decades [16]. While global “epidemics” of diagnosable major depressive disorders (MDD) and anxiety disorders remain a matter of debate, collectively these data indicate that psychological distress is at unacceptably high levels among adults and youth in westernized nations [17,18]. Moreover, increasing perceptions of stress with modernity may be manifesting as somatic complaints [19,20].
Although 1/3 of adults presenting to primary care have symptoms of depression, anxiety, and/or alcohol problems, only a minority (<8%) articulate these overtly as their primary complaint [21]. Subthreshold anxiety and depression are increasingly common in youth and represent a significant public health concern in developed nations [22,23,24], often matching the levels of psychological distress and diminished perceptions of health as defined disorders [25,26]. Importantly, there are noteworthy global trends of a consistent generational drift away from empathic perspective taking and toward narcissism—relevant to our discussion on empathy and positive psychology at some length later [27].
In the developmental context, maternal depression and anxiety in pregnancy have wide ranging implications for the long term burden of disease of the next generation, and for society. For example, emotional distress may provoke unhealthy dietary patterns [28] and higher risk of preterm birth and low birth weight [29,30], which are in turn associated with higher risk for both clinical and subsyndromal mental disorders in offspring [31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42]. Importantly, subthreshold forms of depression can be identified in preschool children and represent a robust marker of subsequent clinical depression [43]. Youth with subthreshold symptoms are on a higher risk trajectory toward both adult mental health disorders [44] and other non-communicable disease (NCD) with associated co-morbidity and diminished life quality later in life [45,46].
Anticipating that additional investigation will only heighten the already existing mass of research linking maternal/paternal depressive symptoms and lifestyle with offspring weight gain, early-life affect/emotional regulation and altered parent-child dynamics [47,48], we must surely ask what ecological factors drive depression or, conversely, positive emotions in the first place? Upstream discourse is necessary because communication surrounding DOHaD runs the risk of holding an individual woman as the sole proprietor of responsibility (and therefore, subject of “mother-blame”) concerning fetal and early-life health [49]. Moreover, this risk in communication can extended to inferences that entire disadvantaged communities are not doing enough—i.e., “community blame” [50]; meanwhile, the more powerful forces—e.g., profits, elitism, neoliberal ideology—[51,52,53] that actually push against individual and community-level lifestyle (e.g., diet) are left out of the discourse.
Society at large (its policies and bio-eco-psychological influences [54]) contribute to the nurturing of each individual fetus, infant, child and adolescent. It does so vis-à-vis its ability to provide a supportive environment for pre-conception health, maternal and paternal health, and that of adult caregivers of any age. It does so by supporting the community in its efforts in providing the best possible “neighborhood womb“ (Figure 2). Although there may be critical developmental windows, privileging DOHaD communication to narrow time frames may also run the risk of inferring that societal policies and practices (those facilitating the presence of chemical/non-chemical toxins/stress and absence of positive exposures) are acceptable once a child is 3, or that they are of less relevance through youth development and for men and women planning to have a child.

7. The Prism of Socioeconomic Gradient

Not only are mental disorders significantly more common in urban regions of Western nations [55,56], the also they sit on an SES gradient [57,58] with depression and fatigue particularly associated with social disadvantage [59]. Disparities in mental health are also evident in SES disadvantaged children and youth [60], most striking in developed nations where inequities are greatest [61]. Mental disorders can contribute further socioeconomic hardship [62] and reduce resiliency. In a large Canadian community study, full recovery from depression was predicted by being Caucasian, affluent and without a significant history of childhood adversities [63]. On the other hand, positive emotions (e.g., optimism and others to be discussed in detail later) are emerging as assets of resiliency that are consistently linked with socioeconomic advantage [4].
Preterm delivery and suboptimal birth weight are also more likely to occur in disadvantaged populations [64,65]. In turn, even moderate preterm (32–36 weeks) birth multiplies the independent risk of low SES on behavioral and emotional problems in children [66]. Thus, supporting healthy pregnancy outcomes represents a salient and early way to begin to dismantle inequalities and transform life.
DOHaD as a collective group may be undervaluing its role in taking some of the valuable fruits of its labor—mechanistic science—toward ecological justice and public health advocacy. Moreover, in the hunt for mechanisms of disease, it may overlook broad social science research. For example, given known connections between parental mental health and offspring health, emerging research connecting relatively small wage increases with subsequent reductions in depression (in SES disadvantaged workers) [67,68] should be of high-level interest to DOHaD. Lack of a living wage magnifies the attraction of unhealthy lifestyle (e.g., dietary choices) and alters opportunity to spend time in natural environments (e.g., time constraints of working additional hours or multiple jobs), even if such areas were equitably distributed.

8. Lifestyle Factors: The Vicious Cycle of Ill-Health

Accumulating evidence suggests that modernity and rapid global urbanization (and more specifically, many of its policies and practices described later in the “Grey Space” section) is encouraging a lifestyle at odds with our evolutionary past. For the nearly 3-million-year history of our genus we have maintained a physically active lifestyle, consumed minimally processed foods and lived entrained to the light-dark cycles. The links between this ancestrally-discordant lifestyle and most modern NCDs is apparent [69]; thus, to some extent, increasingly prevalent NCDs may be described as “ancestral mismatch disorders“.
From the mental health perspective, nutrition is now proven to be an essential consideration in short and long-term neuro-emotional health, particularly in positive mental outlook [7,70]. At the population level, adherence to dietary patterns with relatively lower amounts of highly-processed foods (i.e., traditional patterns known to be less inflammatory) is associated with lowered risk of depressive symptoms of anxiety and depression [71,72]. From the DOHaD perspective, perinatal dietary quality has been linked to good mental health in offspring, and early life nutritional quality with academic performance [73,74,75].
The palatability of the engineered, ultra-processed Western diet is high, an attribute that may temporarily lower stress and improve mood [76]. Thus, this dietary pattern may be reinforced through “self-medication“ [77]. In experimental studies, withdrawal from this palatable, western diet leads to notable changes in gene expression governing stress physiology [76]. Simply put, weaning off such diets appear to be stressful; research on the stress-attenuating effects of sucrose [78] and mood-lifting effects of fat [79] supports this notion in humans. Moreover, it is easy to imagine how difficult this might be when the efforts are hampered by contextual psychosocial stress and a “grey space“ environment that cannot be fully replicated in rodent studies.
Relevant to our later discussions specific to SES disadvantage, research shows that humans often increase their consumption of calorie-dense, nutritionally-poor “comfort foods“ when confronted with psychological stress [80]. In a vicious cycle, the draw towards unhealthy foods is associated with chronic depressive symptoms and psychological distress. Like many lifestyle variables, nutritional quality is intertwined with sleep; low fiber and high saturated fat and sugar intake is associated with lighter, less restorative sleep with more arousals [81].
Adequate and appropriately timed sleep is essential to health and quality of life throughout the life course. Sleep disturbances have been linked to increased risks of cesarean delivery and preterm birth [82]. Sleep, circadian and biological rhythm disruptions are linked to depressive symptoms during and after pregnancy [83,84]. Antenatal paternal depression has also been noted in association with sleep problems [85]. Parental depression has, in turn, been associated with disturbances in infant sleep patterns [86]. Adequate sleep during childhood is emerging as a critical determinant of resiliency (i.e., achieving WHO health) and diminished risk of behavioral problems [87,88].
Screen time has been linked to obesity and mental health problems in children and adolescents. These associations may carry forward into adulthood [89,90]. Minimizing screen time and increasing outdoor play time in very young children may have overlapping benefit on sleep quality [91]. From an academics perspective, higher levels of screen time are associated with lower academic achievement [92]. In addition, greater dependence upon texting and media use has been associated with delay discounting (discussed below) [93]. Screen time as it relates to diminished psychological well-being clearly interacts with physical activity, sleep quality, and social support [94,95], yet it may also be an independent variable in the risk of depression [96].

9. Disadvantage and the Cognitive Tax

Neighborhood-level disadvantage is associated with NCDs and an increased likelihood of experiencing poor health at a much younger age over the life course [97,98]. Of particular importance to DOHaD, lower perceived neighborhood quality (reported by minority women in metro Detroit, USA) is associated with higher depressive symptoms and stress during pregnancy [99]. Perception matters: perceived neighborhood walkability is associated with far higher weekly physical activity and higher perceived availability of healthy foods (vs. lowest) is associated with almost 50% higher intakes of fruits and vegetables [100].
Although convenient (and conveniently common) to consider individual self-control as a primary pathway to NCD reduction and developmental disease resiliency via adherence to healthy lifestyle, the environmental forces pushing down on the bar of self-control (e.g., advertizing and others to be discussed) can be unyielding [53,101]. In addition to the direct influences of neighborhood grey space and marketing forces that push unhealthy lifestyles by default, it is worth noting that cognitive and motivational barriers to individual behavioral change can be a product of both disadvantage itself (e.g., poverty) and the NCDs that are highly associated with disadvantage (e.g., obesity or depression).
First, poverty-based concerns are an omnipresent tax on finite mental resources [102]. This has been demonstrated in laboratory and field studies conducted in North America and India. Focus on monetary concerns appears to diminish cognitive resources that could otherwise be directed to other problems or maintenance of healthy lifestyle habits. In terms of effect size, the deficit of cognitive resources induced by poverty-associated financial concerns is akin to that found when sleep researchers deprive subjects of a full night of sleep or a loss of 13 intelligence quotient IQ points [103].
Second, it is easy to visualize how societal pressures and the neighborhood-level grey space environment might contribute to feelings of powerlessness and impaired optimism. Experimental induction of merely feeling poor has been shown to increase caloric consumption [104]. Further, when researchers set up a game of with a rigged social outcome (i.e., subjects induced to feel of low social status with resultant decreased feelings of pride and powerfulness) young minority participants consume significantly more calories and a higher proportion of their daily calorie needs (in the ad libitum buffet meal vs. subjects manipulated to high social status) [105].
In sedentary adults the motivation to engage in physical activity is low, and the normal post-exercise lift in mood is often not experienced. For example, in those with NCDs such as type-2 diabetes, depression and/or obesity (vs. healthy/normal weight controls), motivation is a primary barrier to physical activity [106]. The same cognitive load carried by disadvantaged populations may also impair motivation to exercise simply by influencing motivation. Mental fatigue primed prior to or during exercise via cognitively demanding tasks can increase perceived exhaustion during and after exercise [107,108].
The line between initiation (short-term adoption) and long-term adherence of voluntary physical activity appears to be drawn by identified behavioral regulation (e.g., consideration of potential beneficial outcomes to self) and intrinsic motivations (e.g., inherent enjoyment of the activity) [109,110]. Thus, consistent cognitive demands, delay discounting, anticipation of fatigue/exhaustion and low levels of exercise enjoyment can compromise engagement in routine physical activity. Moreover, sleep deprivation and circadian disruptions—another burden carried by the disadvantaged—can also have negative influences on physical and mental performance during exercise [111].
Studies demonstrate that simple awareness of the benefits of exercise may not be enough for patients with NCDs; perceived exertion is higher, pleasure ratings and energy levels lower [112,113,114]. This, or course, impairs motivation to engage in subsequent exercise. On the other hand, when positive emotions are in tandem with the experience of exercise and recovery, future participation is more likely [115,116].

10. Lifestyle and Delay (Temporal) Discounting: Future Rewards?

Research pertaining to delay (also called temporal) discounting may be one of the most oft-overlooked topics in chronic NCD and DOHaD discussions; briefly, delay discounting refers to the willingness to postpone (delay) immediate rewards in favor of potentially larger benefits in the future. Discounting the potential value of future rewards, and instead prioritizing smaller immediate rewards, is referred to as steep discounting. Steep discounting is often code for diminished consideration of future consequences and impulsivity, both of which are linked with depression, obesity and unhealthy lifestyle choices [117,118]. For example, higher concern for future consequences is associated with preventive care, optimism, positive affect, lower aggression, interpersonal wellbeing and higher life satisfaction [119].
Steeper gradient of discounting future rewards is also associated with socioeconomic disadvantage [120,121,122]. Cognitive load, stress and diminished mental outlook and even (those outside conscious awareness) can magnify delay discounting [123,124]. Of importance to our grey space theme, physical aspects of the built environment such as fast food outlets may increase delay discounting in stealth ways. For example, discounting the value of a future financial reward and opting for smaller immediate gains is more likely while answering questions in the vicinity of a fast-food outlet (vs. other food establishments). Moreover, in neighborhoods with higher concentrations of fast food outlets, respondents are also more likely to opt for smaller immediate rewards, wave off larger future gains and have diminished levels of savoring (i.e., ability to take notice of positive experiences, an asset linked to mental well-being) [125,126].
On the other hand, images of (or actually being in) vegetation-dense green space have been shown to curb delay discounting in several studies [127,128,129]. It is also encouraging that even subtle reminders of prospect—simple outlooks toward the future via prospective imagery and stimulation of general future experiences—can help to level the discounting gradient [130,131]. Sleep quality appears to be essential for broad consideration of future consequences. It helps keep impulsivity in check and allows an individual to envision the efforts required to obtain future rewards as being less burdensome [132,133,134,135].

11. Positive Emotions and States: Neglected Discussion

While there has been much focus on the detrimental effects of maternal/early-life negative emotions and states, the independent links between positive emotions and health has largely escaped discourse. Positive and negative emotions are not opposite and are only correlated at a modest level [136]. Positive affect has been associated with good sleep [137] and healthy pregnancy outcomes [138]. In 28-year prospective research, infant positive affect (parent rated at baseline) uniquely predicts adult life satisfaction, workplace hope and optimism [139]. Early adolescent positive affect also predicts healthy adult relationships, workplace competency and self-worth [140].
Positive emotions such as awe are linked with lower inflammatory immune markers such as interleukin-6 (IL-6) [141]. Happiness, as distinct from negative emotions such as depression/anxiety, is associated with lower intercellular adhesion molecule-1 (ICAM-1) promoter methylation [142]. ICAM-1 is associated with progression of depressive symptoms, cognitive decline and cardiovascular disease [143,144]. Research shows that the frequency of daily positive events is associated with lower IL-6 and C-reactive protein [145]. In a study examining the emotional responses to daily stressors, elevated IL-6 was associated with decreased positive affect [146].
Optimism, or possessing positive outcome expectancy for future events across life domains, appears to be a particularly important asset within positive psychology. Higher levels of optimism have been associated with lower inflammatory cytokine levels and C-reactive protein [147], and lower inflammatory response to experimental stress [148]. However, optimism is an asset highly associated with socioeconomic advantage [149] and healthy lifestyle habits [5,150,151]. Optimism also has complex ties to sleep; optimism improves sleep, yet poor sleep appears to promote pessimism [152].
Purpose in life, increasingly connected to NCD risk reduction and health and well-being [153,154], is also an important factor that warrants attention. Generally, a high degree of purpose in life is associated with goals, meaning, and a long view. In community samples, high scores on purpose in life are associated with lower (shallow) discounting rates. Thus, a broader life view may diminish impulsivity [155]. Matching this, prospective research shows that higher levels of purpose in life predicts lower allostatic load [156] and purpose in life predicts greater use of preventive healthcare measures [157]. Research shows that purpose in life can mediate relationships between neighborhood disorder and parenting stress in disadvantaged communities [158]; thus, DOHaD conversations around purpose in life may be particularly important.
Empathy is the ability understand or make accurate inferences based on the experiences of another; the combined cognitive and emotional aspects of empathy allow one to take the perspective of another, and to experience some of their emotions in a vicarious way. It has been linked to life satisfaction, wellbeing, rich social networks, healthy relationships and workplace performance, accommodative behavior, and pro-social activity [159,160,161]. Prospective research indicates that higher levels of early-life empathy predict social competencies [162]. In youth, higher empathic concern is linked to greater connectedness to nature; thus, being able to take the point of view of another person and perceiving the natural world as important to health and wellbeing are intertwined [163]. Among youth, elevations in empathic concern over time have been associated with decreases in pro-inflammatory cytokines [164].

12. Natural Environments, Missing Exposures Exaggerate Disparity

Urban dwellers never have the chance to see the Milky Way, or a night radiant with stars, or even a truly blue sky. They never experience the subtle fragrances peculiar to each season; they lose the exhilaration of early spring and the delightful melancholy of autumn. The loss of these experiences is more than an aesthetic affliction; it corresponds to a deprivation of needs which are essential to physical and mental sanity, because they were indelibly woven in man’s fabric during his evolutionary past.
[165]
Natural environments are areas typically defined as those that are relatively unchanged or undisturbed by human culture, although they can include areas that are designed, manipulated and sustained by human interventions [166]. In the context of urban settings this may include gardens, parks, forests and waterside areas. Natural environments as whole (and specific elements within them: e.g., airborne phytoncides released from plants, natural light, negative ions, sounds, tactile opportunity, microbes (discussed in detail later)) may buffer stress, improve cognition, facilitate physical activity, encourage social cohesion, and promote overall health and mental well-being [167,168]. Residential proximity to higher concentrations of green space has been linked to lower mortality [169], and this appears to be especially true for disadvantaged communities [170].
From a developmental perspective, it is noteworthy that residential closeness to green space has been linked with healthy pregnancy outcomes (normal term and/or healthy birth weight) in multiple epidemiological studies [171,172,173,174,175,176,177,178,179,180]. These protective links are intertwined with SES but may be strongest in disadvantaged populations [181]. Surrounding greenness during pregnancy is associated with lower rates of incident asthma in children followed for 10 years [182]. Moreover, green space has been linked to good mental health during pregnancy; the influence of green space on resiliency against depression appears most pronounced in disadvantaged populations [183] (Figure 3).
Higher levels of green space at the neighborhood level have been associated health-related quality of life and reduced aggression in youth, as well as lower risks of depression and/or anxiety in the broad population [184,185,186,187,188,189,190,191]. Research tracking three years of mobility within the U.K. shows that individuals who move into areas with a higher greenness (vs. their previous residence) experience improved mental health [192]. Again, to what extent is this mobility research also capturing exit and entry from/to grey space?
However, unlike the gradient of depression risk that points toward the disadvantaged, the concentration of urban green space, parks, open play space (or perceived and real safety, accessibility, maintenance, quality) and local biodiversity is often slanted in the opposite direction, favoring the affluent and less vulnerable [193,194,195]. Among preschool children, SES and non-accessibility of green space are both associated with mental health problems [196]. Research shows that parks in high-income areas are used more frequently; in part, this may be due to the availability of structured/supervised activities and better marketing/outreach efforts that encourage use [197].
Natural environments research overlaps with screen time. Children residing in urban environments [198] and disadvantaged neighborhoods [199] may have higher daily screen time than rural or affluent counterparts. Research shows that in neighborhoods where walkability is less than optimal, screen time is higher [200,201,202]. When both parents and children perceive their neighborhoods to be safe, physical activity is higher and screen time is significantly lower [203]. Adding to the complexity of oft-isolated research, among children from low-income families in the United States, high levels of screen time are coincident with frequent fast-food consumption [204].
The mental health value of green space as a means to help narrow SES health inequalities has been examined in large-scale epidemiological research throughout European cities. Notably, socioeconomic inequality in mental well-being was 40% (8.1 points on the 5-item World Health Organization Well-Being Index score) narrower among respondents reporting good access to green/recreational areas, compared with those with poorer access [205]. Thus, providing equitable access to natural environments in urban settings can assist in disrupting the usual conversion of socioeconomic inequality to health inequality.
Emerging research shows that the health benefits of visiting natural environments may depend on dose; for example, dose-response analysis indicates that visits to outdoor green spaces of 30 min or more during the course of a week could significantly reduce depression and hypertension within urban populations [206]. Spending time in natural environments has been associated with lower markers of inflammation, stress physiology and oxidative stress [207,208,209]. Residential proximity to and/or engaging with natural environments has been associated with lower cortisol levels [210,211,212,213]. Comparing Scottish socioeconomically disadvantaged neighborhoods, some of which can vary in their green space land use from as low as 14% to as high as 75%, researchers find higher percentages of green space are linked with healthier daytime salivary cortisol patterns and lower perceived stress [214,215].
Access to neighborhood green space has also been linked to healthy sleep [216,217] and a 17-min lunchtime walk in nature (vs. built environment) has been shown to have positive influences on parasympathetic activity during night-time sleep [218]. Normal delivery of adequate light during the day and withdrawal from light at night (LAN) represents an essential biophysical cue during pregnancy. Alterations in the photoperiod (known as chronodisruption) and can interfere with healthy, term pregnancies and have long-term effects on normal offspring [219,220,221]. Artificial LAN has been linked to obesity at the international level [222]. Thus, walkable neighborhoods, quality green space and opportunity for outdoor activity, coincident with lower evening screen time, is connected to sleep hygiene.
In academic settings, proximity to natural environments and classroom views to green space have been associated with positive cognitive development, attention, resilience in stress physiology, and academic performance [223,224,225,226,227]. These results hold for standardized testing even when SES is controlled [223,224]. Moreover, the influence of being in nature or exposure to scenes of nature extends to altruism, helping behavior and social value orientation [228,229,230]. These studies examining cognitive and psychological outcomes lend support to epidemiological findings concerning mental health.

13. Nature Relatedness

Individual psychological constructs such as nature connectivity, nature connectedness and nature relatedness (NR) can measure an individual’s awareness, understanding and fascination with the natural world, as well as an interest in and desire for nature contact. Higher scores on these scales have been consistently linked to mental well-being [231,232]. NR is a strong predictor of visitation to local green space and meeting physical activity guidelines within green space [233].
However, researchers must now explore the ways in which NR is developed, perhaps in early life, and to what extent it is influenced by SES, if only through access and experience in nature. If natural environments can reduce health disparities (especially mental health) along SES lines, it would seem valuable to know if NR is a variable in that equation. Indeed, NR may be of relevance to discussion of the microbiome if it facilitates increased contact with diverse microorganisms.

14. Humans, Microbes, Environment as an Ecological Unit

Thus, life in the world of nature, implying as it does endless contact with all kinds of microbes, early brings forth in animals an adaptive response”.
[234]
The holobiont view of human life underscores that we are assemblages of different species—persistent symbionts—that make up an ecological unit. The human host and its microbiome (trillions of microbes and their collective genomes) are therefore an ecological community. In turn, as we will discuss in more detail later, the holobiont operates within its own ecological theater—that is, the often inequitable biotic and abiotic neighborhood.
Dating back to the pioneer studies of Dubos, the importance of microbes in early life has been a staple of DOHaD research [235]. Functions of the microbiome include, but are not limited to, “education“ of the immune system, protection against pathogens, maintenance of barriers to the external environment, nutrient production and extraction, increasing bioavailability of dietary phytochemicals, lipid metabolism, provision of short chain fatty acids, production of bioactive metabolites, and detoxification of environmental toxins. Collectively these benefits can be described as ecosystem services [54]. Recognition of these functional attributes often surface as a result of observing the consequences of microbial disturbances via antibiotics, stress, westernized diets and/or lifestyle.
Dysbiosis is a term that translates as “difficult living“ or “life in distress“; given neighborhood grey space, massive health disparities, matters of economic and environmental injustices, biodiversity losses, climate change, rapid urbanization and other threats to ecosystems, it has been argued that dysbiosis can apply at the individual, neighborhood and global level [54,236]. In the context of microbiology, dysbiosis refers to perturbations of at least one, or a combination of the following: loss of beneficial microorganisms, and/or the expansion of potentially harmful microbes, and/or the loss of overall microbial diversity [237].
Emerging evidence suggests that gut microbial dysbiosis may, at least in part, have causal relationships with depressive symptoms, anxiety and mood alterations [238,239,240,241,242]. In experimental research antibiotic exposure during pregnancy disturbs microbiota and causes behavioral deficits in offspring [243], while human studies have linked antibiotic use to depression and anxiety [244]. Pathways of microbial influences on brain development, stress physiology, mood, cognition and behavior include, but are not limited to: immune-mediated pathways, enhancement of nutrient bioavailability and neurotransmitter precursors, support of the gastrointestinal barrier, redox homeostasis, and even direct gut microbe-to-brain communication via the vagus nerve [245]. While most microbiota-mood evidence is confined to rodent studies, there are intriguing human intervention studies suggesting that microbiome therapeutics (e.g., probiotics) can improve short-term mental health and improve quality of life [246,247].

15. Encephalobiotics, Promissory Notes

Encephalobiotics are defined here as probiotics, prebiotics, postbiotics, microbes, microbial parts and/or agents that influence the microbiome for cognition, mental well-being and brain health. They are distinct from so-called psychobiotics which are defined as specific living microorganisms used for patients with psychiatric disorders [248]. Since psychobiotics are directed at patients with mental disorder, they are by definition tightly-regulated drugs that will require years if not decades of research. That fact hasn’t curbed enthusiasm for hyperbolic media headlines suggesting those with emotional disorders “forget prozac“ in favor of psychobiotics.
Early-life microbial applications may have a place. However, in our opinion, the promise of microbial application (from early life and beyond) for long-term mental health benefit via encephalobiotics will be unrealized if the drivers of dysbiosis (both in its microbial definition and personal, community and planetary “life in distress” definition) remain in place. Neoliberal ideology suggests that dysbiosis at all levels is fixable at the individual level, and in particular, with product application.
The interconnected, patent-driven, biomedical-biotechnology-microbiome zeitgeist has provided plenty of promissory discussions with little mention of SES disadvantage. Thus, discourse concerning the coincident environmental and marketing forces that might cause their need in the first place—i.e., higher need in the very populations least well-equipped to purchase them—is avoided [236]. Absent is the reality that the external built ecosystem (i.e., the neighborhood) maintains a continual undertow toward dysbiosis.

16. Hygiene Hypothesis, Dysbiotic Drift

As we illustrate, the drivers of microbial dysbiosis (e.g., westernized/ultra-processed diet, stress, alcohol, tobacco, circadian disruptions, antibiotic use, et al.) are slanted toward the SES disadvantaged in developed nations, and increasingly so in developing nations. The presence of grey space (described in detail later; an environment maintained by marketing forces and absence of policy that could otherwise transform opportunity for health) accompanies disadvantage and contributes to what has been referred to as “dysbiotic drift“ [249]. For example, reduced diversity of colonic microbiota (sampled via mucosal biopsy) is evident among residents of lower socioeconomic status (SES) neighborhoods in North America [250].
Some brief contextual background to illustrate the inequity of ecological medicine may be helpful. Briefly, the hygiene hypothesis and its variants suggested that the global rise in allergic disease is (at least in part) a product of diminished opportunity for early life exposure to diverse microbial exposure via increased hygiene, antibiotics and smaller family sizes and westernized dietary patterns [251,252]. Presently, available evidence supports the hygiene hypothesis; indeed lack of evolutionary-rooted microbial immune priming as an NCD-provocateur may also extend to neurocognitive and mental health [245,253].
One barometer of the hygiene hypothesis, and its links to what might be referred to as “microbial deficit disorders“, includes shifts away from fermented foods that have otherwise been a long-standing part of traditional dietary practices [254]. For example, recent studies show that higher consumption of traditionally fermented foods is associated with reduced risk of atopy in adults [255], lower rates of allergy in children [256], anxiety in young adults [257], and eczema in offspring when consumed during pregnancy [258]. However, in North America, affluent Caucasian women maintaining healthy lifestyles—those at far lower risk of depression—are significantly more likely to consume fermented foods and encephalobiotics [259].
The dysbiotic drift theory of SES disadvantage in developed nations does not hinge on diet alone. Antibiotic exposure is often higher in disadvantaged populations [260,261,262]. The microbes carried by mammals are also a product of the ecosystems in which they reside [263,264,265,266]; given that the Earth is home to upward of 1 trillion microbial species [267], human contact with many of these microbes in natural environments (the ecosystem in which we once spent the majority of our time) may have evolutionary-rooted, health-protective properties.
The SES deprivation of natural environments may be an unrecognized consideration in microbiome and health discussions. For example, in humans the level of green space and biodiversity of vegetation surrounding one’s residence appears to drive diversity of cutaneous microbial ecosystems, including Gammaproteobacteria and key species within this microbial family [268,269]. Green spaces contribute unique and diverse beta bacterial signatures to the urban environment [270], and such vegetation makes a significant contribution to the airborne microbial content—up to 10-fold higher than nearby non-vegetated built areas [271].
Mycobacterium vaccae, a generally non-pathogenic microbe commonly encountered in natural environments, has been shown to reduce depression and anxiety-like behavior in animals in concert with activation of central serotonergic pathways [272,273]. Additional experimental research shows that heat-inactivated M. vaccae can enhance fear extinction, improve stress coping, and prevent stress-induced dysbiosis [274]. Potential mechanisms include a suppression of pro-inflammatory cytokine production.
Examining the non-dietary and antibiotic pathways to dysbiosis shows that they are essentially the very same roads of exposure travelled by disadvantaged populations. Human and/or experimental research demonstrates that the risk of microbial dysbiosis is increased by acute and cumulative psychological and physical (e.g., thermal, noise) stress [275,276,277,278,279,280,281], environmental pollutants including lead, polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons and airborne particulate matter [282,283,284], tobacco exposure [285,286,287] and excess alcohol consumption [288]. In addition, the oral periodontopathogen P. gingivalis, reported to be much higher in North American disadvantaged populations [289] has recently been shown to cause dysbiosis when swallowed [290]. Other dysbiotic influences which are more common along SES disadvantaged lines include sedentary behavior, circadian disruptions, sleep problems with functional constipation, and low levels of vitamin D [249].

17. Grey Space, Inequity and the Environmental Push

Thus far we have presented research highlighting that a preponderance of existing research supports healthy dietary patterns, microbial diversity, and access to quality natural environments as factors in positive mental health. Here, we underscore that research topics such as positive psychology, microbiota, diet and green space research are often placed in silos. Closer examination of the inequity of these elements in SES disadvantaged urban environments reveals their important synergy.
North American parks and their immediate vicinity represent important locations where food and beverages can be purchased (running the gamut from sit-down restaurants with healthy choices to fast-food chains, food carts and vending machines) [291,292]. What’s more, residential proximity to urban green space and greater park access is associated with healthier dietary habits (e.g., more fruits, vegetables, whole grains, nuts/beans, and less fast-food, sodium-rich food and sugar-rich beverages) and lower insulin resistance [293,294,295]. In higher population density areas, relatively more natural food/specialty stores, fewer convenience stores and more physical activity resources are associated with higher diet quality [296]. Lower availability of parks (and open spaces) for physical activity may operate in tandem with greater density of fast-food outlets (and less grocery stores) in the promotion of NCDs, especially for the disadvantaged [297,298].

18. Walking in Grey Space

Although it is atypical to invite readers of academic articles to engage in visualization exercises, here we welcome the reader to imagine residing in a socioeconomically deprived community (depicted in aggregate) through the research presented in this section. That is, to fully immerse in the mental environment wherein aggregate individual and neighborhood disadvantage may be classically determined by income, education, social cohesion, racial/ethnic/immigrant segregation, evaluations of neighborhood aesthetic quality, and/or aspects of actual or perceived safety. We recognise the heterogeneity of urban environments in developed nations and that not all disadvantaged areas are alike; however, the visualized community (henceforth referred to as “our neighborhood“) may have some, or even all of the aforementioned disparities.
We believe this visualization is important because academic discussions on environment and lifestyle can be reductionist and isolated; visualizing an environment can effectively influence cognition. Our intention is to encourage readers to absorb at least some tiny slivers of the mental environment (described below) that make up the daily reality in SES disadvantaged urban environments. As Dubos stated in 1965 concerning the scientific disconnect:
A more disturbing aspect of modern science is that the specialist himself commonly loses contact with the aspect of reality which was his primary concern, whether it was matter, life or man”.
[299]
We have already highlighted medically-important lifestyle habits that can be coincident with disadvantage. Higher levels of screen time and indoor sedentary behavior, too little or too much sleep (and frequent daytime sleep [300]), dietary patterns of high-calorie, low-nutrient-density foods (including ultra-processed, high sodium, additive-rich foods; and/or less fruits and vegetables), excess alcohol consumption, and tobacco use are characteristic of disadvantage [249]. Some of these habits may extend to “bystanders“ (e.g., second-hand smoke and shared dietary choices) [301].

19. Behavioral Reinforcement

To begin, we will find a disproportionate availability of bars, liquor stores, convenience stores, fast-food outlets, and tobacco vendors—which significantly alter the mental landscape of both adults and children [54]. This matters. As much as 31% of the variance in excessive fast-food consumption may be attributable to simply living in urban areas with moderate or high density of fast-food outlets [302]. On our walk we will surely encounter greater clustering of fast-food outlets and convenience stores around schools, as seen in numerous international studies [303,304,305,306,307,308]. We will find much higher brand name logo recognition in children in lower SES neighbourhoods [309]. Employment in “our neighborhood“ is more likely to be within fast-food or convenience store establishments—work that is also linked to greater obesity risk [310]. Healthy food and beverage options are less readily available [311,312,313] in this neighborhood. Instead, we are confronted with visual marketing—e.g., billboards, sidewalk signage, exterior-of-store ads, targeted screen media delivery—that strongly encourage us to uphold our unhealthy lifestyle choices such as sugar-added beverages, fast-food consumption and tobacco use [54,249,314,315]. This all means there are numerous additive interactions that explain socioeconomic inequalities in diet and obesity [316], and why the association between exposure to fast-food outlets and obesity is most pronounced for lower SES groups. But the stacked deck does not end here.
Remarkably, pharmacies (outlets that should be community beacons of medicinal products) might market cigarettes at relatively inexpensive prices [317]. Shelf-space and orientation within supermarkets and convenience stores in our neighborhood would likely be set up in such a way as to direct us to energy-dense, low nutrient foods [318,319,320,321,322]. Also inside, in-store marketing may direct us to a significant variety of (relatively) low-priced, high-sugar beverages [323]. On the other hand, healthier staples such as low-fat dairy products may be less available and more expensive [324].
The unhealthy foods in our neighborhood stores would be well-matched to targeted screen-media based marketing [325]. Media exposure can place children in front of celebrity endorsements for high-calorie, low-nutrient foods and beverages [326]. From a DOHaD perspective, the conversation concerning the built environment and drivers of behaviour is imperative; availability of convenience stores is associated with obesity in SES disadvantaged preschool children and higher presence of fast-food outlets with gestational diabetes [327,328].
More fast food outlets in SES disadvantaged communities can mean greater options to “super-size” high-fat foods and sugar-added beverages, obtain free prizes and with purchase, and the lure of branded mascots and licensed, recognizable characters [311]. Ages 3 to 5 are a critical time when taste and food preferences are being developed. Moreover, research shows that unhappiness and negative experiences during childhood are associated with adult food preferences toward comfort foods and away from healthier choices [329]. It is alarming, therefore, that in children within this young age group, knowledge of toys being offered by a multi-national purveyor of fast-food is significantly associated with higher frequency of eating at these locations [330]. Exposure to food marketing—including licensed and brand-equity characters (especially for children 3–12 years old)—is linked to significant, negative effects on food preferences, choices and consumption [331]. Consider that 50% of kids meals offered at North American fast-food and chain outlets exceed the WHO’s proposed daily (added) sugar recommendation of <5% of total energy [332].
Since the sum of combined marketing-dietary choice evidence meets Bradford Hill Criteria for causality in public health research (i.e., strength of association, dose-response, and consistent, biologically plausible and supported by experimental evidence), there is now clear substantiation for urgent restrictions on all forms of food marketing to children [333].
There is little dispute that transnational corporations profit from unhealthy commodities in our neighborhood. Profits—and the corporate political activity that maintains them—could easily drive taste-engineering decisions, and such decisions influence overconsumption of highly processed, calorie-dense, nutrient-poor foods [334,335,336] (Figure 4). The taste-engineering frame operates through three core pathways: environmental engineering (i.e., shaping the conditions in which food choices are made; e.g., point-of-purchase at check-out, all-you-can-eat buffets, vending machines in schools/hospitals, value meals etc.), cognitive engineering (i.e., marketing in all forms of media, sponsorship, celebrity etc.) and physiological engineering (i.e., exploitation of taste preferences for sodium, sugar, fat, caffeine) [337]. Put simply, taste-engineering is bathing the fetus, influencing the parents, the children and caregivers in our societies: this should be of the upmost importance in DOHaD discussions.
There is little doubt that effective marketing works. It shouldn’t be surprising that living in the vicinity of billboards advertising of highly processed foods is associated with decreased daily fruit or vegetable consumption [338]. Food advertising is especially effective in cajoling unhealthy food consumption when cognitive load is high. Thus, we have focused on delay discounting and cognitive resources in our perspective review to underscore the implications of its effects in our neighbourhood. Young adults from low SES backgrounds are more prone to advertising’s effects while under cognitive load [339].
In our neighbourhood, higher levels of persistent fatigue and psychological distress may enhance marketing messages in several key ways: First, as alluded to earlier, palatable, highly processed foods high in sugar, fat and sodium may induce temporary physiological changes to alleviate some degree of stress [76]. Similarly, tobacco and alcohol are also used as a means to mitigate stress [340]. Also, the appeal of readily available fast-food and ready-to-eat highly-processed foods could be magnified higher when fatigue, food insecurity, and economic pressures are at the forefront of thought processes [249]. Research involving pregnant (SES disadvantaged) women confirms that fatigue, irregular schedules, time pressure, and finances are intertwined with cravings for calorie-dense, nutrient poor foods [341]. In this way, omnipresent marketing messages and the subtle positioning of unhealthy foods within our neighbourhood are like a trap rigged toward the most vulnerable. Considering that experimental studies indicate a maternal ultra-processed westernized diet influences the offspring opioid system to reinforce preference for such foods [342], the time is past due for consideration of the neighbourhood as a womb, and for societal responsibility for the rigged, grey neighbourhood.
Thus, grey space is not solely about airborne particulate matter, soil contaminants, and environmental toxins in building materials. Rather, it is a completely disparate mental environment from that which green space represents. Grey space places a physiological and psychological, chemical and non-chemical, burden on available cognitive and other coping resources.
Finally, in regard to our emphasis on the importance of empathy in medical training, or lack thereof, in the context of neighborhood ecology: Research indicates that patients in our neighborhood may perceive the empathy of their physicians to be much lower than that afforded to physicians in affluent areas [343,344]. Moreover, in the relationship between physician empathy, humility and good medical outcomes, perception is likely a reality [345,346]. Given that inappropriate antibiotic prescriptions remain alarmingly high [347], it is worth noting that in the visualized neighborhood, one where primary care demands are typically strong, consultations shorter and provider stress is higher [348], a physician visit in our neighborhood is more likely to conclude with an antibiotic prescription [260,261,262]. Clearly, the “ecology“ of the patient visit and its direct and indirect effects on the microbiome requires further study.

20. Ecological Justice and the Erosion of Health

The tendency to disregard ecology in medical research may have far reaching consequences. For example, it facilitates the interpretation of the “environment” as “psychosocial environment”. The study of the environment is then implicitly relegated to psychology and social science. No wonder then that mental illness, in the orthodox view, gets a biological interpretation which skimps ecology.
van der Steen and Thung. In Faces of Medicine: A Philosophical Study, 1988 [349]
It is our contention that the term ecological justice (or ecological injustice) is well suited to capture the policies and practices that either promote or erode personal, community and planetary health. Healthy ecosystems involve interacting organisms operating within their abiotic (non-living, physical/chemical) environment. An ideal ecosystem (i.e., the “dwelling place” of each individual residence, their neighborhood and the collective city—socially, economically and environmentally supported) should allow each human to reach their potential, while at the same time ensuring planetary health.
The introduction of ecology into equity, justice and DOHaD discourse is a natural, if not necessary, development due to the microbiome revolution. The study of the microbiome has united researchers from virtually every branch of science and medicine. In the process, it has made salient the health relevancy of diverse ecosystems that operate from the largest scale (planetary) to the smallest (the microscopic dwellings on a single intestinal villus) [54].
Ecosystems—microbial and otherwise—can be manipulated by individual human behavior (lifestyle). However, the lifestyles of the host can be manipulated by forces within the larger “dwelling place” (e.g., neighborhood with more grey space and less green space; herein, high concentrations of fast-food outlets may be considered invasive species that limit growth and potential). Moving the lens even further out, the lifestyles (and policies and practices that maintain them) that promote environmental degradation, climate change and biodiversity loss are threats to ecological health. Since the effects of degradation and losses are (and will be in the future) shouldered by the disadvantaged [350], this is a matter of ecological justice. We urge that there is “No Health Without Ecological Health”.
Just as the DOHaD paradigm currently works diligently to prevent disease at its earliest point, so too it must identify the ways in which pro-social/environmental attitudes/behaviors and are shaped early in life. SES inequity is an obvious driver of health disparities, but to what extent does DOHaD place emphasis on research surrounding the societal cues—e.g., even miniscule hints of money, incidental exposure in experimental settings—that reinforce beliefs that socially advantaged groups should dominate socially disadvantaged groups [51]? This research suggests that improving the mental environment in disadvantaged communities—and thus, life-course health—requires education of the advantaged communities.
The crises of climate change, rapid urbanization, environmental degradation and gross biodiversity loss [351,352] in the context of a disconnection from nature [167,353], force emphasis from the DOHaD perspective that human health is dependent upon planetary health. DOHaD is making every effort to undo the NCD crisis at its fetal/first 1000 days origins; however, as a scientific/clinical movement it cannot promote health without widening the lens such that it visualizes the dysbiosis (life in distress) of Earthly ecosystems [236] and the reality that NCDs will be increasingly driven by climate change [354].
As much as natural environments have been associated with health promotion, research also demonstrates that “removal” or gross alteration of such environments is detrimental. Visible environmental degradation—whether through climate change, losses of millions of trees via the activity of invasive species, or human-generated industrial activity—has been clearly linked to diminished physical and mental health [355,356,357,358,359]. This degradation may have negative impacts on lifestyle behaviors such as leisure time spent outdoors in nature [360]. On the other hand, green remediation of vacated urban blight may be set up in such a way as to be aesthetically pleasing, yet also host biodiversity [361]. Relatively simple transformation of vacant urban lots with trees and vegetation has been associated with improved perceptions of the neighborhood, more physical activity, less stress and decreasing violence [362,363,364].
If we are to create change in ecological justice, if are to provide an equitable Petri dish for all humans to reach their potential, growing in an environment that acknowledges the interconnectivity and essentiality of biodiversity [365,366] to personal, public and planetary health, its emphasis through education, experience and the development of empathic concern in early life is imperative. For example, children who frequently experience nature are more likely to develop emotional affinity to and support for protecting biodiversity [367,368]. Outdoor childhood experience and engagement with nature has been reported to be a key driver of adult appreciation for the role of trees in human well-being [369,370].
Participation in personal, community and academic gardening programs (Figure 5) may enhance intake and taste preference for fruits and vegetables [371,372], help maintain healthy BMI while diminishing fast-food consumption [373], increase positive emotions (e.g., joy, awe) and connectedness to nature [374]. Environmental education in elementary school early on in life can influence pro-environmental concerns. However, high levels of screen time have been associated with diminished concern for the environment [375]. Thus, we have emphasized screen time because it is no less a part of the DOHaD ecological picture, and it highlights the limitations of isolated, reductionist research in translation efforts.
Positive affect (vs. negative emotions/mood) which we have emphasized, is an important factor in encouraging prosocial behaviors [376,377] and appears to be critical in the development of pro-social behaviors in early life [378]. So too, environmentally sustainable behaviors and quality of life are significantly interrelated [379]. Engaging in pro-social activity, including in early life, encourages positive affect and reinforces further pro-social behavior [380,381,382]. This could be especially important when considering that in prospective research, associations between low pro-sociality at age 3 and subsequent behavioral problems are exacerbated in disadvantaged neighborhoods and low-performing schools [383].

21. Preparedness: Training Clinicians for Ecological Medicine

Despite what is already known concerning physical activity, nutrition and healthy weight management (and that best-practice guidelines for these variables are already in place), from the DOHaD perspective it should be alarming that so many women receive so little guidance from clinicians. For example, in a recent U.S.A. study, only about half of pregnant women received provider advice on these lifestyle factors [384]. Separate research from Australia also shows that few women receive clinical guidance on nutrition during pregnancy [385].
Volumes of emerging research in animal models demonstrate the importance of healthy dietary patterns, physical activity, adequate sleep and nutrition (and, for example, emergent microbiota-epigenetic mechanisms). These are informative and exciting from a DOHaD basic science perspective [11]. However, as Dubos warned in 1969 concerning technology research, there is a point at which enough is known about existing best practice where an emphasis should be placed on advocacy and barriers to change:
This is wonderfully entertaining, titillating kind of science fiction. We organize meetings about it in all sorts of pleasant places to talk about this, and that saves us the responsibility of walking across the street, where 100,000 children are being poisoned every day by lead in paint…something can be done immediately about this problem, but it is not being done because it is not of sufficient interest or as exciting intellectually”.
[386]
Barriers to change include medical education. Despite recognizing the importance of exercise counseling in clinical settings, as many as 85% of graduating medical students in one North American study reported being to be ill-prepared to do so. The vast majority reported no training on patient interactions concerning exercise guidance [387]. Such findings shouldn’t be surprising given that less than half of all medical school curricula throughout the United States provide any formal training on physical activity [388]. Nutrition education also remains paltry—only 29% of medical schools in the USA provide the recommended minimum 25 h of nutrition education. Moreover, the bulk of nutrition instruction provided is still confined to preclinical (e.g., biochemistry) [389].
Again, from a DOHaD perspective, it should be alarming that medical school graduates entering pediatric residency are deficient in basic nutritional knowledge [390]. Fruit and vegetable intake among children in developed nations is notoriously low [391], especially in SES disadvantaged communities [392,393]. When as many as 87% of children from SES disadvantaged neighborhoods in North America consume less than two servings of fruits and vegetables daily [394], the clinical knowledge gaps should be of grave concern.
Baseline empathy among future doctors predicts resilience against typical declines in empathy toward patients during clinical training. This suggests that medical school admissions processes should consider this an asset, or at least something to be cultivated [395]. Declines in physician empathy are “a threat to health care quality“ [396], and this may be especially true in disadvantaged communities where, as we have already described, empathic concern by doctors is a critical part of outcomes. It may also help physicians understand the broad environmental crises and who will bear the force of climate change first, the disadvantaged.
It is commonly stated that physicians are often the most trusted sources about the environment, including matters of lifestyle and climate change as they pertain to health. But what is the basis of this privileged status when there is little physician training on neighborhood and planetary ecosystems in relation to chronic disease [397]? In recent surveys of American physicians specializing in allergy and lung function, only 7% to 10% reported being very knowledgeable about the association between climate change and health, with as many as 47% indicating that climate change is either not happening, it is mostly a natural phenomenon, or that human activity is no more a factor than natural processes [398,399,400]. These beliefs, and they are beliefs, are in direct opposition to the consensus of every legitimate science organization. The scales are well tipped toward humans as the dominant factor in climate changes over the last century and a half [401].
Board exam/licensing questions to ensure knowledge accountability on ecological health (lifestyle and the influence of the environment), supervised clinical exposure and continuing medical education seems essential (Figure 6). Medical training devoid of course material, clinical exposure and exam accountability related to pharmaceuticals seems unimaginable. Medical training is one small, but important cog in the wheel of transforming the view of interconnectivity of life and lifestyle. The first step in this transformation will be undoing the ability of purveyors of ultra-processed foods from insinuating themselves in to medicine. At present, major health organizations accept tremendous sums of money from the largest players in the global soft drink industry; this, despite these companies lobbying against 97% of public health interventions intended to reduce junk food and improve nutrition [402].

22. Transforming Life: From Epigenetics to Advocacy

The study of man as an integrated unit, and of the ecosystems in which he functions, is grossly neglected…a very different kind of knowledge is needed to understand the nature of the cohesive forces which maintain man in an integrated state, physically, psychologically, and socially, and enable him to relate successfully to his environment.
Rene Dubos, 1965 [299]
It seems fair to ask whether or not the relegation of so-called “softer” science such as positive emotions, empathy, natural environments, marketing, biodiversity, nature-relatedness, pro-environmental and prosocial behavior etc. in the lower ranks of DOHaD discussions is in fact a collective professional act of delay discounting. Life-course health for the holobiont over the long-term will only be as good as these “soft” drivers of equity, cooperation, and societal decisions that determine the neighborhood and overall planetary health. In 1966 Dubos urged his fellow scientists to “try, collectively, to imagine the world in which we want to live...improving the environment should not mean only correcting pollution or other evils of technological and urban growth” [403].
Improving the environment in a holistic way will require imagination on the part of medical doctors, allied health professionals and scientists of all stripes. It will also require advocacy. The upstream drivers that bring increasing numbers of NCDs to the clinic are a product of a system of profits leading to pandemics [334], and absentia urban policies (covering quality of housing, employment opportunity, living wage compensation, social welfare) that that develop and/or maintain grey space. The policies that drive grey space and ecological inequalities, or at the very least, leave them in place, should be as unacceptable as a tobacco campaign aimed at children.
We implore all scientists, physicians and allied health professionals with an interest in DOHaD—especially with an eye toward social/environmental justice—to support advocacy in shifting the focus away from the well-trodden path of individual-only responsibility. Otherwise, we must surely ask to what extent headline-grabbing technological interventions (e.g., dietary supplements, probiotics that might address microbiota via epigenetics) can fulfill their expectations when environmental forces continue to erode the possibility of health, and drive dysbiosis by default? Dubos, perhaps said it more bluntly:
Developing counter technologies to correct new kinds of damage constantly being created by technological innovations is a policy of despair...we must try to imagine the kind of surroundings and of life we want, lest we end up with a jumble of technologies that will eventually smother body and soul.
[404]

23. Green Gentrification

It is not our contention that transformation of urban grey spaces into green spaces and blanket positive psychology interventions are ripe to proceed at an unbridled pace. Although available evidence provides plenty of generalized support for urban greening, what are the risks associated with transformation of urban neighborhoods—turning grey into green—such that they gentrify and displace the very people the action was intended to help [405,406]? As researchers learn more concerning how such transformations might take place (e.g., what constitutes best practice for natural environment quality, hypoallergenic greening, tree canopy height and density, design that might facilitate stress reduction or increase fear (hiding spots), aesthetic factors and more [407]), and the extent to which such changes have real-world benefits with worthwhile “effect sizes”, the unintended consequences of well-meaning transformations could be minimized by the active involvement of community stakeholders (including minorities, immigrants and others living with disadvantage) [408,409].

24. Conclusions

We have traversed broad-ranging topics that, at first glance, may seem only loosely connected to place-based characteristics and their relationships to to DOHaD. However, it is clear that the interconnectedness of urban life and lifestyle—biodiversity and bios, place and policy, practitioner and patient—can be illuminated by closer inspection of the microbiome zeitgeist. In particular, it demands discussion of the ways in which place-based health inequities are driven by upstream factors, such as marketing forces, neoliberal ideology and polices (of the absence thereof) that reinforce a dysbiosis on a gradient slanted toward the disadvantaged.
Available research allows us to draw straighter lines between the absence of healthy exposures and the presence of unhealthy exposures as they relate to the ecology of health—at the most minute and grand scales. We have attempted to underscore the unhealthy negative exposures that often escape DOHaD discourse; the connectivity, for example, between neighborhood fast-food clustering, the cognition of poverty itself, commercial advertising campaigns aimed at children, and how these might permeate the inner ecosystem in a place-based way. Thus, we have introduced the term ecological justice.
As we have attempted to make clear, ecological justice also involves equity in access to positive exposures in urban environments. Neighborhood access to natural environments and biodiversity are emerging as a means to narrow urban health inequities. Since positive psychological factors such as nature relatedness and optimism are emerging as health assets (the latter, optimism, enjoyed by the privileged at the neighborhood level), we need further understanding of ways in which place can determine allostatic load by way of its ability to foster or impede a healthy inner ecosystem.
In sum, a pivot in DOHaD research toward the upstream drivers of place-based health is required. Closer examination of parenting styles [410,411,412] in the urban grey space context, positive psychology interventions (also mindfulness, empathy, and relaxation response training) [413,414,415,416,417,418], and encouragement of time spent in natural environments via large-scale nature engagement campaigns [419,420] are worthy of study. We also need to learn more concerning emerging microbial applications in early life (e.g., through nature contact, controlled seeding of microbes and via environmental design) [421,422,423]. In other cases we simply need to learn more about effective delivery and advocacy. For example, how to promote engagement in uncomplicated exercise interventions such as walking (promotes positive affect even when participants expect the opposite) [424]. Now that a causal relationship between marketing and childhood food choices has been established [333], it is past-time to curb the unbridled marketing/sales of unhealthy foods/lifestyles to both children and adults in disadvantaged communities.
Transforming grey space, particularly its food environment, will require collective action; evidence indicates that it is insufficient to focus on exclusively on individual, interpersonal, organizational, and community levels [425]. Politics and policy will play a role, and this occurs in the context of cultural readiness for change—especially when interests, power and values of public and private actors are not in alignment [426]. However, it seems clear that simple rinse-and-repeat messages to eat healthy via pyramids and plates are far from effective. Rather, emerging research shows that when the topic of healthy nutrition and the food environment is presented to youth from as a food justice and power inequity issue, interest is piqued [427]. This may be especially true in youth from SES disadvantaged communities. Informed on the relationship between food environment issues and poor dietary behavior, empowered youth became community advocates for change, educating parents, teachers and others [428]. However, this same research shows that community efforts must be supported at the larger societal, policy level.
Early indications from Mexico’s innovative tax on nonessential energy-dense foods show a decline in purchases of such items, particularly in lower SES households [429]. Municipal efforts, such as the policy to eliminate sugar-sweetened beverage sales on Boston city property, has also reduced sugar and caloric intake, with potential community-wide benefits [430]. Transformation of vending machine contents in urban parks has also been shown to bring caloric and sugar content in alignment with guidelines for children [431]. Encouragingly, research efforts aimed at evaluating the combined influences of early-life healthy eating, physical activity and nature relatedness in urban children are underway [432].
Transformation of grey space and equity in maximizing the odds for the realization of health—personal, societal and planetary—may occur more rapidly when rights are kept in the forefront of our minds. As we stated at the outset, the United Nations and other international groups state quite clearly that children have the right to thrive, and that all humans have a fundamental right to live in an environment that supports their overall health and well-being [433]. Often this is interpreted almost exclusively as an environment that is absent chemical toxins [434]. Here, we have used a wide-angle lens in how we interpret the “environment“, and its relationship to human rights to health.
We underscore that a mental environment that is absent of green space and filled with “grey” visual, auditory and total sensory cajoling toward unhealthy lifestyle behaviours is at odds with this fundamental right to thrive. It is most certainly at odds with the right of children to develop a healthy respect for the natural environment. Our concern is that the connections between other societal-induced physical contaminants within built landscapes (the exposures to which can be manipulated by marketing) and trans-generational health routinely evade discussion. We need to pull the curtain back and continue to show our youth how they and their neighbourhoods, and indeed even the health organizations they are groomed to trust, can be manipulated by marketers of dysbiosis [402].
We agree with others who have pointed out through research applications that in matters of SES disadvantage, policy and the environment, there often remains more plasticized rhetoric than forthright action [435]. DOHaD research and advocacy can help speed up rhetoric to translation. In keeping with our theme of positive emotions and optimism, and honouring the dedication of the youth who helped transform two corner stores in a disadvantaged Los Angeles community [428], reaching out in multiple directions to individuals, shopkeepers and society as they attempted to brighten a touch of grey, we close on some of the last words ever written by Rene Dubos:
I am eighty years old as I write these lines…I am still vigorous enough not only to resent many aspects of modern civilization but more importantly to enjoy the world and have faith in its future...I have become convinced that resiliency is a universal attribute of all living organisms—from natural ecosystems to individual human beings; it is also one of the most important. In living organisms, resiliency implies the ability both to recover from traumatic experiences and to create new values during the very process of recovery”.
[436]

Author Contributions

Susan L. Prescott, and Alan C. Logan contributed equally to all aspects of the review, including conception, design, literature searches, drafts, and editing of final manuscript. Both authors provided important intellectual content. Both authors approved the final version of the submitted manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

Susan L. Prescott reports the following: Scientific Advisory Board and speakers fees from Danone Nutricia (Schiphol, The Netherlands) and Nestlé Nutrition Institute (Lausanne, Switzerland); compensated consultant to Bayer Pharmaceuticals (Whippany, NJ, USA); Alan C. Logan has received consultancy fees from Genuine Health (Toronto, ON, Canada).

References

  1. Dubos, R. Mere survival is not enough for man. Life 1970, 69, 2. [Google Scholar]
  2. Dubos, R.; Savage, D.; Schaedler, R. Biological freudianism: Lasting effects of early environmental influences. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2005, 34, 5–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Dubos, R. Man Adapting; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
  4. Boehm, J.K.; Trudel-Fitzgerald, C.; Kivimaki, M.; Kubzansky, L.D. The prospective association between positive psychological well-being and diabetes. Health Psychol. 2015, 34, 1013–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Hernandez, R.; Kershaw, K.N.; Siddique, J.; Boehm, J.K.; Kubzansky, L.D.; Diez-Roux, A.; Ning, H.; Lloyd-Jones, D.M. Optimism and cardiovascular health: Multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis (MESA). Health Behav. Policy Rev. 2015, 2, 62–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Matsunaga, M.; Isowa, T.; Yamakawa, K.; Tsuboi, H.; Kawanishi, Y.; Kaneko, H.; Kasugai, K.; Yoneda, M.; Ohira, H. Association between perceived happiness levels and peripheral circulating pro-inflammatory cytokine levels in middle-aged adults in Japan. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 2011, 32, 458–463. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  7. Sarris, J.; Logan, A.C.; Akbaraly, T.N.; Amminger, G.P.; Balanza-Martinez, V.; Freeman, M.P.; Hibbeln, J.; Matsuoka, Y.; Mischoulon, D.; Mizoue, T.; et al. Nutritional medicine as mainstream in psychiatry. Lancet Psychiatry 2015, 2, 271–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. United Nations General Assembly. Convention on the rights of the child. In United Nations Treaty Series; United Nations General Assembly: New York, NY, USA, 1989; Volume 1577, p. 3. [Google Scholar]
  9. Corburn, J. Confronting the challenges in reconnecting urban planning and public health. Am. J. Public Health 2004, 94, 541–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Heindel, J.J.; Balbus, J.; Birnbaum, L.; Brune-Drisse, M.N.; Grandjean, P.; Gray, K.; Landrigan, P.J.; Sly, P.D.; Suk, W.; Cory Slechta, D.; et al. Developmental origins of health and disease: Integrating environmental influences. Endocrinology 2015, 156, 3416–3421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Chavatte-Palmer, P.; Tarrade, A.; Rousseau-Ralliard, D. Diet before and during pregnancy and offspring health: The importance of animal models and what can be learned from them. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Antonelli, M.C.; Pallares, M.E.; Ceccatelli, S.; Spulber, S. Long-term consequences of prenatal stress and neurotoxicants exposure on neurodevelopment. Prog. Neurobiol. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Short, A.K.; Fennell, K.A.; Perreau, V.M.; Fox, A.; O’Bryan, M.K.; Kim, J.H.; Bredy, T.W.; Pang, T.Y.; Hannan, A.J. Elevated paternal glucocorticoid exposure alters the small noncoding rna profile in sperm and modifies anxiety and depressive phenotypes in the offspring. Transl. Psychiatry 2016, 6, e837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Babenko, O.; Kovalchuk, I.; Metz, G.A. Stress-induced perinatal and transgenerational epigenetic programming of brain development and mental health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 48, 70–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Metz, G.A.; Ng, J.W.; Kovalchuk, I.; Olson, D.M. Ancestral experience as a game changer in stress vulnerability and disease outcomes. BioEssays News Rev. Mol. Cell. Dev. Biol. 2015, 37, 602–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Baxter, A.J.; Scott, K.M.; Ferrari, A.J.; Norman, R.E.; Vos, T.; Whiteford, H.A. Challenging the myth of an “epidemic” of common mental disorders: Trends in the global prevalence of anxiety and depression between 1990 and 2010. Depress Anxiety 2014, 31, 506–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Cohen, S.; Janicki-Deverts, D. Who’s stressed? Distributions of psychological stress in the United States in probability samples from 1983, 2006, and 2009. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 42, 1320–1334. [Google Scholar]
  18. Boak, A.; Hamilton, H.A.; Adlaf, E.M.; Henderson, J.L.; Mann, R.E. The Mental Well-Being of Ontario Students, 1991–2015; CAMH Research Document No. 43; The Centre for Addiction and Mental Health: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  19. Rowlands, I.J.; Dobson, A.J.; Mishra, G.D. Physical health of young, australian women: A comparison of two national cohorts surveyed 17 years apart. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0142088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Twenge, J.M. Time period and birth cohort differences in depressive symptoms in the U.S., 1982–2013. Soc. Indic Res. 2015, 121, 437–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Nordstrom, A.; Bodlund, O. Every third patient in primary care suffers from depression, anxiety or alcohol problems. Nord. J. Psychiatry 2008, 62, 250–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Safer, D.J.; Rajakannan, T.; Burcu, M.; Zito, J.M. Trends in subthreshold psychiatric diagnoses for youth in community treatment. JAMA Psychiatry 2015, 72, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Roberts, R.E.; Fisher, P.W.; Turner, J.B.; Tang, M. Estimating the burden of psychiatric disorders in adolescence: The impact of subthreshold disorders. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2015, 50, 397–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Park, S.; Kim, B.N.; Cho, S.C.; Kim, J.W.; Shin, M.S.; Yoo, H.J. Prevalence, correlates, and comorbidities of DSM-IV psychiatric disorders in children in Seoul, Korea. Asia Pac. J. Public Health 2015, 27, NP1942–NP1951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Haller, H.; Cramer, H.; Lauche, R.; Gass, F.; Dobos, G.J. The prevalence and burden of subthreshold generalized anxiety disorder: A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 2014, 14, 128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Rucci, P.; Gherardi, S.; Tansella, M.; Piccinelli, M.; Berardi, D.; Bisoffi, G.; Corsino, M.A.; Pini, S. Subthreshold psychiatric disorders in primary care: Prevalence and associated characteristics. J. Affect. Disord. 2003, 76, 171–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Twenge, J.M. Overwhelming evidence for generation me. Emerg. Adulthood 2013, 1, 21–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Saeed, A.; Raana, T.; Saeed, A.M.; Humayun, A. Effect of antenatal depression on maternal dietary intake and neonatal outcome: A prospective cohort. Nutr. J. 2016, 15, 64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Liou, S.R.; Wang, P.; Cheng, C.Y. Effects of prenatal maternal mental distress on birth outcomes. Women Birth 2016, 29, 376–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Venkatesh, K.K.; Riley, L.; Castro, V.M.; Perlis, R.H.; Kaimal, A.J. Association of antenatal depression symptoms and antidepressant treatment with preterm birth. Obstet. Gynecol. 2016, 127, 926–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Nosarti, C.; Reichenberg, A.; Murray, R.M.; Cnattingius, S.; Lambe, M.P.; Yin, L.; MacCabe, J.; Rifkin, L.; Hultman, C.M. Preterm birth and psychiatric disorders in young adult life. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2012, 69, E1–E8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Lund, L.K.; Vik, T.; Lydersen, S.; Lohaugen, G.C.; Skranes, J.; Brubakk, A.M.; Indredavik, M.S. Mental health, quality of life and social relations in young adults born with low birth weight. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 2012, 10, 146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Hack, M.; Youngstrom, E.A.; Cartar, L.; Schluchter, M.; Taylor, H.G.; Flannery, D.; Klein, N.; Borawski, E. Behavioral outcomes and evidence of psychopathology among very low birth weight infants at age 20 years. Pediatrics 2004, 114, 932–940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Boyle, M.H.; Miskovic, V.; van Lieshout, R.; Duncan, L.; Schmidt, L.A.; Hoult, L.; Paneth, N.; Saigal, S. Psychopathology in young adults born at extremely low birth weight. Psychol. Med. 2011, 41, 1763–1774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Lund, L.K.; Vik, T.; Skranes, J.; Lydersen, S.; Brubakk, A.M.; Indredavik, M.S. Low birth weight and psychiatric morbidity; stability and change between adolescence and young adulthood. Early Hum. Dev. 2012, 88, 623–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Abel, K.M.; Wicks, S.; Susser, E.S.; Dalman, C.; Pedersen, M.G.; Mortensen, P.B.; Webb, R.T. Birth weight, schizophrenia, and adult mental disorder: Is risk confined to the smallest babies? Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2010, 67, 923–930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Moster, D.; Lie, R.T.; Markestad, T. Long-term medical and social consequences of preterm birth. N. Engl. J. Med. 2008, 359, 262–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. D’Onofrio, B.M.; Class, Q.A.; Rickert, M.E.; Larsson, H.; Langstrom, N.; Lichtenstein, P. Preterm birth and mortality and morbidity: A population-based quasi-experimental study. JAMA Psychiatry 2013, 70, 1231–1240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Monfils Gustafsson, W.; Josefsson, A.; Ekholm Selling, K.; Sydsjo, G. Preterm birth or foetal growth impairment and psychiatric hospitalization in adolescence and early adulthood in a Swedish population-based birth cohort. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2009, 119, 54–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Raikkonen, K.; Pesonen, A.K.; Heinonen, K.; Kajantie, E.; Hovi, P.; Jarvenpaa, A.L.; Eriksson, J.G.; Andersson, S. Depression in young adults with very low birth weight: The Helsinki study of very low-birth-weight adults. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2008, 65, 290–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Heinonen, K.; Raikkonen, K.; Pesonen, A.K.; Andersson, S.; Kajantie, E.; Eriksson, J.G.; Wolke, D.; Lano, A. Behavioural symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preterm and term children born small and appropriate for gestational age: A longitudinal study. BMC Pediatr. 2010, 10, 91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Strang-Karlsson, S.; Raikkonen, K.; Pesonen, A.K.; Kajantie, E.; Paavonen, E.J.; Lahti, J.; Hovi, P.; Heinonen, K.; Jarvenpaa, A.L.; Eriksson, J.G.; et al. Very low birth weight and behavioral symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young adulthood: The Helsinki study of very-low-birth-weight adults. Am. J. Psychiatry 2008, 165, 1345–1353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Luby, J.L.; Gaffrey, M.S.; Tillman, R.; April, L.M.; Belden, A.C. Trajectories of preschool disorders to full DSM depression at school age and early adolescence: Continuity of preschool depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 2014, 171, 768–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Hill, R.M.; Pettit, J.W.; Lewinsohn, P.M.; Seeley, J.R.; Klein, D.N. Escalation to major depressive disorder among adolescents with subthreshold depressive symptoms: Evidence of distinct subgroups at risk. J. Affect. Disord. 2014, 158, 133–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Goldney, R.D.; Fisher, L.J.; Dal Grande, E.; Taylor, A.W. Subsyndromal depression: Prevalence, use of health services and quality of life in an Australian population. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2004, 39, 293–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Rodriguez, M.R.; Nuevo, R.; Chatterji, S.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L. Definitions and factors associated with subthreshold depressive conditions: A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 2012, 12, 181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Braungart-Rieker, J.M.; Lefever, J.B.; Planalp, E.M.; Moore, E.S. Body mass index at 3 years of age: Cascading effects of prenatal maternal depression and mother-infant dynamics. J. Pediatr. 2016, 177, 128–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Barroso, N.E.; Hartley, C.M.; Bagner, D.M.; Pettit, J.W. The effect of preterm birth on infant negative affect and maternal postpartum depressive symptoms: A preliminary examination in an underrepresented minority sample. Infant Behav. Dev. 2015, 39, 159–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Winett, L.B.; Wulf, A.B.; Wallack, L. Framing strategies to avoid mother-blame in communicating the origins of chronic disease. Am. J. Public Health 2016, 106, 1369–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Winett, L.; Wallack, L.; Richardson, D.; Boone-Heinonen, J.; Messer, L. A framework to address challenges in communicating the developmental origins of health and disease. Curr. Environ. Health Rep. 2016, 3, 169–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Caruso, E.M.; Vohs, K.D.; Baxter, B.; Waytz, A. Mere exposure to money increases endorsement of free-market systems and social inequality. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2013, 142, 301–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Wright, D. More equal societies have less mental illness: What should therapists do on monday morning? Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2014, 60, 436–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Glasgow, S.; Schrecker, T. The double burden of neoliberalism? Noncommunicable disease policies and the global political economy of risk. Health Place 2016, 39, 204–211. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  54. Prescott, S.L.; Logan, A.C.; Millstein, R.A.; Katszman, M.A. Biodiversity, the human microbiome and mental health: Moving toward a new clinical ecology for the 21st century? Int. J. Biodivers. 2016, 2016, 2718275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Penkalla, A.M.; Kohler, S. Urbanicity and mental health in europe: A systematic review. Eur. J. Ment. Health 2014, 9, 163–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Hyland, P.; Shevlin, M.; Elklit, A.; Christoffersen, M.; Murphy, J. Social, familial and psychological risk factors for mood and anxiety disorders in childhood and early adulthood: A birth cohort study using the danish registry system. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2016, 51, 331–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Allen, J.; Balfour, R.; Bell, R.; Marmot, M. Social determinants of mental health. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2014, 26, 392–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Delobel-Ayoub, M.; Ehlinger, V.; Klapouszczak, D.; Maffre, T.; Raynaud, J.P.; Delpierre, C.; Arnaud, C. Socioeconomic disparities and prevalence of autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0141964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Robinson, R.L.; Stephenson, J.J.; Dennehy, E.B.; Grabner, M.; Faries, D.; Palli, S.R.; Swindle, R.W. The importance of unresolved fatigue in depression: Costs and comorbidities. Psychosomatics 2015, 56, 274–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Roberts, J.; Donkin, A.M.M. Opportunities for reducing socioeconomic inequalities in the mental health of children and young people—Reducing adversity and increasing resilience. J. Public Ment. Health 2016, 15, 4–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Pickett, K.E.; Wilkinson, R.G. Inequality: An underacknowledged source of mental illness and distress. Br. J. Psychiatry J. Ment. Sci. 2010, 197, 426–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Tiikkaja, S.; Sandin, S.; Hultman, C.M.; Modin, B.; Malki, N.; Sparen, P. Psychiatric disorder and work life: A longitudinal study of intra-generational social mobility. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 2016, 62, 156–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Fuller-Thomson, E.; Agbeyaka, S.; LaFond, D.M.; Bern-Klug, M. Flourishing after depression: Factors associated with achieving complete mental health among those with a history of depression. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 242, 111–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Bonet, M.; Smith, L.K.; Pilkington, H.; Draper, E.S.; Zeitlin, J. Neighbourhood deprivation and very preterm birth in an English and French cohort. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 2013, 13, 97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Glinianaia, S.V.; Ghosh, R.; Rankin, J.; Pearce, M.S.; Parker, L.; Pless-Mulloli, T. No improvement in socioeconomic inequalities in birthweight and preterm birth over four decades: A population-based cohort study. BMC Public Health 2013, 13, 345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Potijk, M.R.; de Winter, A.F.; Bos, A.F.; Kerstjens, J.M.; Reijneveld, S.A. Behavioural and emotional problems in moderately preterm children with low socioeconomic status: A population-based study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2015, 24, 787–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Reeves, A.; McKee, M.; Mackenbach, J.; Whitehead, M.; Stuckler, D. Introduction of a national minimum wage reduced depressive symptoms in low-wage workers: A quasi-natural experiment in the UK. Health Econ. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Burmaster, K.B.; Landefeld, J.C.; Rehkopf, D.H.; Lahiff, M.; Sokal-Gutierrez, K.; Adler-Milstein, S.; Fernald, L.C. Impact of a private sector living wage intervention on depressive symptoms among apparel workers in the dominican republic: A quasi-experimental study. BMJ Open 2015, 5, e007336. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  69. Logan, A.C.; Katzman, M.A.; Balanza-Martinez, V. Natural environments, ancestral diets, and microbial ecology: Is there a modern “paleo-deficit disorder”? Part II. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2015, 34, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Logan, A.C.; Jacka, F.N. Nutritional psychiatry research: An emerging discipline and its intersection with global urbanization, environmental challenges and the evolutionary mismatch. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2014, 33, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Shivappa, N.; Schoenaker, D.A.; Hebert, J.R.; Mishra, G.D. Association between inflammatory potential of diet and risk of depression in middle-aged women: The Australian longitudinal study on women’s health. Br. J. Nutr. 2016, 116, 1077–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Akbaraly, T.N.; Kerlau, C.; Wyart, M.; Chevallier, N.; Ndiaye, L.; Shivappa, N.; Hebert, J.R.; Kivimaki, M. Dietary inflammatory index and recurrence of depressive symptoms: Results from the whitehall II study. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Jacka, F.N.; Ystrom, E.; Brantsaeter, A.L.; Karevold, E.; Roth, C.; Haugen, M.; Meltzer, H.M.; Schjolberg, S.; Berk, M. Maternal and early postnatal nutrition and mental health of offspring by age 5 years: A prospective cohort study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2013, 52, 1038–1047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Pina-Camacho, L.; Jensen, S.K.; Gaysina, D.; Barker, E.D. Maternal depression symptoms, unhealthy diet and child emotional-behavioural dysregulation. Psychol. Med. 2015, 45, 1851–1860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Purtell, K.M.; Gershoff, E.T. Fast food consumption and academic growth in late childhood. Clin. Pediatr. 2015, 54, 871–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Morris, M.J.; Beilharz, J.E.; Maniam, J.; Reichelt, A.C.; Westbrook, R.F. Why is obesity such a problem in the 21st century? The intersection of palatable food, cues and reward pathways, stress, and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 58, 36–45. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  77. Zeeni, N.; Bassil, M.; Fromentin, G.; Chaumontet, C.; Darcel, N.; Tome, D.; Daher, C.F. Environmental enrichment and cafeteria diet attenuate the response to chronic variable stress in rats. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 139, 41–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Tryon, M.S.; Stanhope, K.L.; Epel, E.S.; Mason, A.E.; Brown, R.; Medici, V.; Havel, P.J.; Laugero, K.D. Excessive sugar consumption may be a difficult habit to break: A view from the brain and body. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2015, 100, 2239–2247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Van Oudenhove, L.; McKie, S.; Lassman, D.; Uddin, B.; Paine, P.; Coen, S.; Gregory, L.; Tack, J.; Aziz, Q. Fatty acid-induced gut-brain signaling attenuates neural and behavioral effects of sad emotion in humans. J. Clin. Investig. 2011, 121, 3094–3099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Weltens, N.; Zhao, D.; van Oudenhove, L. Where is the comfort in comfort foods? Mechanisms linking fat signaling, reward, and emotion. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 2014, 26, 303–315. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  81. St-Onge, M.P.; Roberts, A.; Shechter, A.; Choudhury, A.R. Fiber and saturated fat are associated with sleep arousals and slow wave sleep. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2016, 12, 19–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Li, R.; Zhang, J.; Zhou, R.; Liu, J.; Dai, Z.; Liu, D.; Wang, Y.; Zhang, H.; Li, Y.; Zeng, G. Sleep disturbances during pregnancy are associated with cesarean delivery and preterm birth. J. Matern. Fetal Neonatal Med. 2016, 16, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Krawczak, E.M.; Minuzzi, L.; Hidalgo, M.P.; Frey, B.N. Do changes in subjective sleep and biological rhythms predict worsening in postpartum depressive symptoms? A prospective study across the perinatal period. Arch. Womens Ment. Health 2016, 19, 591–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Okun, M.L. Disturbed sleep and postpartum depression. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2016, 18, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Da Costa, D.; Zelkowitz, P.; Dasgupta, K.; Sewitch, M.; Lowensteyn, I.; Cruz, R.; Hennegan, K.; Khalife, S. Dads get sad too: Depressive symptoms and associated factors in expectant first-time fathers. Am. J. Men’s Health 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Armitage, R.; Flynn, H.; Hoffmann, R.; Vazquez, D.; Lopez, J.; Marcus, S. Early developmental changes in sleep in infants: The impact of maternal depression. Sleep 2009, 32, 693–696. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  87. Hairston, I.S.; Solnik-Menilo, T.; Deviri, D.; Handelzalts, J.E. Maternal depressed mood moderates the impact of infant sleep on mother-infant bonding. Arch. Womens Ment. Health 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Hairston, I.S.; Waxler, E.; Seng, J.S.; Fezzey, A.G.; Rosenblum, K.L.; Muzik, M. The role of infant sleep in intergenerational transmission of trauma. Sleep 2011, 34, 1373–1383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Goldfield, G.S.; Murray, M.; Maras, D.; Wilson, A.L.; Phillips, P.; Kenny, G.P.; Hadjiyannakis, S.; Alberga, A.; Cameron, J.D.; Tulluch, H.; et al. Screen time is associated with depressive symptomatology among obese adolescents: A hearty study. Eur. J. Pediatr. 2016, 175, 909–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Grontved, A.; Singhammer, J.; Froberg, K.; Moller, N.C.; Pan, A.; Pfeiffer, K.A.; Kristensen, P.L. A prospective study of screen time in adolescence and depression symptoms in young adulthood. Prev. Med. 2015, 81, 108–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Xu, H.L.; Wen, L.M.; Hardy, L.L.; Rissel, C. Associations of outdoor play and screen time with nocturnal sleep duration and pattern among young children. Acta Paediatr. 2016, 105, 297–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Trinh, L.; Wong, B.; Faulkner, G.E. The independent and interactive associations of screen time and physical activity on mental health, school connectedness and academic achievement among a population-based sample of youth. J. Can. Acad. Child Adolesc. 2015, 24, 17–24. [Google Scholar]
  93. Ferraro, F.R.; Weatherly, J.N. Texting dependence, ipod dependence, and delay discounting. Am. J. Psychol. 2016, 129, 161–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Costigan, S.A.; Barnett, L.; Plotnikoff, R.C.; Lubans, D.R. The health indicators associated with screen-based sedentary behavior among adolescent girls: A systematic review. J. Adolesc. Health 2013, 52, 382–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Cao, H.; Qian, Q.; Weng, T.; Yuan, C.; Sun, Y.; Wang, H.; Tao, F. Screen time, physical activity and mental health among urban adolescents in China. Prev. Med. 2011, 53, 316–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Feng, Q.; Zhang, Q.L.; Du, Y.; Ye, Y.L.; He, Q.Q. Associations of physical activity, screen time with depression, anxiety and sleep quality among chinese college freshmen. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e100914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Schule, S.A.; von Kries, R.; Fromme, H.; Bolte, G. Neighbourhood socioeconomic context, individual socioeconomic position, and overweight in young children: A multilevel study in a large german city. BMC Obes. 2016, 3, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Ellaway, A.; Benzeval, M.; Green, M.; Leyland, A.; Macintyre, S. “Getting sicker quicker”: Does living in a more deprived neighbourhood mean your health deteriorates faster? Health Place 2012, 18, 132–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Giurgescu, C.; Misra, D.P.; Sealy-Jefferson, S.; Caldwell, C.H.; Templin, T.N.; Slaughter-Acey, J.C.; Osypuk, T.L. The impact of neighborhood quality, perceived stress, and social support on depressive symptoms during pregnancy in African American women. Soc. Sci. Med. 2015, 130, 172–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Chor, D.; Cardoso, L.O.; Nobre, A.A.; Griep, R.H.; Fonseca Mde, J.; Giatti, L.; Bensenor, I.; Del Carmen Bisi Molina, M.; Aquino, E.M.; Diez-Roux, A.; et al. Association between perceived neighbourhood characteristics, physical activity and diet quality: Results of the brazilian longitudinal study of adult health (Elsa-Brasil). BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Giese, H.; Konig, L.M.; Taut, D.; Ollila, H.; Baban, A.; Absetz, P.; Schupp, H.; Renner, B. Exploring the association between television advertising of healthy and unhealthy foods, self-control, and food intake in three European countries. Appl. Psychol. Health Well 2015, 7, 41–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Vohs, K.D. Psychology. The poor’s poor mental power. Science 2013, 341, 969–970. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  103. Mani, A.; Mullainathan, S.; Shafir, E.; Zhao, J. Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science 2013, 341, 976–980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Bratanova, B.; Loughnan, S.; Klein, O.; Claassen, A.; Wood, R. Poverty, inequality, and increased consumption of high calorie food: Experimental evidence for a causal link. Appetite 2016, 100, 162–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Cardel, M.I.; Johnson, S.L.; Beck, J.; Dhurandhar, E.; Keita, A.D.; Tomczik, A.C.; Pavela, G.; Huo, T.; Janicke, D.M.; Muller, K.; et al. The effects of experimentally manipulated social status on acute eating behavior: A randomized, crossover pilot study. Physiol. Behav. 2016, 162, 93–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Searle, A.; Calnan, M.; Lewis, G.; Campbell, J.; Taylor, A.; Turner, K. Patients’ views of physical activity as treatment for depression: A qualitative study. Br. J. Gen. Pract. 2011, 61, 149–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Marcora, S.M.; Staiano, W.; Manning, V. Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. (1985) 2009, 106, 857–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. Blakely, M.J.; Kemp, S.; Helton, W.S. Volitional running and tone counting: The impact of cognitive load on running over natural terrain. Trans. Occup. Ergon. Hum. Factors 2016, 4, 104–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Thogersen-Ntoumani, C.; Shepherd, S.O.; Ntoumanis, N.; Wagenmakers, A.J.; Shaw, C.S. Intrinsic motivation in two exercise interventions: Associations with fitness and body composition. Health Psychol. 2016, 35, 195–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Teixeira, P.J.; Carraca, E.V.; Markland, D.; Silva, M.N.; Ryan, R.M. Exercise, physical activity, and self-determination theory: A systematic review. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2012, 9, 78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Ben Cheikh, R.; Latiri, I.; Dogui, M.; Ben Saad, H. Effects of one night sleep deprivation on selective attention and isometric force in adolescent karate athletes. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fit. 2016, in press. [Google Scholar]
  112. Ekkekakis, P.; Parfitt, G.; Petruzzello, S.J. The pleasure and displeasure people feel when they exercise at different intensities decennial update and progress towards a tripartite rationale for exercise intensity prescription. Sports Med. 2011, 41, 641–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Weinstein, A.A.; Deuster, P.A.; Francis, J.L.; Beadling, C.; Kop, W.J. The role of depression in short-term mood and fatigue responses to acute exercise. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2010, 17, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Huebschmann, A.G.; Kohrt, W.M.; Herlache, L.; Wolfe, P.; Daugherty, S.; Reusch, J.E.; Bauer, T.A.; Regensteiner, J.G. Type 2 diabetes exaggerates exercise effort and impairs exercise performance in older women. BMJ Open Diabetes Res. Care 2015, 3, e000124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Annesi, J.J. Relations of self-motivation, perceived physical condition, and exercise-induced changes in revitalization and exhaustion with attendance in women initiating a moderate cardiovascular exercise regimen. Women Health 2005, 42, 77–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Kwan, B.M.; Bryan, A. In-task and post-task affective response to exercise: Translating exercise intentions into behaviour. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2010, 15, 115–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  117. Sarmugam, R.; Worsley, A. Dietary behaviours, impulsivity and food involvement: Identification of three consumer segments. Nutrients 2015, 7, 8036–8057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Lawyer, S.R.; Boomhower, S.R.; Rasmussen, E.B. Differential associations between obesity and behavioral measures of impulsivity. Appetite 2015, 95, 375–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Joireman, J.; King, S. Individual differences in the consideration of future and (more) immediate consequences: A review and directions for future research. Soc. Personal. Psychol. 2016, 10, 313–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Ishii, K. Subjective socioeconomic status and cigarette smoking interact to delay discounting. Springerplus 2015, 4, 560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Lovallo, W.R.; Farag, N.H.; Sorocco, K.H.; Acheson, A.; Cohoon, A.J.; Vincent, A.S. Early life adversity contributes to impaired cognition and impulsive behavior: Studies from the Oklahoma family health patterns project. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2013, 37, 616–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Acheson, A.; Vincent, A.S.; Sorocco, K.H.; Lovallo, W.R. Greater discounting of delayed rewards in young adults with family histories of alcohol and drug use disorders: Studies from the Oklahoma family health patterns project. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2011, 35, 1607–1613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Fields, S.A.; Lange, K.; Ramos, A.; Thamotharan, S.; Rassu, F. The relationship between stress and delay discounting: A meta-analytic review. Behav. Pharmacol. 2014, 25, 434–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  124. Worthy, D.A.; Byrne, K.A.; Fields, S. Effects of emotion on prospection during decision-making. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. DeVoe, S.E.; House, J.; Zhong, C.B. Fast food and financial impatience: A socioecological approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 105, 476–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. House, J.; DeVoe, S.E.; Zhong, C.B. Too impatient to smell the roses: Exposure to fast food impedes happiness. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2014, 5, 534–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Van der Wal, A.J.; Schade, H.M.; Krabbendam, L.; van Vugt, M. Do natural landscapes reduce future discounting in humans? Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2013, 280, 20132295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Berry, M.S.; Sweeney, M.M.; Morath, J.; Odum, A.L.; Jordan, K.E. The nature of impulsivity: Visual exposure to natural environments decreases impulsive decision-making in a delay discounting task. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e97915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  129. Berry, M.S.; Repke, M.A.; Nickerson, N.P.; Conway, L.G.; Odum, A.L.; Jordan, K.E. Making time for nature: Visual exposure to natural environments lengthens subjective time perception and reduces impulsivity. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0141030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Cheng, Y.Y.; Shein, P.P.; Chiou, W.B. Escaping the impulse to immediate gratification: The prospect concept promotes a future-oriented mindset, prompting an inclination towards delayed gratification. Br. J. Psychol. 2012, 103, 129–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Dassen, F.C.M.; Jansen, A.; Nederkoorn, C.; Houben, K. Focus on the future: Episodic future thinking reduces discount rate and snacking. Appetite 2016, 96, 327–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Martin, L.E.; Pollack, L.; McCune, A.; Schulte, E.; Savage, C.R.; Lundgren, J.D. Comparison of obese adults with poor versus good sleep quality during a functional neuroimaging delay discounting task: A pilot study. Psychiatry Res. Imaging 2015, 234, 90–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  133. Engle-Friedman, M. The effects of sleep loss on capacity and effort. Sleep Sci. 2014, 7, 213–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  134. Libedinsky, C.; Massar, S.A.; Ling, A.; Chee, W.; Huettel, S.A.; Chee, M.W. Sleep deprivation alters effort discounting but not delay discounting of monetary rewards. Sleep 2013, 36, 899–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  135. Chan, W.S. Delay discounting and response disinhibition moderate associations between actigraphically measured sleep parameters and body mass index. J. Sleep Res. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Park, N.; Peterson, C.; Szvarca, D.; Vander Molen, R.J.; Kim, E.S.; Collon, K. Positive psychology and physical health: Research and applications. Am. J. Lifestyle Med. 2016, 10, 200–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Steptoe, A.; O’Donnell, K.; Marmot, M.; Wardle, J. Positive affect, psychological well-being, and good sleep. J. Psychosom. Res. 2008, 64, 409–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  138. Voellmin, A.; Entringer, S.; Moog, N.; Wadhwa, P.D.; Buss, C. Maternal positive affect over the course of pregnancy is associated with the length of gestation and reduced risk of preterm delivery. J. Psychosom. Res. 2013, 75, 336–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  139. Coffey, J.K.; Warren, M.T.; Gottfried, A.W. Does infant happiness forecast adult life satisfaction? Examining subjective well-being in the first quarter century of life. J. Happiness Stud. 2015, 16, 1401–1421. [Google Scholar]
  140. Kansky, J.; Allen, J.P.; Diener, E. Early adolescent affect predicts later life outcomes. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 2016, 8, 192–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  141. Stellar, J.E.; John-Henderson, N.; Anderson, C.L.; Gordon, A.M.; McNeil, G.D.; Keltner, D. Positive affect and markers of inflammation: Discrete positive emotions predict lower levels of inflammatory cytokines. Emotion 2015, 15, 129–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  142. Kim, D.; Kubzansky, L.D.; Baccarelli, A.; Sparrow, D.; Spiro, A., 3rd; Tarantini, L.; Cantone, L.; Vokonas, P.; Schwartz, J. Psychological factors and DNA methylation of genes related to immune/inflammatory system markers: The VA normative aging study. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e009790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  143. Reininghaus, E.Z.; Lackner, N.; Birner, A.; Bengesser, S.; Fellendorf, F.T.; Platzer, M.; Rieger, A.; Queissner, R.; Kainzbauer, N.; Reininghaus, B.; et al. Extracellular matrix proteins matrix metallopeptidase 9 (mmp9) and soluble intercellular adhesion molecule 1 (sicam-1) and correlations with clinical staging in euthymic bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2016, 18, 155–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  144. Wirtz, P.H.; Redwine, L.S.; Linke, S.; Hong, S.; Rutledge, T.; Greenberg, B.H.; Mills, P.J. Circulating levels of soluble intercellular adhesion molecule-1 (sicam-1) independently predict depressive symptom severity after 12 months in heart failure patients. Brain Behav. Immun. 2010, 24, 366–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  145. Sin, N.L.; Graham-Engeland, J.E.; Almeida, D.M. Daily positive events and inflammation: Findings from the national study of daily experiences. Brain Behav. Immun. 2015, 43, 130–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  146. Sin, N.L.; Graham-Engeland, J.E.; Ong, A.D.; Almeida, D.M. Affective reactivity to daily stressors is associated with elevated inflammation. Health Psychol. 2015, 34, 1154–1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  147. Roy, B.; Diez-Roux, A.V.; Seeman, T.; Ranjit, N.; Shea, S.; Cushman, M. Association of optimism and pessimism with inflammation and hemostasis in the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis (mesa). Psychosom. Med. 2010, 72, 134–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  148. Brydon, L.; Walker, C.; Wawrzyniak, A.J.; Chart, H.; Steptoe, A. Dispositional optimism and stress-induced changes in immunity and negative mood. Brain Behav. Immun. 2009, 23, 810–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  149. Boehm, J.K.; Chen, Y.; Williams, D.R.; Ryff, C.; Kubzansky, L.D. Unequally distributed psychological assets: Are there social disparities in optimism, life satisfaction, and positive affect? PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0118066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  150. Boehm, J.K.; Williams, D.R.; Rimm, E.B.; Ryff, C.; Kubzansky, L.D. Association between optimism and serum antioxidants in the midlife in the united states study. Psychosom. Med. 2013, 75, 2–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  151. Hingle, M.D.; Wertheim, B.C.; Tindle, H.A.; Tinker, L.; Seguin, R.A.; Rosal, M.C.; Thomson, C.A. Optimism and diet quality in the women’s health initiative. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2014, 114, 1036–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  152. Lau, E.Y.; Harry Hui, C.; Cheung, S.F.; Lam, J. Bidirectional relationship between sleep and optimism with depressive mood as a mediator: A longitudinal study of chinese working adults. J. Psychosom. Res. 2015, 79, 428–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  153. Cohen, R.; Bavishi, C.; Rozanski, A. Purpose in life and its relationship to all-cause mortality and cardiovascular events: A meta-analysis. Psychosom. Med. 2016, 78, 122–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  154. Strecher, V.; Terry, P. Health and life purpose: What’s the connection? Am. J. Health Promot. 2014, 29, TAHP2–TAHP4. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  155. Burrow, A.L.; Spreng, R.N. Waiting with purpose: A reliable but small association between purpose in life and impulsivity. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2016, 90, 187–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  156. Zilioli, S.; Slatcher, R.B.; Ong, A.D.; Gruenewald, T.L. Purpose in life predicts allostatic load ten years later. J. Psychosom. Res. 2015, 79, 451–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  157. Kim, E.S.; Strecher, V.J.; Ryff, C.D. Purpose in life and use of preventive health care services. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2014, 111, 16331–16336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  158. Lamis, D.A.; Wilson, C.K.; Tarantino, N.; Lansford, J.E.; Kaslow, N.J. Neighborhood disorder, spiritual well-being, and parenting stress in African American women. J. Fam. Psychol. 2014, 28, 769–778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  159. Williams, A.; O’Driscoll, K.; Moore, C. The influence of empathic concern on prosocial behavior in children. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  160. Bourgault, P.; Lavoie, S.; Paul-Savoie, E.; Gregoire, M.; Michaud, C.; Gosselin, E.; Johnston, C.C. Relationship between empathy and well-being among emergency nurses. J. Emerg. Nurs. 2015, 41, 323–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  161. Ma-Kellams, C.; Lerner, J. Trust your gut or think carefully? Examining whether an intuitive, versus a systematic, mode of thought produces greater empathic accuracy. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2016, in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  162. Allemand, M.; Steiger, A.E.; Fend, H.A. Empathy development in adolescence predicts social competencies in adulthood. J. Pers. 2015, 83, 229–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  163. Di Fabio, A.; Bucci, O. Green positive guidance and green positive life counseling for decent work and decent lives: Some empirical results. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  164. Schreier, H.M.; Schonert-Reichl, K.A.; Chen, E. Effect of volunteering on risk factors for cardiovascular disease in adolescents: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA Pediatr. 2013, 167, 327–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  165. Dubos, R. Adapting to pollution. Sci. Citiz. 1968, 10, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  166. Mantler, A.; Logan, A.C. Natural environments and mental health. Adv. Integr. Med. 2015, 2, 5–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  167. Craig, J.M.; Logan, A.C.; Prescott, S.L. Natural environments, nature relatedness and the ecological theater: Connecting satellites and sequencing to Shinrin-Yoku. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2016, 35, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  168. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of nature therapy: A review of the research in Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  169. James, P.; Hart, J.E.; Banay, R.F.; Laden, F. Exposure to greenness and mortality in a nationwide prospective cohort study of women. Environ. Health Perspect. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  170. Mitchell, R.; Popham, F. Effect of exposure to natural environment on health inequalities: An observational population study. Lancet 2008, 372, 1655–1660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  171. Dadvand, P.; Wright, J.; Martinez, D.; Basagana, X.; McEachan, R.R.; Cirach, M.; Gidlow, C.J.; de Hoogh, K.; Grazuleviciene, R.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Inequality, green spaces, and pregnant women: Roles of ethnicity and individual and neighbourhood socioeconomic status. Environ. Int. 2014, 71, 101–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  172. Hystad, P.; Davies, H.W.; Frank, L.; van Loon, J.; Gehring, U.; Tamburic, L.; Brauer, M. Residential greenness and birth outcomes: Evaluating the influence of spatially correlated built-environment factors. Environ. Health Perspect. 2014, 122, 1095–1102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  173. Grazuleviciene, R.; Danileviciute, A.; Dedele, A.; Vencloviene, J.; Andrusaityte, S.; Uzdanaviciute, I.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Surrounding greenness, proximity to city parks and pregnancy outcomes in Kaunas cohort study. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 2015, 218, 358–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  174. Ebisu, K.; Holford, T.R.; Bell, M.L. Association between greenness, urbanicity, and birth weight. Sci. Total Environ. 2016, 542, 750–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  175. Casey, J.A.; James, P.; Rudolph, K.E.; Wu, C.D.; Schwartz, B.S. Greenness and birth outcomes in a range of pennsylvania communities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  176. Laurent, O.; Wu, J.; Li, L.; Milesi, C. Green spaces and pregnancy outcomes in southern California. Health Place 2013, 24, 190–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  177. Dadvand, P.; de Nazelle, A.; Figueras, F.; Basagana, X.; Su, J.; Amoly, E.; Jerrett, M.; Vrijheid, M.; Sunyer, J.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Green space, health inequality and pregnancy. Environ. Int. 2012, 40, 110–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  178. Dadvand, P.; Sunyer, J.; Basagana, X.; Ballester, F.; Lertxundi, A.; Fernandez-Somoano, A.; Estarlich, M.; Garcia-Esteban, R.; Mendez, M.A.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Surrounding greenness and pregnancy outcomes in four Spanish birth cohorts. Environ. Health Perspect. 2012, 120, 1481–1487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  179. Kihal-Talantikite, W.; Padilla, C.M.; Lalloue, B.; Gelormini, M.; Zmirou-Navier, D.; Deguen, S. Green space, social inequalities and neonatal mortality in France. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 2013, 13, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  180. Markevych, I.; Fuertes, E.; Tiesler, C.M.; Birk, M.; Bauer, C.P.; Koletzko, S.; von Berg, A.; Berdel, D.; Heinrich, J. Surrounding greenness and birth weight: Results from the Giniplus and Lisaplus birth cohorts in Munich. Health Place 2014, 26, 39–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  181. Cusack, L.; Larkin, A.; Carozza, S.; Hystad, P. Associations between residential greenness and birth outcomes across texas. Environ. Res. 2016, 152, 88–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  182. Sbihi, H.; Tamburic, L.; Koehoorn, M.; Brauer, M. Greenness and incident childhood asthma: A 10-year follow-up in a population-based birth cohort. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 2015, 192, 1131–1133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  183. McEachan, R.R.; Prady, S.L.; Smith, G.; Fairley, L.; Cabieses, B.; Gidlow, C.; Wright, J.; Dadvand, P.; van Gent, D.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. The association between green space and depressive symptoms in pregnant women: Moderating roles of socioeconomic status and physical activity. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2016, 70, 253–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  184. Kim, J.H.; Lee, C.; Sohn, W. Urban natural environments, obesity, and health-related quality of life among hispanic children living in inner-city neighborhoods. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  185. Younan, D.; Tuvblad, C.; Li, L.F.; Wu, J.; Lurmann, F.; Franklin, M.; Berhane, K.; McConnell, R.; Wu, A.H.; Baker, L.A.; et al. Environmental determinants of aggression in adolescents: Role of urban neighborhood greenspace. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2016, 55, 591–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  186. Gariepy, G.; Kaufman, J.S.; Blair, A.; Kestens, Y.; Schmitz, N. Place and health in diabetes: The neighbourhood environment and risk of depression in adults with type 2 diabetes. Diabet. Med. J. Br. Diabet. Assoc. 2015, 32, 944–950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  187. Maas, J.; Verheij, R.A.; de Vries, S.; Spreeuwenberg, P.; Schellevis, F.G.; Groenewegen, P.P. Morbidity is related to a green living environment. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2009, 63, 967–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  188. Beyer, K.M.M.; Kaltenbach, A.; Szabo, A.; Bogar, S.; Nieto, F.J.; Malecki, K.M. Exposure to neighborhood green space and mental health: Evidence from the survey of the health of Wisconsin. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 3453–3472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  189. Bodicoat, D.H.; O’Donovan, G.; Dalton, A.M.; Gray, L.J.; Yates, T.; Edwardson, C.; Hill, S.; Webb, D.R.; Khunti, K.; Davies, M.J.; et al. The association between neighbourhood greenspace and type 2 diabetes in a large cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 2014, 4, e006076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  190. Cohen-Cline, H.; Turkheimer, E.; Duncan, G.E. Access to green space, physical activity and mental health: A twin study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2015, 69, 523–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  191. Nutsford, D.; Pearson, A.L.; Kingham, S. An ecological study investigating the association between access to urban green space and mental health. Public Health 2013, 127, 1005–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  192. Alcock, I.; White, M.P.; Wheeler, B.W.; Fleming, L.E.; Depledge, M.H. Longitudinal effects on mental health of moving to greener and less green urban areas. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2014, 48, 1247–1255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  193. Mills, J.R.; Cunningham, P.; Donovan, G.H. Urban forests and social inequality in the Pacific Northwest. Urban For. Urban Green. 2016, 16, 188–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  194. Arroyo-Johnson, C.; Woodward, K.; Milam, L.; Ackermann, N.; Komaie, G.; Goodman, M.S.; Hipp, J.A. Still separate, still unequal: Social determinants of playground safety and proximity disparities in St. Louis. J. Urban Health Bull. N. Y. Acad. Med. 2016, 139–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  195. Leong, M.; Bertone, M.A.; Bayless, K.M.; Dunn, R.R.; Trautwein, M.D. Exoskeletons and economics: Indoor arthropod diversity increases in affluent neighbourhoods. Biol. Lett. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  196. Zach, A.; Meyer, N.; Hendrowarsito, L.; Kolb, S.; Bolte, G.; Nennstiel-Ratzel, U.; Stilianakis, N.I.; Herr, C.; Grp, G.S. Association of sociodemographic and environmental factors with the mental health status among preschool children-results from a cross-sectional study in Bavaria, Germany. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 2016, 219, 458–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  197. Cohen, D.A.; Han, B.; Nagel, C.J.; Harnik, P.; McKenzie, T.L.; Evenson, K.R.; Marsh, T.; Williamson, S.; Vaughan, C.; Katta, S. The first national study of neighborhood parks: Implications for physical activity. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2016, 51, 419–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  198. Salmon, J.; Veitch, J.; Abbott, G.; ChinAPaw, M.; Brug, J.J.; teVelde, S.J.; Cleland, V.; Hume, C.; Crawford, D.; Ball, K. Are associations between the perceived home and neighbourhood environment and children’s physical activity and sedentary behaviour moderated by urban/rural location? Health Place 2013, 24, 44–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  199. Tandon, P.S.; Zhou, C.; Sallis, J.F.; Cain, K.L.; Frank, L.D.; Saelens, B.E. Home environment relationships with children’s physical activity, sedentary time, and screen time by socioeconomic status. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. 2012, 9, 88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  200. McCormack, G.R.; Mardinger, C. Neighbourhood urban form and individual-level correlates of leisure-based screen time in Canadian adults. BMJ Open 2015, 5, e009418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  201. Ding, D.; Sugiyama, T.; Winkler, E.; Cerin, E.; Wijndaele, K.; Owen, N. Correlates of change in adults’ television viewing time: A four-year follow-up study. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2012, 44, 1287–1292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  202. Sugiyama, T.; Salmon, J.; Dunstan, D.W.; Bauman, A.E.; Owen, N. Neighborhood walkability and TV viewing time among Australian adults. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2007, 33, 444–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  203. Cote-Lussier, C.; Mathieu, M.E.; Barnett, T.A. Independent associations between child and parent perceived neighborhood safety, child screen time, physical activity and bmi: A structural equation modeling approach. Int. J. Obes. 2015, 39, 1475–1481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  204. Lee, S.C.; Koleilat, M.; Hernandez, L.M.; Whaley, S.E.; Davis, J.N. Screen time associated to unhealthy diets in low-income children. J. Food Nutr. Res. 2016, 4, 94–99. [Google Scholar]
  205. Mitchell, R.J.; Richardson, E.A.; Shortt, N.K.; Pearce, J.R. Neighborhood environments and socioeconomic inequalities in mental well-being. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2015, 49, 80–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  206. Shanahan, D.F.; Bush, R.; Gaston, K.J.; Lin, B.B.; Dean, J.; Barber, E.; Fuller, R.A. Health benefits from nature experiences depend on dose. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 28551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  207. Im, S.G.; Choi, H.; Jeon, Y.H.; Song, M.K.; Kim, W.; Woo, J.M. Comparison of effect of two-hour exposure to forest and urban environments on cytokine, anti-oxidant, and stress levels in young adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  208. Seo, S.C.; Park, S.J.; Park, C.W.; Yoon, W.S.; Choung, J.T.; Yoo, Y. Clinical and immunological effects of a forest trip in children with asthma and atopic dermatitis. Iran. J. Allergy Asthma Immunol. 2015, 14, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  209. Li, Q.; Kobayashi, M.; Kumeda, S.; Ochiai, T.; Miura, T.; Kagawa, T.; Imai, M.; Wang, Z.; Otsuka, T.; Kawada, T. Effects of forest bathing on cardiovascular and metabolic parameters in middle-aged males. Evid. Based Complement. Altern. Med. 2016, 2016, 2587381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  210. Grazuleviciene, R.; Vencloviene, J.; Kubilius, R.; Grizas, V.; Danileviciute, A.; Dedele, A.; Andrusaityte, S.; Vitkauskiene, A.; Steponaviciute, R.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Tracking restoration of park and urban street settings in coronary artery disease patients. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  211. Gidlow, C.J.; Randall, J.; Gillman, J.; Smith, G.R.; Jones, M.V. Natural environments and chronic stress measured by hair cortisol. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2016, 148, 61–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  212. Honold, J.; Lakes, T.; Beyer, R.; van der Meer, E. Restoration in urban spaces: Nature views from home, greenways, and public parks. Environ. Behav. 2016, 48, 796–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  213. Ochiai, H.; Ikei, H.; Song, C.; Kobayashi, M.; Miura, T.; Kagawa, T.; Li, Q.; Kumeda, S.; Imai, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological effects of a forest therapy program on middle-aged females. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 15222–15232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  214. Thompson, C.W.; Roe, J.; Aspinall, P.; Mitchell, R.; Clow, A.; Miller, D. More green space is linked to less stress in deprived communities: Evidence from salivary cortisol patterns. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2012, 105, 221–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  215. Roe, J.J.; Thompson, C.W.; Aspinall, P.A.; Brewer, M.J.; Duff, E.I.; Miller, D.; Mitchell, R.; Clow, A. Green space and stress: Evidence from cortisol measures in deprived urban communities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2013, 10, 4086–4103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  216. Grigsby-Toussaint, D.S.; Turi, K.N.; Krupa, M.; Williams, N.J.; Pandi-Perumal, S.R.; Jean-Louis, G. Sleep insufficiency and the natural environment: Results from the US behavioral risk factor surveillance system survey. Prev. Med. 2015, 78, 78–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  217. Astell-Burt, T.; Feng, X.Q.; Kolt, G.S. Does access to neighbourhood green space promote a healthy duration of sleep? Novel findings from a cross-sectional study of 259,319 Australians. BMJ Open 2013, 3, e003094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  218. Gladwell, V.F.; Kuoppa, P.; Tarvainen, M.P.; Rogerson, M. A lunchtime walk in nature enhances restoration of autonomic control during night-time sleep: Results from a preliminary study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  219. Seron-Ferre, M.; Richter, H.G.; Valenzuela, G.J.; Torres-Farfan, C. Circadian rhythms in the fetus and newborn: Significance of interactions with maternal physiology and the environment. Neuromethods 2016, 109, 147–165. [Google Scholar]
  220. Reiter, R.J.; Tan, D.X.; Korkmaz, A.; Rosales-Corral, S.A. Melatonin and stable circadian rhythms optimize maternal, placental and fetal physiology. Hum. Reprod. Update 2014, 20, 293–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  221. Lin, Y.C.; Chen, M.H.; Hsieh, C.J.; Chen, P.C. Effect of rotating shift work on childbearing and birth weight: A study of women working in a semiconductor manufacturing factory. World J. Pediatr. 2011, 7, 129–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  222. Rybnikova, N.A.; Haim, A.; Portnov, B.A. Does artificial light-at-night exposure contribute to the worldwide obesity pandemic? Int. J. Obes. (Lond.) 2016, 40, 815–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  223. Matsuoka, R.H. Student performance and high school landscapes: Examining the links. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2010, 97, 273–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  224. Wu, C.D.; McNeely, E.; Cedeno-Laurent, J.G.; Pan, W.C.; Adamkiewicz, G.; Dominici, F.; Lung, S.C.C.; Su, H.J.; Spengler, J.D. Linking student performance in Massachusetts elementary schools with the “greenness” of school surroundings using remote sensing. PLoS ONE 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  225. Benfield, J.A.; Rainbolt, G.N.; Bell, P.A.; Donovan, G.H. Classrooms with nature views: Evidence of differing student perceptions and behaviors. Environ. Behav. 2015, 47, 140–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  226. Dadvand, P.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J.; Esnaola, M.; Forns, J.; Basagana, X.; Alvarez-Pedrerol, M.; Rivas, I.; Lopez-Vicente, M.; de Castro Pascual, M.; Su, J.; et al. Green spaces and cognitive development in primary schoolchildren. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 7937–7942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  227. Li, D.; Sullivan, W.C. Impact of views to school landscapes on recovery from stress and mental fatigue. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2016, 148, 149–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  228. Weinstein, N.; Przybylski, A.K.; Ryan, R.M. Can nature make us more caring? Effects of immersion in nature on intrinsic aspirations and generosity. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 35, 1315–1329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  229. Zelenski, J.M.; Dopko, R.L.; Capaldi, C.A. Cooperation is in our nature: Nature exposure may promote cooperative and environmentally sustainable behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 42, 24–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  230. Gueguen, N.; Stefan, J. “Green altruism”: Short immersion in natural green environments and helping behavior. Environ. Behav. 2016, 48, 324–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  231. Zelenski, J.M.; Nisbet, E.K. Happiness and feeling connected: The distinct role of nature relatedness. Environ. Behav. 2014, 46, 3–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  232. Capaldi, C.A.; Dopko, R.L.; Zelenski, J.M. The relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: A meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  233. Flowers, E.P.; Freeman, P.; Gladwell, V.F. A cross-sectional study examining predictors of visit frequency to local green space and the impact this has on physical activity levels. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  234. Dubos, R. The Mirage of Health: Utopias, Progress and Biological Change; Rutgers University Press: New Brunswick, NJ, USA, 1959. [Google Scholar]
  235. Prescott, S.L. Disease prevention in the age of convergence—The need for a wider, long ranging and collaborative vision. Allergol. Int. 2014, 63, 11–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  236. Logan, A.C.; Jacka, F.N.; Prescott, S.L. Immune-microbiota interactions: Dysbiosis as a global health issue. Curr. Allergy Asthma Rep. 2016, 16, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  237. Petersen, C.; Round, J.L. Defining dysbiosis and its influence on host immunity and disease. Cell. Microbiol. 2014, 16, 1024–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  238. Szczesniak, O.; K, A.H.; Hanssen, J.F.; Rudi, K. Isovaleric acid in stool correlates with human depression. Nutr. Neurosci. 2015, 19, 279–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  239. Naseribafrouei, A.; Hestad, K.; Avershina, E.; Sekelja, M.; Linlokken, A.; Wilson, R.; Rudi, K. Correlation between the human fecal microbiota and depression. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 2014, 26, 1155–1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  240. Zheng, P.; Zeng, B.; Zhou, C.; Liu, M.; Fang, Z.; Xu, X.; Zeng, L.; Chen, J.; Fan, S.; Du, X.; et al. Gut microbiome remodeling induces depressive-like behaviors through a pathway mediated by the host’s metabolism. Mol. Psychiatry 2016, 21, 786–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  241. Kelly, J.R.; Borre, Y.; C, O.B.; Patterson, E.; El Aidy, S.; Deane, J.; Kennedy, P.J.; Beers, S.; Scott, K.; Moloney, G.; et al. Transferring the blues: Depression-associated gut microbiota induces neurobehavioural changes in the rat. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2016, 82, 109–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  242. Aizawa, E.; Tsuji, H.; Asahara, T.; Takahashi, T.; Teraishi, T.; Yoshida, S.; Ota, M.; Koga, N.; Hattori, K.; Kunugi, H. Possible association of bifidobacterium and lactobacillus in the gut microbiota of patients with major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 202, 254–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  243. Tochitani, S.; Ikeno, T.; Ito, T.; Sakurai, A.; Yamauchi, T.; Matsuzaki, H. Administration of non-absorbable antibiotics to pregnant mice to perturb the maternal gut microbiota is associated with alterations in offspring behavior. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0138293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  244. Lurie, I.; Yang, Y.X.; Haynes, K.; Mamtani, R.; Boursi, B. Antibiotic exposure and the risk for depression, anxiety, or psychosis: A nested case-control study. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2015, 76, 1522–1528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  245. Logan, A.C.; Jacka, F.N.; Craig, J.M.; Prescott, S.L. The microbiome and mental health: Looking back, moving forward with lessons from allergic diseases. Clin. Psychopharmacol. Neurosci. 2016, 14, 131–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  246. Pirbaglou, M.; Katz, J.; de Souza, R.J.; Stearns, J.C.; Motamed, M.; Ritvo, P. Probiotic supplementation can positively affect anxiety and depressive symptoms: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Nutr. Res. 2016, 36, 889–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  247. Huang, R.; Wang, K.; Hu, J. Effect of probiotics on depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Nutrients 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  248. Dinan, T.G.; Stanton, C.; Cryan, J.F. Psychobiotics: A novel class of psychotropic. Biol. Psychiatry 2013, 74, 720–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  249. Logan, A.C. Dysbiotic drift: Mental health, environmental grey space, and microbiota. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2015, 34, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  250. Miller, G.E.; Engen, P.A.; Gillevet, P.M.; Shaikh, M.; Sikaroodi, M.; Forsyth, C.B.; Mutlu, E.; Keshavarzian, A. Lower neighborhood socioeconomic status associated with reduced diversity of the colonic microbiota in healthy adults. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0148952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  251. Strachan, D.P. Hay fever, hygiene, and household size. BMJ 1989, 299, 1259–1260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  252. Wold, A.E. The hygiene hypothesis revised: Is the rising frequency of allergy due to changes in the intestinal flora? Allergy 1998, 53, 20–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  253. Fox, M.; Knapp, L.A.; Andrews, P.W.; Fincher, C.L. Hygiene and the world distribution of alzheimer’s disease: Epidemiological evidence for a relationship between microbial environment and age-adjusted disease burden. Evol. Med. Public Health 2013, 2013, 173–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  254. Selhub, E.M.; Logan, A.C.; Bested, A.C. Fermented foods, microbiota, and mental health: Ancient practice meets nutritional psychiatry. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2014, 33, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  255. Park, S.; Bae, J.H. Fermented food intake is associated with a reduced likelihood of atopic dermatitis in an adult population (Korean national health and nutrition examination survey 2012–2013). Nutr. Res. 2016, 36, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  256. Hesselmar, B.; Hicke-Roberts, A.; Wennergren, G. Allergy in children in hand versus machine dishwashing. Pediatrics 2015, 135, e590–e597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  257. Hilimire, M.R.; DeVylder, J.E.; Forestell, C.A. Fermented foods, neuroticism, and social anxiety: An interaction model. Psychiatry Res. 2015, 228, 203–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  258. Ozawa, N.; Shimojo, N.; Suzuki, Y.; Ochiai, S.; Nakano, T.; Morita, Y.; Inoue, Y.; Arima, T.; Suzuki, S.; Kohno, Y. Maternal intake of natto, a Japan’s traditional fermented soybean food, during pregnancy and the risk of eczema in Japanese babies. Allergol. Int. 2014, 63, 261–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  259. Cepeda, M.S.; Katz, E.G.; Blacketer, C. Microbiome-gut-brain axis: Probiotics and their association with depression. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  260. Covvey, J.R.; Johnson, B.F.; Elliott, V.; Malcolm, W.; Mullen, A.B. An association between socioeconomic deprivation and primary care antibiotic prescribing in Scotland. J. Antimicrob. Chemother. 2014, 69, 835–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  261. Hicks, L.A.; Bartoces, M.G.; Roberts, R.M.; Suda, K.J.; Hunkler, R.J.; Taylor, T.H., Jr.; Schrag, S.J. US outpatient antibiotic prescribing variation according to geography, patient population, and provider specialty in 2011. Clin. Infect. Dis. 2015, 60, 1308–1316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  262. Jensen, J.N.; Bjerrum, L.; Boel, J.; Jarlov, J.O.; Arpi, M. Parents’ socioeconomic factors related to high antibiotic prescribing in primary health care among children aged 0–6 years in the capital region of Denmark. Scand. J. Prim. Health Care 2016, 34, 274–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  263. De Araujo, H.B.; Carmo-Rodrigues, M.S.; Mello, C.S.; Melli, L.C.F.L.; Tahan, S.; Pignatari, A.C.C.; de Morais, M.B. Children living near a sanitary landfill have increased breath methane and methanobrevibacter smithii in their intestinal microbiota. Archaea 2014, 2014, 576249. [Google Scholar]
  264. Seedorf, H.; Griffin, N.W.; Ridaura, V.K.; Reyes, A.; Cheng, J.Y.; Rey, F.E.; Smith, M.I.; Simon, G.M.; Scheffrahn, R.H.; Woebken, D.; et al. Bacteria from diverse habitats colonize and compete in the mouse gut. Cell 2014, 159, 253–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  265. Wu, G.D.; Compher, C.; Chen, E.Z.; Smith, S.A.; Shah, R.D.; Bittinger, K.; Chehoud, C.; Albenberg, L.G.; Nessel, L.; Gilroy, E.; et al. Comparative metabolomics in vegans and omnivores reveal constraints on diet-dependent gut microbiota metabolite production. Gut 2016, 65, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  266. Escobar, J.S.; Klotz, B.; Valdes, B.E.; Agudelo, G.M. The gut microbiota of colombians differs from that of americans, europeans and asians. BMC Microbiol. 2014, 14, 311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  267. Locey, K.J.; Lennon, J.T. Scaling laws predict global microbial diversity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, 5970–5975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  268. Ruokolainen, L.; von Hertzen, L.; Fyhrquist, N.; Laatikainen, T.; Lehtomaki, J.; Auvinen, P.; Karvonen, A.M.; Hyvarinen, A.; Tillmann, V.; Niemela, O.; et al. Green areas around homes reduce atopic sensitization in children. Allergy 2015, 70, 195–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  269. Hanski, I.; von Hertzen, L.; Fyhrquist, N.; Koskinen, K.; Torppa, K.; Laatikainen, T.; Karisola, P.; Auvinen, P.; Paulin, L.; Makela, M.J.; et al. Environmental biodiversity, human microbiota, and allergy are interrelated. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 8334–8339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  270. Mhuireach, G.; Johnson, B.R.; Altrichter, A.E.; Ladau, J.; Meadow, J.F.; Pollard, K.S.; Green, J.L. Urban greenness influences airborne bacterial community composition. Sci. Total Environ. 2016, 571, 680–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  271. Lymperopoulou, D.S.; Adams, R.I.; Lindow, S.E. Contribution of vegetation to the microbial composition of nearby outdoor air. Appl. Environ. Microbiol. 2016, 82, 3822–3833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  272. Lowry, C.A.; Hollis, J.H.; de Vries, A.; Pan, B.; Brunet, L.R.; Hunt, J.R.; Paton, J.F.; van Kampen, E.; Knight, D.M.; Evans, A.K.; et al. Identification of an immune-responsive mesolimbocortical serotonergic system: Potential role in regulation of emotional behavior. Neuroscience 2007, 146, 756–772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  273. Matthews, D.M.; Jenks, S.M. Ingestion of mycobacterium vaccae decreases anxiety-related behavior and improves learning in mice. Behav. Process. 2013, 96, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  274. Reber, S.O.; Siebler, P.H.; Donner, N.C.; Morton, J.T.; Smith, D.G.; Kopelman, J.M.; Lowe, K.R.; Wheeler, K.J.; Fox, J.H.; Hassell, J.E., Jr.; et al. Immunization with a heat-killed preparation of the environmental bacterium mycobacterium vaccae promotes stress resilience in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, E3130–E3139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  275. Knowles, S.R.; Nelson, E.A.; Palombo, E.A. Investigating the role of perceived stress on bacterial flora activity and salivary cortisol secretion: A possible mechanism underlying susceptibility to illness. Biol. Psychol. 2008, 77, 132–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  276. Sundin, J.; Rangel, I.; Fuentes, S.; Heikamp-de Jong, I.; Hultgren-Hornquist, E.; de Vos, W.M.; Brummer, R.J. Altered faecal and mucosal microbial composition in post-infectious irritable bowel syndrome patients correlates with mucosal lymphocyte phenotypes and psychological distress. Aliment. Pharmacol. Ther. 2015, 41, 342–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  277. Zijlmans, M.A.; Korpela, K.; Riksen-Walraven, J.M.; de Vos, W.M.; de Weerth, C. Maternal prenatal stress is associated with the infant intestinal microbiota. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2015, 53, 233–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  278. Lyte, M. The effect of stress on microbial growth. Anim. Health Res. Rev. 2014, 15, 172–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  279. Tannock, G.W. The lactic microflora of pigs, mice and rats. Lactic Acid Bact. 1992, 1, 21–48. [Google Scholar]
  280. Timofeyev, I.; Loseva, E.; Alekseeva, T.; Perminova, N. Stability to sound stress and changeability in intestinal microflora. Eur. Psychiatr. 2002, 17, 200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  281. Suzuki, K.; Harasawa, R.; Yoshitake, Y.; Mitsuoka, T. Effects of crowding and heat-stress on intestinal flora, body-weight gain, and feed-efficiency of growing-rats and chicks. Jpn. J. Vet. Sci. 1983, 45, 331–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  282. Wu, J.F.; Wen, X.W.; Faulk, C.; Boehnke, K.; Zhang, H.P.; Dolinoy, D.C.; Xi, C.W. Perinatal lead exposure alters gut microbiota composition and results in sex-specific bodyweight increases in adult mice. Toxicol. Sci. 2016, 151, 324–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  283. Ribiere, C.; Peyret, P.; Parisot, N.; Darcha, C.; Dechelotte, P.J.; Barnich, N.; Peyretaillade, E.; Boucher, D. Oral exposure to environmental pollutant benzo[a]pyrene impacts the intestinal epithelium and induces gut microbial shifts in murine model. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 31027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  284. Salim, S.Y.; Kaplan, G.G.; Madsen, K.L. Air pollution effects on the gut microbiota: A link between exposure and inflammatory disease. Gut Microbes 2014, 5, 215–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  285. Biedermann, L.; Brulisauer, K.; Zeitz, J.; Frei, P.; Scharl, M.; Vavricka, S.R.; Fried, M.; Loessner, M.J.; Rogler, G.; Schuppler, M. Smoking cessation alters intestinal microbiota: Insights from quantitative investigations on human fecal samples using fish. Inflamm. Bowel Dis. 2014, 20, 1496–1501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  286. Wang, H.; Zhao, J.X.; Hu, N.; Ren, J.; Du, M.; Zhu, M.J. Side-stream smoking reduces intestinal inflammation and increases expression of tight junction proteins. World J. Gastroenterol. 2012, 18, 2180–2187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  287. Allais, L.; Kerckhof, F.M.; Verschuere, S.; Bracke, K.R.; de Smet, R.; Laukens, D.; van den Abbeele, P.; de Vos, M.; Boon, N.; Brusselle, G.G.; et al. Chronic cigarette smoke exposure induces microbial and inflammatory shifts and mucin changes in the murine gut. Environ. Microbiol. 2016, 18, 1352–1363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  288. Dubinkina, V.B.; Tyakht, A.V.; Ilina, E.N.; Ischenko, D.S.; Kovarsky, B.A.; Yarygin, K.S.; Pavlenko, A.V.; Popenko, A.S.; Alexeev, D.G.; Taraskina, A.E.; et al. Metagenomic analysis of taxonomic and functional changes in gut microbiota of patients with alcoholic dependence syndrome. Biomed. Khimiia 2015, 61, 742–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  289. Davis, J.E.; Freel, N.; Findley, A.; Tomlin, K.; Howard, K.M.; Seran, C.C.; Cruz, P.; Kingsley, K. A molecular survey of S. mutans and P. gingivalis oral microbial burden in human saliva using Relative Endpoint Polymerase Chain Reaction (RE-PCR) within the population of a Nevada dental school revealed disparities among minorities. BMC Oral Health 2012, 12, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  290. Arimatsu, K.; Yamada, H.; Miyazawa, H.; Minagawa, T.; Nakajima, M.; Ryder, M.I.; Gotoh, K.; Motooka, D.; Nakamura, S.; Iida, T.; et al. Oral pathobiont induces systemic inflammation and metabolic changes associated with alteration of gut microbiota. Sci. Rep. 2014, 4, 4828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  291. Mason, M.; Zaganjor, H.; Bozlak, C.T.; Lammel-Harmon, C.; Gomez-Feliciano, L.; Becker, A.B. Working with community partners to implement and evaluate the chicago park district’s 100% healthier snack vending initiative. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2014, 11, E135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  292. Blanck, H.M.; Allen, D.; Bashir, Z.; Gordon, N.; Goodman, A.; Merriam, D.; Rutt, C. Let’s go to the park today: The role of parks in obesity prevention and improving the public’s health. Child. Obes. 2012, 8, 423–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  293. Mena, C.; Fuentes, E.; Ormazabal, Y.; Palomo-Velez, G.; Palomo, I. Role of access to parks and markets with anthropometric measurements, biological markers, and a healthy lifestyle. Int. J. Environ. Health Res. 2015, 25, 373–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  294. Hsieh, S.; Klassen, A.C.; Curriero, F.C.; Caulfield, L.E.; Cheskin, L.J.; Davis, J.N.; Goran, M.I.; Weigensberg, M.J.; Spruijt-Metz, D. Fast-food restaurants, park access, and insulin resistance among Hispanic youth. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2014, 46, 378–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  295. Carroll-Scott, A.; Gilstad-Hayden, K.; Rosenthal, L.; Peters, S.M.; McCaslin, C.; Joyce, R.; Ickovics, J.R. Disentangling neighborhood contextual associations with child body mass index, diet, and physical activity: The role of built, socioeconomic, and social environments. Soc. Sci. Med. 2013, 95, 106–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  296. Meyer, K.A.; Boone-Heinonen, J.; Duffey, K.J.; Rodriguez, D.A.; Kiefe, C.I.; Lewis, C.E.; Gordon-Larsen, P. Combined measure of neighborhood food and physical activity environments and weight-related outcomes: The cardia study. Health Place 2015, 33, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  297. Chum, A.; O’Campo, P. Cross-sectional associations between residential environmental exposures and cardiovascular diseases. BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  298. Coughenour, C.; Burns, M.S. Community design impacts on health habits in low-income southern Nevadans. Am. J. Health Behav. 2016, 40, 534–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  299. Dubos, R. Science and man’s nature. J. Am. Acad. Art Sci. 1965, 94, 223–244. [Google Scholar]
  300. Basner, M.; Spaeth, A.M.; Dinges, D.F. Sociodemographic characteristics and waking activities and their role in the timing and duration of sleep. Sleep 2014, 37, 1889–1906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  301. Shiue, I. Correlations of indoor second-hand smoking, household smoking rules, regional deprivation and children mental health: Scottish health survey, 2013. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. Int. 2015, 22, 9858–9863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  302. Laxer, R.E.; Janssen, I. The proportion of excessive fast-food consumption attributable to the neighbourhood food environment among youth living within 1 km of their school. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 2014, 39, 480–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  303. Coffee, N.T.; Kennedy, H.P.; Niyonsenga, T. Fast-food exposure around schools in urban Adelaide. Public Health Nutr. 2016, 14, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  304. Kestens, Y.; Daniel, M. Social inequalities in food exposure around schools in an urban area. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2010, 39, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  305. Zenk, S.N.; Powell, L.M. US secondary schools and food outlets. Health Place 2008, 14, 336–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  306. Simon, P.A.; Kwan, D.; Angelescu, A.; Shih, M.; Fielding, J.E. Proximity of fast food restaurants to schools: Do neighborhood income and type of school matter? Prev. Med. 2008, 47, 284–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  307. Vandevijvere, S.; Sushil, Z.; Exeter, D.J.; Swinburn, B. Obesogenic retail food environments around New Zealand schools: A national study. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2016, 51, e57–e66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  308. D’Angelo, H.; Ammerman, A.; Gordon-Larsen, P.; Linnan, L.; Lytle, L.; Ribisl, K.M. Sociodemographic disparities in proximity of schools to tobacco outlets and fast-food restaurants. Am. J. Public Health 2016, 106, 1556–1562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  309. Arredondo, E.; Castaneda, D.; Elder, J.P.; Slymen, D.; Dozier, D. Brand name logo recognition of fast food and healthy food among children. J. Community Health 2009, 34, 73–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  310. Michimi, A.; Wimberly, M.C. The food environment and adult obesity in US metropolitan areas. Geospat. Health 2015, 10, 368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  311. Lee, R.E.; Heinrich, K.M.; Reese-Smith, J.Y.; Regan, G.R.; Adamus-Leach, H.J. Obesogenic and youth oriented restaurant marketing in public housing neighborhoods. Am. J. Health Behav. 2014, 38, 218–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  312. Hill, J.L.; Olive, N.C.; Waters, C.N.; Estabrooks, P.A.; You, W.; Zoellner, J.M. Lack of healthy food options on children’s menus of restaurants in the health-disparate dan river region of Virginia and North Carolina, 2013. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2015, 12, E40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  313. Zenk, S.N.; Powell, L.M.; Rimkus, L.; Isgor, Z.; Barker, D.C.; Ohri-Vachaspati, P.; Chaloupka, F. Relative and absolute availability of healthier food and beverage alternatives across communities in the United States. Am. J. Public Health 2014, 104, 2170–2178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  314. Isgor, Z.; Powell, L.; Rimkus, L.; Chaloupka, F. Associations between retail food store exterior advertisements and community demographic and socioeconomic composition. Health Place 2016, 39, 43–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  315. Kwate, N.O.; Lee, T.H. Ghettoizing outdoor advertising: Disadvantage and ad panel density in black neighborhoods. J. Urban Health Bull. N. Y. Acad. Med. 2007, 84, 21–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  316. Burgoine, T.; Forouhi, N.G.; Griffin, S.J.; Brage, S.; Wareham, N.J.; Monsivais, P. Does neighborhood fast-food outlet exposure amplify inequalities in diet and obesity? A cross-sectional study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2016, 103, 1540–1547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  317. Henriksen, L.; Schleicher, N.C.; Barker, D.C.; Liu, Y.; Chaloupka, F.J. Prices for Tobacco and Nontobacco Products in Pharmacies Versus Other Stores: Results from Retail Marketing Surveillance in California and in the United States. Am. J. Public Health 2016, 106, 1858–1864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  318. Cameron, A.J.; Thornton, L.E.; McNaughton, S.A.; Crawford, D. Variation in supermarket exposure to energy-dense snack foods by socio-economic position. Public Health Nutr. 2013, 16, 1178–1185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  319. Farley, T.A.; Rice, J.; Bodor, J.N.; Cohen, D.A.; Bluthenthal, R.N.; Rose, D. Measuring the food environment: Shelf space of fruits, vegetables, and snack foods in stores. J. Urban Health Bull. N. Y. Acad. Med. 2009, 86, 672–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  320. Rose, D.; Hutchinson, P.L.; Bodor, J.N.; Swalm, C.M.; Farley, T.A.; Cohen, D.A.; Rice, J.C. Neighborhood food environments and body mass index: The importance of in-store contents. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2009, 37, 214–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  321. Franco, M.; Diez Roux, A.V.; Glass, T.A.; Caballero, B.; Brancati, F.L. Neighborhood characteristics and availability of healthy foods in baltimore. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2008, 35, 561–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  322. Andreyeva, T.; Blumenthal, D.M.; Schwartz, M.B.; Long, M.W.; Brownell, K.D. Availability and prices of foods across stores and neighborhoods: The case of new haven, connecticut. Health Aff. 2008, 27, 1381–1388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  323. Adjoian, T.; Dannefer, R.; Sacks, R.; van Wye, G. Comparing sugary drinks in the food retail environment in six NYC neighborhoods. J. Community Health 2014, 39, 327–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  324. Rimkus, L.; Isgor, Z.; Ohri-Vachaspati, P.; Zenk, S.N.; Powell, L.M.; Barker, D.C.; Chaloupka, F.J. Disparities in the availability and price of low-fat and higher-fat milk in US food stores by community characteristics. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2015, 115, 1975–1985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  325. Grigsby-Toussaint, D.S.; Moise, I.K.; Geiger, S.D. Observations of marketing on food packaging targeted to youth in retail food stores. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2011, 19, 1898–1900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  326. Bragg, M.A.; Miller, A.N.; Elizee, J.; Dighe, S.; Elbel, B.D. Popular music celebrity endorsements in food and nonalcoholic beverage marketing. Pediatrics 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  327. Howlett, E.; Davis, C.; Burton, S. From food desert to food oasis: The potential influence of food retailers on childhood obesity rates. J. Bus. Ethics 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  328. Kahr, M.K.; Suter, M.A.; Ballas, J.; Ramin, S.M.; Monga, M.; Lee, W.; Hu, M.; Shope, C.D.; Chesnokova, A.; Krannich, L.; et al. Geospatial analysis of food environment demonstrates associations with gestational diabetes. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  329. Russell, S.J.; Hughes, K.; Bellis, M.A. Impact of childhood experience and adult well-being on eating preferences and behaviours. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e007770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  330. Longacre, M.R.; Drake, K.M.; Titus, L.J.; Cleveland, L.P.; Langeloh, G.; Hendricks, K.; Dalton, M.A. A toy story: Association between young children’s knowledge of fast food toy premiums and their fast food consumption. Appetite 2016, 96, 473–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  331. McGale, L.S.; Halford, J.C.; Harrold, J.A.; Boyland, E.J. The influence of brand equity characters on children’s food preferences and choices. J. Pediatr. 2016, 177, 33–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  332. Scourboutakos, M.J.; Semnani-Azad, Z.; L’Abbe, M.R. Added sugars in kids’ meals from chain restaurants. Prev. Med. Rep. 2016, 3, 391–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  333. Norman, J.; Kelly, B.; Boyland, E.; McMahon, A.T. The impact of marketing and advertising on food behaviours: Evaluating the evidence for a causal relationship. Curr. Nutr. Rep. 2016, 5, 139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  334. Moodie, R.; Stuckler, D.; Monteiro, C.; Sheron, N.; Neal, B.; Thamarangsi, T.; Lincoln, P.; Casswell, S.; Lancet, N.C.D.A.G. Profits and pandemics: Prevention of harmful effects of tobacco, alcohol, and ultra-processed food and drink industries. Lancet 2013, 381, 670–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  335. Mialon, M.; Swinburn, B.; Wate, J.; Tukana, I.; Sacks, G. Analysis of the corporate political activity of major food industry actors in Fiji. Glob. Health 2016, 12, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  336. Mialon, M.; Swinburn, B.; Allender, S.; Sacks, G. Systematic examination of publicly-available information reveals the diverse and extensive corporate political activity of the food industry in Australia. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  337. Ortiz, S.E.; Zimmerman, F.J.; Adler, G.J., Jr. Increasing public support for food-industry related, obesity prevention policies: The role of a taste-engineering frame and contextualized values. Soc. Sci. Med. 2016, 156, 142–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  338. Goryakin, Y.; Rocco, L.; Suhrcke, M.; Roberts, B.; McKee, M. Fruit and vegetable consumption in the Former Soviet Union: The role of individual- and community-level factors. Public Health Nutr. 2015, 18, 2825–2835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  339. Zimmerman, F.J.; Shimoga, S.V. The effects of food advertising and cognitive load on food choices. BMC Public Health 2014, 14, 342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  340. Childs, E.; de Wit, H. Effects of acute psychosocial stress on cigarette craving and smoking. Nicotine Tob. Res. 2010, 12, 449–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  341. Groth, S.W.; Simpson, A.H.; Fernandez, I.D. The dietary choices of women who are low-income, pregnant, and African American. J. Midwifery Womens Health 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  342. Gugusheff, J.R.; Ong, Z.Y.; Muhlhausler, B.S. The early origins of food preferences: Targeting the critical windows of development. FASEB J. 2015, 29, 365–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  343. Mercer, S.W.; Higgins, M.; Bikker, A.M.; Fitzpatrick, B.; McConnachie, A.; Lloyd, S.M.; Little, P.; Watt, G.C.M. General practitioners’ empathy and health outcomes: A prospective observational study of consultations in areas of high and low deprivation. Ann. Fam. Med. 2016, 14, 117–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  344. Jani, B.; Bikker, A.P.; Higgins, M.; Fitzpatrick, B.; Little, P.; Watt, G.C.M.; Mercer, S.W. Patient centredness and the outcome of primary care consultations with patients with depression in areas of high and low socioeconomic deprivation. Br. J. Gen. Pract. 2012, 62, e576–e581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  345. Mercer, S.W.; Jani, B.D.; Maxwell, M.; Wong, S.Y.S.; Watt, G.C.M. Patient enablement requires physician empathy: A cross-sectional study of general practice consultations in areas of high and low socioeconomic deprivation in scotland. BMC Fam. Pract. 2012, 13, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  346. Ruberton, P.M.; Huynh, H.P.; Miller, T.A.; Kruse, E.; Chancellor, J.; Lyubomirsky, S. The relationship between physician humility, physician-patient communication, and patient health. Patient Educ. Couns. 2016, 99, 1138–1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  347. Fleming-Dutra, K.E.; Hersh, A.L.; Shapiro, D.J.; Bartoces, M.; Enns, E.A.; File, T.M.; Finkelstein, J.A.; Gerber, J.S.; Hyun, D.Y.; Linder, J.A.; et al. Prevalence of Inappropriate Antibiotic Prescriptions among US Ambulatory Care Visits, 2010–2011. JAMA J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2016, 315, 1864–1873. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  348. Mercer, S.W.; Watt, G.C.M. The inverse care law: Clinical primary care encounters in deprived and affluent areas of Scotland. Ann. Fam. Med. 2007, 5, 503–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  349. Van der Steen, W.J.; Thung, P.J. Faces of Medicine: A Philosophical Study; Kluwer Academic Press: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1988; p. 50. [Google Scholar]
  350. Levy, B.S.; Patz, J.A. Climate change, human rights, and social justice. Ann. Glob. Health 2015, 81, 310–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  351. Royal Botanic Gardens, Kew. The State of the World’s Plants Report 2016; Royal Botanic Gardens Publication: Kew, UK, 2016.
  352. Von Hertzen, L.; Beutler, B.; Bienenstock, J.; Blaser, M.; Cani, P.D.; Eriksson, J.; Farkkila, M.; Haahtela, T.; Hanski, I.; Jenmalm, M.C.; et al. Helsinki alert of biodiversity and health. Ann. Med. 2015, 47, 218–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  353. Soga, M.; Gaston, K.J. Extinction of experience: The loss of human-nature interactions. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2016, 14, 94–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  354. Lancet, O. Climate change and non-communicable diseases. Lancet Oncol. 2016, 17, 1. [Google Scholar]
  355. Speldewinde, P.C.; Cook, A.; Davies, P.; Weinstein, P. A relationship between environmental degradation and mental health in Rural Western Australia. Health Place 2009, 15, 880–887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  356. Donovan, G.H.; Butry, D.T.; Michael, Y.L.; Prestemon, J.P.; Liebhold, A.M.; Gatziolis, D.; Mao, M.Y. The Relationship between Trees and Human Health Evidence from the Spread of the Emerald Ash Borer. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2013, 44, 139–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  357. Donovan, G.H.; Michael, Y.L.; Gatziolis, D.; Prestemon, J.P.; Whitsel, E.A. Is tree loss associated with cardiovascular-disease risk in the women’s health initiative? A natural experiment. Health Place 2015, 36, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  358. Willox, A.C.; Harper, S.L.; Ford, J.D.; Edge, V.L.; Landman, K.; Houle, K.; Blake, S.; Wolfrey, C. Climate change and mental health: An exploratory case study from Rigolet, Nunatsiavut, Canada. Clim. Chang. 2013, 121, 255–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  359. Hendrryx, M.; Innes-Wimsatt, K.A. Increased risk of depression for people living in coal mining areas of Central Appalachia. Ecopsychology 2013, 5, 179–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  360. Jones, B.A. Work more and play less? Time use impacts of changing ecosystem services: The case of the invasive emerald ash borer. Ecol. Econ. 2016, 124, 49–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  361. Gardiner, M.M.; Burkman, C.E.; Prajzner, S.P. The value of urban vacant land to support arthropod biodiversity and ecosystem services. Environ. Entomol. 2013, 42, 1123–1136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  362. Garvin, E.C.; Cannuscio, C.C.; Branas, C.C. Greening vacant lots to reduce violent crime: A randomised controlled trial. Inj. Prev. 2013, 19, 198–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  363. South, E.C.; Kondo, M.C.; Cheney, R.A.; Branas, C.C. Neighborhood blight, stress, and health: A walking trial of urban greening and ambulatory heart rate. Am. J. Public Health 2015, 105, 909–913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  364. Branas, C.C.; Kondo, M.C.; Murphy, S.M.; South, E.C.; Polsky, D.; MacDonald, J.M. Urban blight remediation as a cost-beneficial solution to firearm violence. Am. J. Public Health 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  365. Cardinale, B.J.; Duffy, J.E.; Gonzalez, A.; Hooper, D.U.; Perrings, C.; Venail, P.; Narwani, A.; Mace, G.M.; Tilman, D.; Wardle, D.A.; et al. Biodiversity loss and its impact on humanity. Nature 2012, 486, 59–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  366. Bernstein, A.S. Biodiversity and public health. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2014, 35, 153–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  367. Soga, M.; Gaston, K.J.; Yamaura, Y.; Kurisu, K.; Hanaki, K. Both direct and vicarious experiences of nature affect children’s willingness to conserve biodiversity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  368. Lewis, M.; Townsend, M. 'Ecological embeddedness’ and its public health implications: Findings from an exploratory study. Ecohealth 2015, 12, 244–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  369. Lohr, V.I.; Pearson-Mims, C.H. The relative influence of childhood activities and demographics on adult appreciation for the role of trees in human well-being. Acta Hortic. 2004, 253–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  370. Lohr, V.I.; Pearson-Mims, C.H. Children’s active and passive interactions with plants influence their attitudes and actions toward trees and gardening as adults. Horttechnology 2005, 15, 472–476. [Google Scholar]
  371. Ratcliffe, M.M.; Merrigan, K.A.; Rogers, B.L.; Goldberg, J.P. The effects of school garden experiences on middle school-aged students’ knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors associated with vegetable consumption. Health Promot. Pract. 2011, 12, 36–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  372. Davis, J.N.; Martinez, L.C.; Spruijt-Metz, D.; Gatto, N.M. La sprouts: A 12-week gardening, nutrition, and cooking randomized control trial improves determinants of dietary behaviors. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2016, 48, 2–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  373. Utter, J.; Denny, S.; Dyson, B. School gardens and adolescent nutrition and BMI: Results from a national, multilevel study. Prev. Med. 2016, 83, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  374. Alaimo, K.; Beavers, A.W.; Crawford, C.; Snyder, E.H.; Litt, J.S. Amplifying health through community gardens: A framework for advancing multicomponent, behaviorally based neighborhood interventions. Curr. Environ. Health Rep. 2016, 3, 302–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  375. Shay-Margalit, B.; Rubin, O.D. Effect of the israeli “green schools” reform on pupils’ environmental attitudes and behavior. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2016, 30, 112–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  376. Rand, D.G.; Kraft-Todd, G.; Gruber, J. The collective benefits of feeling good and letting go: Positive emotion and (dis)inhibition interact to predict cooperative behavior. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0117426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  377. Prade, C.; Saroglou, V. Awe’s effects on generosity and helping. J. Posit. Psychol. 2016, 11, 522–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  378. Wang, M.; Saudino, K.J. Positive affect: Phenotypic and etiologic associations with prosocial behaviors and internalizing problems in toddlers. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  379. Tapia-Fonllem, C.; Corral-Verdugo, V.; Fraijo-Sing, B. Sustainable behavior and quality of life. In Handbook of Environmental Psychology and Quality of Life Research; Fleury-Bahi, G., Pol, E., Navarro, O., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  380. Aknin, L.B.; Broesch, T.; Hamlin, J.K.; van de Vondervoort, J.W. Prosocial behavior leads to happiness in a small-scale rural society. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2015, 144, 788–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  381. Tasimi, A.; Young, L. Memories of good deeds past: The reinforcing power of prosocial behavior in children. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2016, 147, 159–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  382. Nelson, S.K.; Layous, K.; Cole, S.W.; Lyubomirsky, S. Do unto others or treat yourself? The effects of prosocial and self-focused behavior on psychological flourishing. Emotion 2016, 16, 850–861. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  383. Flouri, E.; Sarmadi, Z. Prosocial behavior and childhood trajectories of internalizing and externalizing problems: The role of neighborhood and school contexts. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 253–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  384. Whitaker, K.M.; Wilcox, S.; Liu, J.; Blair, S.N.; Pate, R.R. Provider advice and women’s intentions to meet weight gain, physical activity, and nutrition guidelines during pregnancy. Matern. Child Health J. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  385. Lee, A.; Belski, R.; Radcliffe, J.; Newton, M. What do pregnant women know about the healthy eating guidelines for pregnancy? A web-based questionnaire. Matern. Child Health J. 2016, 20, 2179–2188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  386. Dubos, R. The genius of design. In Proceedings of the Rest of Our Lives, International Design Conference, Aspen, CO, USA, 15–20 June 1969; Chermayeff, I., Wolf, H., Eds.; 1969; pp. 57–63. [Google Scholar]
  387. Holtz, K.A.; Kokotilo, K.J.; Fitzgerald, B.E.; Frank, E. Exercise behaviour and attitudes among fourth-year medical students at the University of British Columbia. Can. Fam. Phys. 2013, 59, e26–e32. [Google Scholar]
  388. Cardinal, B.J.; Park, E.A.; Kim, M.; Cardinal, M.K. If exercise is medicine, where is exercise in medicine? Review of U.S. Medical education curricula for physical activity-related content. J. Phys. Act. Health 2015, 12, 1336–1343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  389. Adams, K.M.; Kohlmeier, M.; Zeisel, S.H. Nutrition education in U.S. Medical schools: Latest update of a national survey. Acad. Med. 2010, 85, 1537–1542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  390. Castillo, M.; Feinstein, R.; Tsang, J.; Fisher, M. Basic nutrition knowledge of recent medical graduates entering a pediatric residency program. Int. J. Adolesc. Med. Health 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  391. Attorp, A.; Scott, J.E.; Yew, A.C.; Rhodes, R.E.; Barr, S.I.; Naylor, P.J. Associations between socioeconomic, parental and home environment factors and fruit and vegetable consumption of children in grades five and six in British Columbia, Canada. BMC Public Health 2014, 14, 150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  392. Finger, J.D.; Varnaccia, G.; Tylleskar, T.; Lampert, T.; Mensink, G.B. Dietary behaviour and parental socioeconomic position among adolescents: The german health interview and examination survey for children and adolescents 2003–2006 (KIGGS). BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  393. Fismen, A.S.; Smith, O.R.; Torsheim, T.; Samdal, O. A school based study of time trends in food habits and their relation to socio-economic status among norwegian adolescents, 2001–2009. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2014, 11, 115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  394. Glen, K.E.; Thomas, H.M.; Loebach, J.E.; Gilliland, J.A.; Gobert, C.P. Fruit and vegetable consumption among children in a socioeconomically disadvantaged neighbourhood. Can. J. Diet. Pract. Res. 2013, 74, 114–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  395. Chen, D.C.; Kirshenbaum, D.S.; Yan, J.; Kirshenbaum, E.; Aseltine, R.H. Characterizing changes in student empathy throughout medical school. Med. Teach. 2012, 34, 305–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  396. Neumann, M.; Edelhauser, F.; Tauschel, D.; Fischer, M.R.; Wirtz, M.; Woopen, C.; Haramati, A.; Scheffer, C. Empathy decline and its reasons: A systematic review of studies with medical students and residents. Acad. Med. 2011, 86, 996–1009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  397. Gomez, A.; Balsari, S.; Nusbaum, J.; Heerboth, A.; Lemery, J. Perspective: Environment, biodiversity, and the education of the physician of the future. Acad. Med. 2013, 88, 168–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  398. Sarfaty, M.; Kreslake, J.M.; Casale, T.B.; Maibach, E.W. Views of AAAAI members on climate change and health. J. Allergy Clin. Immunol. Pract. 2016, 4, 333–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  399. Sarfaty, M.; Bloodhart, B.; Ewart, G.; Thurston, G.D.; Balmes, J.R.; Guidotti, T.L.; Maibach, E.W. American thoracic society member survey on climate change and health. Ann. Am. Thorac. Soc. 2015, 12, 274–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  400. Sarfaty, M.; Kreslake, J.M.; Bloodhart, B.; Price, K.; Montoro, M.; Casale, T.B.; Folstein, S.; Maibach, E.W. George Mason University Centre for Climate Change Communication: Views of Allergy Specialists on Health Effects of Climate Change. Available online: https://www.aaaai.org/Aaaai/media/MediaLibrary/PDFDocuments/Libraries/Climate-Change-Survey.pdf (accessed on 29 August 2016).
  401. Huber, M.; Knutti, R. Anthropogenic and natural warming inferred from changes in earth’s energy balance. Nat. Geosci. 2012, 5, 31–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  402. Aaron, D.G.; Siegel, M.B. Sponsorship of national health organizations by two major soda companies. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  403. Dubos, R. Man and his environment: Scope, impact and nature. In Environmental Improvement; Marquis, R.W., Ed.; Graduate School Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1966; pp. 3–21. [Google Scholar]
  404. Dubos, R. The spaceship earth. J. Allergy 1969, 44, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  405. Rutt, R.L.; Gulsrud, N.M. Green justice in the city: A new agenda for urban green space research in Europe. Urban For. Urban Green. 2016, 19, 123–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  406. Wolch, J.R.; Byrne, J.; Newell, J.P. Urban green space, public health, and environmental justice: The challenge of making cities ‘just green enough’. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2014, 125, 234–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  407. Gatersleben, B.; Andrews, M. When walking in nature is not restorative-the role of prospect and refuge. Health Place 2013, 20, 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  408. Kabisch, N.; Qureshi, S.; Haase, D. Human-environment interactions in urban green spaces—A systematic review of contemporary issues and prospects for future research. Environ. Impact Assess. Rev. 2015, 50, 25–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  409. Kabisch, N.; Haase, D. Green justice or just green? Provision of urban green spaces in Berlin, Germany. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2014, 122, 129–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  410. Appleton, A.A.; Buka, S.L.; Loucks, E.B.; Rimm, E.B.; Martin, L.T.; Kubzansky, L.D. A prospective study of positive early-life psychosocial factors and favorable cardiovascular risk in adulthood. Circulation 2013, 127, 905–912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  411. Demakakos, P.; Pillas, D.; Marmot, M.; Steptoe, A. Parenting style in childhood and mortality risk at older ages: A longitudinal cohort study. Br. J. Psychiatry J. Ment. Sci. 2016, 209, 135–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  412. Pulkki-Raback, L.; Elovainio, M.; Hakulinen, C.; Lipsanen, J.; Hintsanen, M.; Jokela, M.; Kubzansky, L.D.; Hintsa, T.; Serlachius, A.; Laitinen, T.T.; et al. Cumulative effect of psychosocial factors in youth on ideal cardiovascular health in adulthood: The cardiovascular risk in young finns study. Circulation 2015, 131, 245–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  413. Haworth, C.M.; Nelson, S.K.; Layous, K.; Carter, K.; Jacobs Bao, K.; Lyubomirsky, S.; Plomin, R. Stability and change in genetic and environmental influences on well-being in response to an intervention. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0155538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  414. Nikrahan, G.R.; Laferton, J.A.; Asgari, K.; Kalantari, M.; Abedi, M.R.; Etesampour, A.; Rezaei, A.; Suarez, L.; Huffman, J.C. Effects of positive psychology interventions on risk biomarkers in coronary patients: A randomized, wait-list controlled pilot trial. Psychosomatics 2016, 57, 359–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  415. Layous, K.; Chancellor, J.; Lyubomirsky, S. Positive activities as protective factors against mental health conditions. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2014, 123, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  416. Stahl, J.E.; Dossett, M.L.; LaJoie, A.S.; Denninger, J.W.; Mehta, D.H.; Goldman, R.; Fricchione, G.L.; Benson, H. Relaxation response and resiliency training and its effect on healthcare resource utilization. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0140212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  417. Hatcher, S.L.; Nadeau, M.S.; Walsh, L.K.; Reynolds, M.; Galea, J.; Marz, K. The teaching of empathy for high-school and college-students—Testing rogerian methods with the interpersonal reactivity index. Adolescence 1994, 29, 961–974. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  418. Barley, E.; Lawson, V. Using health psychology to help patients: Promoting wellbeing. Br. J. Nurs. 2016, 25, 852–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  419. Capaldi, C.A.; Passmore, H.; Nisbet, E.K.; Zelenski, J.M.; Dopko, R.L. Flourishing in nature: A review of connecting with nature and its application as a well-being intervention. Int. J. Wellbeing 2015, 5, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  420. Richardson, M.; Cormack, A.; McRobert, L.; Underhill, R. 30 days wild: Development and evaluation of a large-scale nature engagement campaign to improve well-being. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0149777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  421. Lowry, C.A.; Smith, D.G.; Siebler, P.H.; Schmidt, D.; Stamper, C.E.; Hassell, J.E., Jr.; Yamashita, P.S.; Fox, J.H.; Reber, S.O.; Brenner, L.A.; et al. The microbiota, immunoregulation, and mental health: Implications for public health. Curr. Environ. Health Rep. 2016, 3, 270–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  422. Hoisington, A.J.; Brenner, L.A.; Kinney, K.A.; Postolache, T.T.; Lowry, C.A. The microbiome of the built environment and mental health. Microbiome 2015, 3, 60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  423. Dominguez-Bello, M.G.; de Jesus-Laboy, K.M.; Shen, N.; Cox, L.M.; Amir, A.; Gonzalez, A.; Bokulich, N.A.; Song, S.J.; Hoashi, M.; Rivera-Vinas, J.I.; et al. Partial restoration of the microbiota of cesarean-born infants via vaginal microbial transfer. Nat. Med. 2016, 22, 250–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  424. Miller, J.C.; Krizan, Z. Walking facilitates positive affect even when expecting the opposite. Emotion 2016, in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  425. Paskett, E.; Thompson, B.; Ammerman, A.S.; Ortega, A.N.; Marsteller, J.; Richardson, D. Multilevel interventions to address health disparities show promise in improving population health. Health Aff. 2016, 35, 1429–1434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  426. Mah, C.L.; Cook, B.; Rideout, K.; Minaker, L.M. Policy options for healthier retail food environments in city-regions. Can. J. Public Health 2016, 107, 5343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  427. Bryan, C.J.; Yeager, D.S.; Hinojosa, C.P.; Chabot, A.; Bergen, H.; Kawamura, M.; Steubing, F. Harnessing adolescent values to motivate healthier eating. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, 10830–10835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  428. Thompson, B.; Molina, Y.; Viswanath, K.; Warnecke, R.; Prelip, M.L. Strategies to empower communities to reduce health disparities. Health Aff. 2016, 35, 1424–1428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  429. Batis, C.; Rivera, J.A.; Popkin, B.M.; Taillie, L.S. First-year evaluation of Mexico’s tax on nonessential energy-dense foods: An observational study. PLoS Med. 2016, 13, e1002057. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  430. Cradock, A.L.; Kenney, E.L.; McHugh, A.; Conley, L.; Mozaffarian, R.S.; Reiner, J.F.; Gortmaker, S.L. Evaluating the impact of the healthy beverage executive order for city agencies in Boston, Massachusetts, 2011–2013. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2015, 12, 140549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  431. Narain, K.; Mata, A.; Flores, J. Nutrition policy decreases sugar-sweetened beverages in municipal parks: Lessons learned from Carson, California. J. Public Health Manag. Pract. 2016, 22, 392–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  432. Sobko, T.; Tse, M.; Kaplan, M. A randomized controlled trial for families with preschool children—Promoting healthy eating and active playtime by connecting to nature. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  433. Shelton, D.L. Problems in Environmental Protection and Human Rights: A Human Right to the Environment; Social Science Research Network: Rochester, NY, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  434. Obama, B. Presidential Proclamation—20th Anniversary of Executive Order 12898 on Environmental Justice; The White House Office of the Press Secretary: Washington, DC, USA, 2014.
  435. Geltman, E.G.; Gill, G.; Jovanovic, M. Beyond baby steps: An empirical study of the impact of environmental justice executive order 12898. Fam. Community Health 2016, 39, 143–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  436. Dubos, R. Celebrations of Life; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Dysbiosis (defined as “life in distress“) is the result of the modern environmental changes that are adversely affecting all ecosystems, progressively displacing “green space“ with “grey space“ and creating conditions that erode physical, mental and societal health from the first moments of life. This dual burden is compounded and exacerbated by the greyspace environment that perpetuates unhealthy behaviour, with lifelong effects. Commercial forces and absence of policy drive inequity and dysbiosis by default.
Figure 1. Dysbiosis (defined as “life in distress“) is the result of the modern environmental changes that are adversely affecting all ecosystems, progressively displacing “green space“ with “grey space“ and creating conditions that erode physical, mental and societal health from the first moments of life. This dual burden is compounded and exacerbated by the greyspace environment that perpetuates unhealthy behaviour, with lifelong effects. Commercial forces and absence of policy drive inequity and dysbiosis by default.
Ijerph 13 01075 g001
Figure 2. The Tale of Two Cities: Addressing the “upstream“ drivers of health and human potential. While individuals can influence their environment, the environmental effects on individuals (and their opportunities and choices) are much greater. These operate widely to influence both the health of environments and the health of individuals across life.
Figure 2. The Tale of Two Cities: Addressing the “upstream“ drivers of health and human potential. While individuals can influence their environment, the environmental effects on individuals (and their opportunities and choices) are much greater. These operate widely to influence both the health of environments and the health of individuals across life.
Ijerph 13 01075 g002
Figure 3. The health of humanity depends on the health of our environment. The physical and mental health consequences of environmental degradation should not be underestimated.
Figure 3. The health of humanity depends on the health of our environment. The physical and mental health consequences of environmental degradation should not be underestimated.
Ijerph 13 01075 g003
Figure 4. Transnational corporations profit from unhealthy commodities which directly contribute to health disparities in disadvantaged communities. These industries should have no role in the formation of health policy. There is no evidence that public-private partnerships are effective. Public regulation and market intervention are the only evidence-based measures (see [334]).
Figure 4. Transnational corporations profit from unhealthy commodities which directly contribute to health disparities in disadvantaged communities. These industries should have no role in the formation of health policy. There is no evidence that public-private partnerships are effective. Public regulation and market intervention are the only evidence-based measures (see [334]).
Ijerph 13 01075 g004
Figure 5. Community Gardens: Participation in personal, community and academic gardening programs has multiple social, personal and economic benefits. It increases nature-connectedness including the health benefits of microbial biodiversity, social and community cohesion, and positive emotions. It promotes a positive mental outlook and healthier behavior in general, including increased physical activity, increased intake of fruits and vegetables, and reduced fast-food consumption.
Figure 5. Community Gardens: Participation in personal, community and academic gardening programs has multiple social, personal and economic benefits. It increases nature-connectedness including the health benefits of microbial biodiversity, social and community cohesion, and positive emotions. It promotes a positive mental outlook and healthier behavior in general, including increased physical activity, increased intake of fruits and vegetables, and reduced fast-food consumption.
Ijerph 13 01075 g005
Figure 6. The future of human and societal health will depend on improved training and empathy of health care graduates, who need to be better prepared to understand the up-stream drivers, address the lifestyle diseases they need to prevent and treat, and to be advocates for solutions for the social, cultural and economic determinants of health.
Figure 6. The future of human and societal health will depend on improved training and empathy of health care graduates, who need to be better prepared to understand the up-stream drivers, address the lifestyle diseases they need to prevent and treat, and to be advocates for solutions for the social, cultural and economic determinants of health.
Ijerph 13 01075 g006

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Prescott, S.L.; Logan, A.C. Transforming Life: A Broad View of the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease Concept from an Ecological Justice Perspective. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 1075. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph13111075

AMA Style

Prescott SL, Logan AC. Transforming Life: A Broad View of the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease Concept from an Ecological Justice Perspective. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2016; 13(11):1075. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph13111075

Chicago/Turabian Style

Prescott, Susan L., and Alan C. Logan. 2016. "Transforming Life: A Broad View of the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease Concept from an Ecological Justice Perspective" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 13, no. 11: 1075. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph13111075

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop