Next Article in Journal
Variation in the Effect of Particulate Matter on Pulmonary Function in Schoolchildren in Western Japan and Its Relation with Interleukin-8
Next Article in Special Issue
The Effects of the Urban Built Environment on Mental Health: A Cohort Study in a Large Northern Italian City
Previous Article in Journal
Spatially Explicit Landscape-Level Ecological Risks Induced by Land Use and Land Cover Change in a National Ecologically Representative Region in China
Previous Article in Special Issue
Perceived and Objective Measures of Neighborhood Walkability and Physical Activity among Adults in Japan: A Multilevel Analysis of a Nationally Representative Sample
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Physiological and Psychological Effects of a Walk in Urban Parks in Fall

1
Center for Environment, Health and Field Sciences, Chiba University, 6-2-1 Kashiwa-no-ha, Kashiwa, Chiba 277-0882, Japan
2
Forestry and Forest Products Research Institute, 1 Matsunosato, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8687, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12(11), 14216-14228; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121114216
Submission received: 7 September 2015 / Revised: 26 October 2015 / Accepted: 2 November 2015 / Published: 9 November 2015
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Impacts of the Built Environment on Public Health)

Abstract

:
In recent times, attention has been focused on the role of urban green spaces in promoting human health and well-being. However, there is a lack of evidence-based research on the physiological effects of walking in urban green areas. This study aimed to clarify the physiological and psychological effects of walking in urban parks during fall. Twenty-three males (mean age 22.3 ± 1.2 years) were instructed to walk predetermined 15-min courses in an urban park and in a nearby city area (control). Heart rate and heart rate variability were measured to assess physiological responses, and the semantic differential method, Profile of Mood States, and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory were used to measure psychological responses. We observed that walking in an urban park resulted in a significantly lower heart rate, higher parasympathetic nerve activity, and lower sympathetic nerve activity than walking through the city area. In subjective evaluations, participants were more “comfortable,” “natural,” “relaxed,” and “vigorous” after a walk in the urban park. Furthermore, they exhibited significantly lower levels of negative emotions and anxiety. These findings provide scientific evidence for the physiological and psychological relaxation effects of walking in urban parks during fall.

1. Introduction

Nowadays, most people live in cities, and this trend will likely continue in the future [1]. Urbanization has improved living conditions [2]. Moreover, it is a major factor in increasing life expectancies observed in many populations worldwide [1,3]. In addition, during health and nutrition improvements associated with urbanization, the burden of illness shifts from acute childhood infections to chronic and mostly non-communicable diseases, such as mental health disorders, diabetes, and cardiovascular disease, in adults [1,4,5].
However, from an evolutionary perspective, urbanization is a very drastic change that has occurred over a very short period. Rapid urbanization and artificial environments have caused environmental changes, such as increased traffic, polluted air and water, decreased agricultural land and natural open space [6], and anthropogenic climate change [7]. These changes threaten human health and quality of life [6,7]. Cities have been reported to have higher temperatures and thus act as urban heat islands [8,9,10] that are associated with sensations of discomfort and heat stress [11,12]. Exposure to outdoor urban air pollution is also associated with various adverse health outcomes, including heart disease, respiratory disease, and mortality [13]. Moreover, physical inactivity is recognized as a major problem because of increased sedentary behavior due to transport and office work [14,15].
Furthermore, the rapid spread of information technology in recent years has caused an increase in stress, which was referred to as “techno stress” in 1984 [16]. This is a modern disease of adaptation caused by an inability to cope with new computer technologies in a healthy manner. These stress is associated with poor psychological health [17,18], and many studies have reported the negative physiological impacts of stress on organisms, including humans [19,20,21].
In such a situation, attention has been focused on the role of urban green spaces in promoting human health and well-being. Urban green spaces can enhance the city environment by influencing temperature, wind, humidity, rainfall, soil erosion, flooding, air quality, visual quality, and sound quality, while also encouraging plant and animal diversity [22,23,24,25]. In addition, urban green space may provide important social and psychological benefits that enrich human life [26,27]. Recent demographic studies have found a positive association between exposure to urban green space and the perceived general health of residents [28,29,30]. Living in areas with accessible green spaces for walking also increased the longevity of senior citizens, independent of age, sex, marital status, baseline functional status, and socioeconomic status [30].
It is important to clarify physiological and psychological effects of urban green spaces. In a previous study, we examined the effects of walking in urban parks in spring [31] and winter [32] using the same experimental design and locations as in this study. Walking in an urban park improved positive mood and decreased negative mood and anxiety in both these seasons [31,32]. However, results of the physiological indices differed. A springtime walk in an urban park decreased heart rate, increased parasympathetic nerve activity, and suppressed sympathetic nerve activity compared with a walk in the city streets [31]. During low temperatures in winter, no significant difference in sympathetic nerve activity was detected between the two sites [32]. The effects of walking in urban parks were apparently different between the seasons. However, there is a lack of evidence-based research on the physiological and psychological effects of walking in urban green areas and the effects the seasons may have on these parameters. This study aimed to clarify the physiological and psychological effects of walking in urban parks during fall (autumn).

2. Experimental Section

2.1. Experimental Sites

This field experiment was conducted on 7, 15, and 16 October 2014, in the Kashiwa-no-ha Park (hereafter denoted as the urban park) in Kashiwa City, Chiba Prefecture, Japan. A city area around the urban park (hereafter denoted as the city area) was selected as the control site. The urban park contained many hardwood trees, such as maple, tulip trees, cherry trees, and chestnut. Moreover, there was a large pond in the park center. As shown in Figure 1, in the urban park, the subjects walked around its large pond, whereas in the city area, they walked in the nearby residential area. Weather on the days of the experiment was sunny, except on October 15 when it was drizzling. The temperature, relative humidity, and intensity of illumination of the two experimental sites where the subjects walked (at 9–11 am and 1–3 pm) are shown in Table 1.
Figure 1. Experimental sites.
Figure 1. Experimental sites.
Ijerph 12 14216 g001
Table 1. Physical variables measured in the urban park and city area.
Table 1. Physical variables measured in the urban park and city area.
Value (Mean ± SD)P Value
Urban ParkCity Area
Temperature (°C)18.0 ± 1.719.2 ± 1.90.258
Relative humidity (%)71.5 ± 10.164.7 ± 8.70.244
Intensity of illumination (lx)24,230 ± 11,22038,870 ± 22,3300.192
P value by unpaired t-test.

2.2. Participants

Twenty-three Japanese male university students (mean age: 22.3 ± 1.2 years; Table 2) participated in this experiment. Participants were informed of the study’s aims and procedures prior to conducting the experiment; informed consent was also obtained. Alcohol, tobacco, and caffeine consumption was prohibited during the study period. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Center for Environment, Health, and Field Sciences, Chiba University, Japan (Project identification code number: 5).
Table 2. Participant demographics.
Table 2. Participant demographics.
ParameterValue (Mean ± SD)
Total sample size23
SexMale
Age (years)22.3 ± 1.2
Height (cm)171.1 ± 4.7
Weight (kg)63.4 ± 8.1
BMI (kg/m2)21.5 ± 2.1

2.3. Experimental Design

Each subject walked in the urban park or city area for 15 min (Figure 2). We performed a within-subject experiment; two subjects were paired to eliminate the effect of the order of the sites walked. One subject walked in the urban park first and in the city area later, whereas the other walked in the city area first followed by the urban park. After walking, the subjects returned to the waiting room and completed the questionnaires. They rested for approximately 20 min and repeated the experiment in the reverse order. There were no significant differences in average walking speed between the two environments.
Figure 2. Experimental scenes.
Figure 2. Experimental scenes.
Ijerph 12 14216 g002

2.4. Physiological Indices

Heart rate and heart rate variability (HRV) were measured to assess autonomic nerve activity. HRV was measured using a portable electrocardiograph (Activtracer AC-301A, GMS, Tokyo, Japan); frequency spectra were generated using a HRV software tool (MemCalc/Win, GMS). For real-time HRV analysis using the maximum entropy method, inter-beat (R-R) intervals were continuously measured. Here two broad HRV spectral components were calculated: low (LF; 0.04–0.15 Hz) and high frequencies (HF; 0.15–0.40 Hz). HF component is an estimate of parasympathetic nerve activity, whereas LF/HF ratio is an estimate of sympathetic nerve activity [33,34]. To normalize HRV parameters across the subjects, we used natural logarithmic transformed values for the analysis [35]. Heart rate and HRV data were collected at 1-min intervals for each experimental location; the 15-min average was compared between sites.

2.5. Psychological Indices

Three different questionnaires were used to investigate the psychological responses after walking at each site. The semantic differential (SD) method [36] used three adjective pairs on seven scales, including “comfortable to uncomfortable”, “natural to artificial”, and “relaxed to awakening”. Profile of Mood State (POMS) [37,38,39] scores were determined for the following six sub-scales: “tension–anxiety”, “depression–dejection”, “anger–hostility”, “fatigue”, “confusion”, and “vigor”. A short form of POMS was used. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [40,41] was used to evaluate anxiety.

2.6. Statistical Analyses

Physiological data were used from 20 participants (data from the remaining three participants were excluded because of data collection errors). A paired t-test was used to compare mean physiological parameters between the two sites. Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to analyze differences in the psychological indices reported after walking in the two environments. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 20.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). Statistical significance was fixed at p < 0.05. A one-side test was used in this study on the basis of the hypothesis that humans would also be relaxed by walking in urban parks during fall because it was found that nature-derived stimulation confers physiological and psychological relaxation effects, as reported in our previous research [31,32,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50].

3. Results

We confirmed that there were no significant differences in the walking speed between the two environments (urban park: 4.11 km/h; city area: 4.07 km/h; p > 0.05), and energy expenditure in the urban park and the city area was 2.67 kcal/min and 2.82 kcal/min, respectively. Furthermore, before the walk, the baseline of physiological indices did not significantly differ between the two areas.
The participants exhibited statistically significant differences in their physiological and psychological responses to the 15-min walk in the urban park and city areas. Figure 3 shows the natural logarithm of the HF component ln(HF), which is an estimate of the parasympathetic nerve activity. In the 1-min segment analysis, all ln(HF) values were higher when participants walked in the urban park than when they walked in the city area (Figure 3A). The mean ln(HF) over the entire walking period was significantly higher in the urban park walking than in the city area walking (urban park: 4.1 ± 0.2 lnms2; city area: 3.6 ± 0.2 lnms2; p < 0.01, Figure 3B). In contrast, the natural logarithm of LF/HF (ln(LF/HF)), an estimate of sympathetic nerve activity, was lower during the urban park walk in the 1-min segment analysis (Figure 4A), whereas the average ln(LF/HF) over 15 min was significantly lower than that during the city walk (urban park: 1.54 ± 0.13; city area: 1.93 ± 0.13; p < 0.01, Figure 4B).
Figure 3. The 1-min averages and the overall mean ln(HF) value of heart rate variability during the urban park walk and the city area walk. (A) Change in each 1-min ln(HF) value; (B) Overall mean ln(HF) values. N = 20, mean ± standard error. ** p < 0.01, determined by the paired t-test (one-sided).
Figure 3. The 1-min averages and the overall mean ln(HF) value of heart rate variability during the urban park walk and the city area walk. (A) Change in each 1-min ln(HF) value; (B) Overall mean ln(HF) values. N = 20, mean ± standard error. ** p < 0.01, determined by the paired t-test (one-sided).
Ijerph 12 14216 g003
Figure 4. The 1-min averages and the overall mean ln(LF/HF) value of heart rate variability during the urban park walk and the city area walk. (A) Change in each 1-min ln(LF/HF) value; (B) Overall mean ln(LF/HF) values. N = 20, mean ± standard error. ** p < 0.01, determined by the paired t-test (one sided).
Figure 4. The 1-min averages and the overall mean ln(LF/HF) value of heart rate variability during the urban park walk and the city area walk. (A) Change in each 1-min ln(LF/HF) value; (B) Overall mean ln(LF/HF) values. N = 20, mean ± standard error. ** p < 0.01, determined by the paired t-test (one sided).
Ijerph 12 14216 g004
In addition, mean heart rate values within the 1-min segments were lower during the urban park walk than during the city area walk, except for the period of 1 min (Figure 5A). The mean heart rate over the entire 15-min period was significantly lower during the urban park walk than during the city walk (urban park: 86.4 ± 1.6 bpm; city area: 89.7 ± 1.6 bpm; p < 0.01, Figure 5B).
This study also included data for five subjects who walked on a rainy day. We examined additional data from these subjects. Compared with the city area walk, the mean ln(HF) over the entire walking period was marginally significantly enhanced (urban park: 4.2 ± 0.6 lnms2; city area: 3.9 ± 0.6 lnms2; p = 0.082), and the heart rate was marginally significantly reduced (urban park: 88.0. ± 2.4 bpm; city area: 90.0 ± 2.7 bpm; p = 0.066) during the brief walk in the urban park. Although there was no significant difference in ln(LF/HF).
Figure 5. The 1-min averages and the overall mean heart rate during the urban park walk and the city area walk. (A) Changes in each 1-min average heart rate over the 15-min walk; (B) Overall mean heart rates. N = 20, mean ± standard error. ** p < 0.01, determined by the paired t-test (one-sided).
Figure 5. The 1-min averages and the overall mean heart rate during the urban park walk and the city area walk. (A) Changes in each 1-min average heart rate over the 15-min walk; (B) Overall mean heart rates. N = 20, mean ± standard error. ** p < 0.01, determined by the paired t-test (one-sided).
Ijerph 12 14216 g005
The analysis of the responses to the three questionnaires completed after the urban park and city area walks, including the SD method, POMS scores, and STAI scores, revealed differences in psychological responses between the two environments. Significantly higher SD scores were observed following the urban park walk than those following the city area walk for the following three adjectives: “comfortable”, “natural”, and “relaxed” (p < 0.01, Figure 6). Differences were also detected in the POMS test with scores for the negative subscales of “tension–anxiety”, “anger–hostility”, “fatigue”, and “confusion” being significantly lower after walking in the urban park than after walking in the city area (p < 0.05, Figure 7). Conversely, the positive mood state “vigor” was significantly higher after the urban park walk (p < 0.01, Figure 7). The score of “depression-dejection” was marginally significantly lower after walking in the urban park than after walking in the city area (p < 0.10, Figure 7). Finally, the total STAI score was 19.3% significantly lower after the urban park walk than after the city area walk (urban park: 39.0 ± 6.3; city area: 48.4 ± 7.5; p < 0.01, Figure 8).

4. Discussion

The findings indicated that a 15-min walk in an urban park induced physiological relaxation. Compared with those after a brief walk in the city area, parasympathetic nerve activity was significantly enhanced, sympathetic nerve activity was significantly suppressed, and heart rate was significantly lower during a brief walk in the urban park. These data are partly in agreement with those from previous studies investigating physiological responses to urban park walks during spring and winter [31,32]. Furthermore, in previous studies, the relaxation effects of walking or being in nature forests relative to those in an urban area were shown. These studies reported decreased blood pressure [42,43,44,45,46] and pulse rate [42,43,44,45,46,47,48], suppressed sympathetic nerve activity [42,44,45,46,47,48,49], increased parasympathetic nerve activity [42,44,45,46,47,48,49], decreased salivary cortisol levels [43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50], and decreased cerebral blood flow in the prefrontal cortex [50]. Recently, it has also been demonstrated that brief walking in a nature setting decreases not only self-reported rumination but also neural activity in the subgenual prefrontal cortex that is linked to mental illness risk [51]. The results of this study reveal an increased parasympathetic nerve activity and decreased sympathetic nerve activity and heart rate, and these observations are in accordance with the previous results for forest and urban walking, suggesting that even small natural areas within a larger urban area can confer similar health benefits.
Figure 6. Comparison of subjective scoring for “comfortable”, “natural”, and “relaxed” feelings between the two environments according to the semantic differential method. N = 23, mean ± standard deviation. ** p < 0.01, determined by the Wilcoxon signed-rank test (one-sided).
Figure 6. Comparison of subjective scoring for “comfortable”, “natural”, and “relaxed” feelings between the two environments according to the semantic differential method. N = 23, mean ± standard deviation. ** p < 0.01, determined by the Wilcoxon signed-rank test (one-sided).
Ijerph 12 14216 g006
Figure 7. Comparison of subjective Profile of Mood State (POMS) scores between the two environments. T–A, tension–anxiety; D, depression–dejection; A–H, anger–hostility; F, fatigue; C, confusion; V, vigor. N = 23, mean ± standard deviation. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, determined by the Wilcoxon signed-rank test (one-sided).
Figure 7. Comparison of subjective Profile of Mood State (POMS) scores between the two environments. T–A, tension–anxiety; D, depression–dejection; A–H, anger–hostility; F, fatigue; C, confusion; V, vigor. N = 23, mean ± standard deviation. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, determined by the Wilcoxon signed-rank test (one-sided).
Ijerph 12 14216 g007
Figure 8. Comparison of subjective State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) scores between the two environments. N = 23, mean ± standard deviation. ** p < 0.01, determined by the Wilcoxon signed-rank test (one sided).
Figure 8. Comparison of subjective State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) scores between the two environments. N = 23, mean ± standard deviation. ** p < 0.01, determined by the Wilcoxon signed-rank test (one sided).
Ijerph 12 14216 g008
According to the three questionnaires, the subjects in this study felt more “comfortable”, “natural”, “relaxed”, and “vigorous” after a walk in the urban park. In addition, negative emotions and anxiety were significantly lower after the urban park walk. These results, which exhibit the psychological benefits of walking in an urban park, are partly consistent with previous findings [31,32,42,43,44,45,48,49,50]. In modern times, mental health problems associated with living in urban environment are profound. For example, McKenzie et al. reported that urban living environments are associated with higher rates of psychotropic medication prescriptions for anxiety, depression, and psychosis [52]. Therefore, the psychological benefits of walking in urban green space, (i.e., improved mood state and decreased anxiety level) are very significant, and urban green space is expected to play a very important role in the promotion of mental health in the future.
The beneficial effects of urban green space suggest a simple, accessible, and cost-effective method to improve the quality of life and health of urban residents. In a previous study, in addition to fall, we examined the effects of walking in the urban park in spring [31] and winter [32] using the same experimental design and locations. The current results corroborate our previous findings. However, in the comparisons of the three seasons, in winter, there was no significant difference detected in sympathetic nervous activity between the two sites, whereas all other parameters (parasympathetic nervous activity, heart rate, and psychological indices) demonstrated similar differences between the walking environments. We do not know the exact reason for the different results obtained for winter; however, we suppose they resulted from seasonal differences in environmental conditions, such as temperature (spring: 24.7 °C, fall: 18.0 °C, and winter: 13.8 °C), humidity (spring: 39.2%, fall: 71.5%, and winter: 50.9%), and wind speed as well as the state of trees, such as the color and quantity of leaves. This issue will need to be considered in more depth in the future after more data is accumulated.
This study also included data for five subjects who were out walking on a rainy day. Most previous studies relating to the physiological effect of the natural environment were conducted on a sunny day. To the best of our knowledge, there are no evidence-based research studies that clarify the physiological and psychological effects of walking in an urban park on a rainy day. Therefore, although the sample size is small, we examined these subjects who walked in an urban park and city area on a rainy day. As a result, similar results were obtained. Compared with the city area walk, parasympathetic nerve activity was marginally significantly enhanced (urban park: 4.2 ± 0.6 lnms2; city area: 3.9 ± 0.6 lnms2; p = 0.082), and heart rate was marginally significantly reduced (urban park: 88.0. ± 2.4 bpm; city area: 90.0 ± 2.7 bpm; p = 0.066) during a brief walk in the urban park. It has been observed that walking in an urban park confers an effect of physiological relaxation even on a rainy day.
These findings provide scientific evidence for the beneficial physiological and psychological effects of walking in urban parks during fall. However, this study had several limitations. First, to generalize the findings, further studies based on a larger sample (including various other demographic groups, such as females and different ages) are required. Second, for overall discussion, future studies should determine the effects of urban green space using other physiological indices, such as brain activity, autonomic nerve activity, and endocrine activity. Finally, the subjects’ prior expectations and experience with nature and nature relatedness may influence the results. All these limitations must be considered in future research.

5. Conclusions

These findings provide scientific evidence for the beneficial physiological and psychological effects of walking in urban parks during fall. A brief walk in an urban park can induce parasympathetic nerve activity, suppress sympathetic nerve activity, decrease the heart rate, enhance the mood state, and reduce anxiety. In conclusion, walking in urban parks confers physiological and psychological relaxation effects during fall.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Council for Science, Technology and Innovation (CSTI), Cross-ministerial Strategic Innovation Promotion Program (SIP), “Technologies for creating next-generation agriculture, forestry and fisheries” (funding agency: Bio-oriented Technology Research Advancement Institution, NARO). This study was performed with the permission of the Corporation for Urban Enhancement of Chiba Prefecture, Japan. We also appreciate their co-operation. We are grateful to Mr. Taku Okuda for his valuable contributions in the data collection phase of this study.

Author Contributions

Chorong Song contributed to the experimental design, data acquisition, statistical analysis, interpretation of results, and manuscript preparation. Harumi Ikei and Miho Igarashi contributed to data acquisition and statistical analysis. Michiko Takagaki participated in study design and data interpretation. Yoshifumi Miyazaki contributed to the study design, interpretation of results, and manuscript preparation. All authors contributed to the preparation of this manuscript and are responsible for the final version.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Dye, C. Health and urban living. Science 2008, 319, 766–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Vlahov, D.; Freudenberg, N.; Proietti, F.; Ompad, D.; Quinn, A.; Nandi, V.; Galea, S. Urban as a determinant of health. J. Urban Health 2007, 84, 16–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Zuckerman, M.K. Modern Environments and Human Health: Revisiting the Second Epidemiological Transition; Wiley-Blackwell: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  4. Srakash, P. Nutrition transition and its health outcomes. Indian J. Pediatr. 2013, 80, S21–S27. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  5. Wagner, K.H.; Brath, H. A global view on the development of non communicable diseases. Prev. Med. 2012, 54, S38–S41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Pronczuk, J.; Surdu, S. Children’s environmental health in the twenty-first century. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2008, 1140, 143–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Patz, J.A.; Campbell, L.D.; Holloway, T.; Foley, J.A. Impact of regional climate change on human health. Nature 2005, 438, 310–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Basara, J.B.; Basara, H.G.; Illston, B.G.; Crawford, K.C. The impact of the urban heat island during an intense heat wave in Oklahoma City. Adv. Meteorol. 2010, 2010, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Changnon, S.A.; Kunkel, K.E.; Reinke, B.C. Impacts and responses to the 1995 heat wave: A call to action. Bull. Am. Meteorol. Soc. 1996, 77, 1497–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kosatsky, T. The 2003 European heat waves. Euro. Surveill. 2005, 10, 148–149. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  11. Kovats, R.S.; Hajat, S. Heat stress and public health: A critical review. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2008, 29, 41–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Abdel-Ghany, A.M.; Al-Helal, I.M.; Shady, M.R. Human thermal comfort and heat stress in an outdoor urban arid environment: A case study. Adv. Meteorol. 2013, 2013, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Brunekreef, B.; Holgate, S.T. Air pollution and health. Lancet 2002, 360, 1233–1242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Lee, I.M.; Shiroma, E.J.; Lobelo, F.; Puska, P.; Blair, S.N.; Katzmarzyk, P.T.; Lancet Physical Activity Series Working Group. Effect of physical inactivity on major non-communicable diseases worldwide: An analysis of burden of disease and life expectancy. Lancet 2012, 380, 219–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kohl, H.W.; Craig, C.L.; Lambert, E.V.; Inoue, S.; Alkandari, J.R.; Leetongin, G.; Kahlmeier, S.; Lancet Physical Activity Series Working Group. The pandemic of physical inactivity: Global action for public health. Lancet 2012, 380, 294–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Craig, B. Technostress: The Human Cost of the Computer Revolution; Addison-Wesley Publishing Company: Boston, MA, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  17. Salanova, M.; Llorens, S.; Cifre, E. The dark side of technologies: Technostress among users of information and communication technologies. Int. J. Psychol. 2013, 48, 422–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Misra, S.; Stokols, D. Psychological and health outcomes of perceived information overload. Environ. Behav. 2012, 44, 737–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Herbert, T.B.; Cohen, S. Stress and immunity in humans: A meta-analytic review. Psychosom. Med. 1993, 55, 364–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Gémes, K.; Ahnve, S.; Janszky, I. Inflammation a possible link between economical stress and coronary heart disease. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2008, 23, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Lederbogen, F.; Kirsch, P.; Haddad, L.; Streit, F.; Tost, H.; Schuch, P.; Wüst, S.; Pruessner, J.C.; Rietschel, M.; Deuschle, M.; et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature 2011, 474, 498–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Dwyer, J.F.; McPherson, E.G.; Schroeder, H.W.; Rowntree, R.A. Assessing the benefits and costs of the urban forest. J. Arboric. 1992, 18, 227–234. [Google Scholar]
  23. Saaroni, H.; Ziv, B. The impact of a small lake on heat stress in a Mediterranean urban park: The case of Tel Aviv, Israel. Int. J. Biometeorol. 2003, 47, 156–165. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  24. Cohen, P.; Potchter, O.; Schnell, I. The impact of an urban park on air pollution and noise levels in the Mediterranean city of Tel-Aviv, Israel. Environ. Pollut. 2014, 195, 73–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Tse, M.S.; Chau, C.K.; Choy, Y.S.; Tsui, W.K.; Chan, C.N.; Tang, S.K. Perception of urban park soundscape. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2012, 131, 2762–2771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Chiesura, A. The role of urban parks for the sustainable city. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2004, 68, 129–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Nutsford, D.; Pearson, A.L.; Kingham, S. An ecological study investigating the association between access to urban green space and mental health. Public Health 2013, 127, 1005–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Maas, J.; Verheij, R.A.; Groenewegen, P.P.; Vries, S.D.; Spreeuwenberg, P. Green space, urbanity, and health: How strong is the relation? J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2006, 60, 587–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Mitchell, R.; Popham, F. Effect of exposure to natural environment on health inequalities: An observational population study. Lancet 2008, 372, 1655–1660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Takano, T.; Nakamura, K.; Watanabe, M. Urban residential environments and senior citizens’ longevity in megacity areas: The importance of walkable green spaces. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2002, 56, 913–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Igarashi, M.; Miwa, M.; Takagaki, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological responses of young males during spring-time walks in urban parks. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2014, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Song, C.; Joung, D.; Ikei, H.; Igarashi, M.; Aga, M.; Park, B.J.; Miwa, M.; Takagaki, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological effects of walking on young males in urban parks in winter. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2013, 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Circulation 1996, 93, 1043–1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Pagani, M.; Lombardi, F.; Guzzetti, S.; Rimoldi, O.; Furlan, R.; Pizzinelli, P.; Sandrone, G.; Malfatto, G.; Dell’Orto, S.; Piccaluga, E.; et al. Power spectral analysis of heart rate and arterial pressure variabilities as a marker of sympatho-vagal interaction in man and conscious dog. Circ. Res. 1986, 59, 178–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Kobayashi, H.; Park, B.J.; Miyazaki, Y. Normative references of heart rate variability and salivary alpha-amylase in a healthy young male population. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2012, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Osgood, C.E.; Suci, G.J.; Tannenbaum, P. The Measurement of Meaning; University of Illinois Press: Urbana, IL, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
  37. McNair, D.M.; Lorr, M. An analysis of mood in neurotics. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1964, 69, 620–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. McNair, D.M.; Lorr, M.; Droppleman, L. Profile of Mood States Manual; Educational and Industrial Testing Services: San Diego, CA, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  39. Yokoyama, K. POMS Shortened Version-Manual and Commentary on Cases; Kaneko Syoboh: Tokyo, Japan, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  40. Spielberger, C.D. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: STAI (Form Y); Consulting Psychologist Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  41. Hidano, N.; Fukuhara, M.; Iwawaki, M.; Soga, S.; Spielberger, C.D. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory—Form JYZ; Jitsumu-Kyoiku Syuppan: Tokyo, Japan, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  42. Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kasetani, T.; Morikawa, T.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of forest recreation in a young conifer forest in Hinokage town, Japan. Silva Fenn. 2009, 43, 291–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Lee, J.; Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Restorative effects of viewing real forest landscapes, based on a comparison with urban landscapes. Scand. J. For. Res. 2009, 24, 227–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Tsunetsugu, Y.; Park, B.J.; Ishii, H.; Hirano, H.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the atmosphere of the forest) in an old-growth broadleaf forest in Yamagata prefecture, Japan. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2007, 26, 135–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Tsunetsugu, Y.; Lee, J.; Park, B.J.; Tyrväinend, L.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological effects of viewing urban forest landscapes assessed by multiple measurement. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013, 113, 90–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Lee, J.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Effect of the forest environment on physiological relaxation using the results of field tests at 35 sites throughout Japan. In Forest Medicine; Li, Q., Ed.; Nova Science Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 55–65. [Google Scholar]
  47. Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Ishii, H.; Furuhashi, S.; Hirano, H.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the atmosphere of the forest) in a mixed forest in Shinano Town, Japan. Scand. J. For. Res. 2008, 23, 278–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Lee, J.; Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Ohira, T.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Effect of forest bathing on physiological and psychological responses in young Japanese male subjects. Public Health 2011, 125, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Lee, J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Takayama, N.; Park, B.J.; Li, Q.; Song, C.; Komatsu, M.; Ikei, H.; Tyrväinen, L.; Kagawa, T.; et al. Influence of forest therapy on cardiovascular relaxation in young adults. Evid.-Based Complement. Altern. Med. 2014, 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kasetani, T.; Hirano, H.; Kagawa, T.; Sato, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of Shinrin-yoku (taking in the atmosphere of the forest)—Using salivary cortisol and cerebral activity as indicators. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2007, 26, 123–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Bratman, G.N.; Hamilton, J.P.; Hahn, K.S.; Daily, G.C.; Gross, J.J. Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual prefrontal cortex activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 8567–8572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. McKenzie, K.; Murray, A.L.; Booth, T. Do urban environments increase the risk of anxiety, depression and psychosis? An epidemiological study. J. Affect. Disord. 2013, 150, 1019–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Igarashi, M.; Takagaki, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and Psychological Effects of a Walk in Urban Parks in Fall. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 14216-14228. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121114216

AMA Style

Song C, Ikei H, Igarashi M, Takagaki M, Miyazaki Y. Physiological and Psychological Effects of a Walk in Urban Parks in Fall. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2015; 12(11):14216-14228. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121114216

Chicago/Turabian Style

Song, Chorong, Harumi Ikei, Miho Igarashi, Michiko Takagaki, and Yoshifumi Miyazaki. 2015. "Physiological and Psychological Effects of a Walk in Urban Parks in Fall" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 12, no. 11: 14216-14228. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph121114216

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop