Next Article in Journal
Effects of Vitamin D Supplementation on Bone Turnover Markers: A Randomized Controlled Trial
Next Article in Special Issue
Dietary Patterns among Vietnamese and Hispanic Immigrant Elementary School Children Participating in an After School Program
Previous Article in Journal
Factors Associated with Decreased Lean Tissue Index in Patients with Chronic Kidney Disease
Previous Article in Special Issue
Intake Levels of Fish in the UK Paediatric Population
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Snacking Quality Is Associated with Secondary School Academic Achievement and the Intention to Enroll in Higher Education: A Cross-Sectional Study in Adolescents from Santiago, Chile

1
Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology, University of Chile, Santiago 7830490, Chile
2
Division of Child Development and Community Health, University of California, San Diego, CA 92093, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2017, 9(5), 433; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9050433
Submission received: 16 March 2017 / Revised: 20 April 2017 / Accepted: 24 April 2017 / Published: 27 April 2017
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Dietary Intake and Behavior in Children)

Abstract

:
Although numerous studies have approached the effects of exposure to a Western diet (WD) on academic outcomes, very few have focused on foods consumed during snack times. We explored whether there is a link between nutritious snacking habits and academic achievement in high school (HS) students from Santiago, Chile. We conducted a cross-sectional study with 678 adolescents. The nutritional quality of snacks consumed by 16-year-old was assessed using a validated food frequency questionnaire. The academic outcomes measured were HS grade point average (GPA), the likelihood of HS completion, and the likelihood of taking college entrance exams. A multivariate analysis was performed to determine the independent associations of nutritious snacking with having completed HS and having taken college entrance exams. An analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) estimated the differences in GPA by the quality of snacks. Compared to students with healthy in-home snacking behaviors, adolescents having unhealthy in-home snacks had significantly lower GPAs (M difference: −40.1 points, 95% confidence interval (CI): −59.2, −16.9, d = 0.41), significantly lower odds of HS completion (adjusted odds ratio (aOR): 0.47; 95% CI: 0.25–0.88), and significantly lower odds of taking college entrance exams (aOR: 0.53; 95% CI: 0.31–0.88). Unhealthy at-school snacking showed similar associations with the outcome variables. Poor nutritional quality snacking at school and at home was associated with poor secondary school academic achievement and the intention to enroll in higher education.

1. Introduction

In spite of efforts by public agencies to monitor the types of food sold in school settings or regulate food advertising aimed at young people, their exposure to energy-dense foods (those with a high caloric concentration per bite) at and away from school remains high [1]. A recent study on the consumption of fast food in 36 developed and developing countries showed that more than 50% of adolescents consume fast food frequently or very frequently [2]. In Latin America, the Global School-based Health Survey (GSHS) showed that two-thirds of adolescents (13–17 years old) in Argentina, Chile and Uruguay reported daily intake of sugar-sweetened beverages [3]. In the early 2010s, among European 15-year-old, daily soft drink consumption was more than 40% in England, the Netherlands, Belgium, Slovakia and Slovenia [4].
Evidence is available on the role of Western-type diets (WD) in limiting cognitive abilities in critical brain maturation periods (i.e., infancy and childhood) [5,6]. Animal models show that exposure to a high-fat, high-sugar (HFS) diet in adolescence is related to impairment in hippocampal learning and memory processes, regardless of weight status [7,8]. One important mechanism that is proposed to underlie HFS-induced impaired hippocampal function is the reduced synthesis, secretion, and action of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). BDNF facilitates synaptic efficacy by converting changes in electrical activity to long-lasting changes in synaptic function, which is a suggested key process for memory formation [9]. Reduced levels of BDNF in association with impaired memory function has been well documented in the literature [10,11].
Impairment of memory consolidation and memory performance is a risk factor for learning difficulties and poor academic progress [12]. Thus, a diet of poor nutritional value may compromise students’ ability to perform well in school. Longitudinal and cross-sectional studies, mostly conducted in developed countries, have examined the relationship between diet and school grades [13,14,15], as well as the relationship between diet and performance on standardized academic tests [16,17,18]. Results collectively suggest that better educational outcomes are associated with regular consumption of nutritious breakfasts, lower intake of energy-dense, nutrient-poor foods, and maintaining a healthy diet [19].
The effect of WDs on academic results can be used to strengthen health promotion strategies. While the connection between unhealthy diet and poor academic performance (as measured by school grades and standardized test scores) in elementary and middle schoolers has been well described, less is known about the relationship between dietary habits and postsecondary educational aspirations—that is, the intention to pursue higher education after secondary school. The increasing number of HS graduates seeking entrance to higher education institutions, including in non-industrialized nations, has made this a particularly important topic for students, families and policymakers.
Since the question of how WD foods may compromise students’ intention to pursue higher education is also of interest to non-academic audiences, we used a translational-research approach to provide evidence that can be translated from research and applied to practice and policy. Thus, we examined the relationship of nutritional quality of snacks with academic outcomes using functional cognition measures like grade point average (GPA), high school (HS) completion, and college entrance examination participation rates. Our decision to concentrate on snacks rather than overall diet or meals such as breakfast, lunch or supper was based on wanting to focus on food choices made by adolescents rather than consumption of foods over which they may have little volition. We hypothesized that students habitually eating unhealthy snacks would have lower grades and be less likely to complete HS and take college admission exams.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Population

We studied 16–17-year-old adolescents living in Santiago, Chile, from low-to-middle socioeconomic status (SES), who were part of an infancy cohort. Participants were recruited at 4 months from public healthcare facilities in the southeast area of Santiago (n = 1791). They were born at term of uncomplicated vaginal births, weighed >3.0 kg, and were free of acute or chronic health problems. At 6 months, infants free of iron deficiency anemia (n = 1657) were randomly assigned to receive iron supplementation or no added iron (ages 6–12 months). They were assessed for developmental outcomes in infancy, and at 5, 10 and 15 years [20]. At 16–17 years, those with complete data in each wave (n = 678) were also assessed for obesity risk and the presence of cardiovascular risk factors in a half-day evaluation that included assessment of dietary habits and nutritional content of food intake. Ethical approval was obtained by the institutional review boards of the University of Michigan, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology (INTA), University of Chile, and the University of California, San Diego. Participants and their primary caregiver provided informed and written consent, according to the norms for Human Experimentation, Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki, 1995).

2.2. Nutritional Quality of Snacking at Age 16

Nutritional quality of in-school and at-home snacking was measured considering the amount of saturated fat, fiber, sugar and salt in the food. Assessment was performed with a food frequency questionnaire, validated using three 24 h recalls to include weekends [21,22]. A section of this questionnaire was specially designed to assess the usual diet during the snack time at school and at home, by asking about the frequency of food consumption within the past three months. A list of 50 foods and beverages was used. The frequency of food consumption was assessed by a multiple response grid; respondents were asked to estimate how often a particular food or beverage was consumed. Categories ranged from ”never” to ”five or more times a week”. The electronic version of the Chilean Food Composition Tables/Database was used to assess the quality of snacks composition [23]. Food items were classified as unhealthy (poor nutritional value items, high in fat, sugar, salt and calories), unhealthy-to-fair (highly processed items although low in fat) and healthy (nutrient rich foods). We assigned adjustment weights to each food item conditioned to its nutritional quality. A score ranging from 0–10 was computed by adjusting the frequency of food consumption to the nutritional quality of foods consumed during the snack time. For each snacking type (in-school or at-home), participants had a continuous score, with higher scores representing healthier snacking habits. We applied quartile cutoffs for the Chilean adolescent population (comprising students of high-, middle- and low-SES) to classify the nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking of participants into three groups: unhealthy (≤4.3 or ≤25th percentile), unhealthy-to-fair (from 4.4 to 5.9 or >25th percentile and <75th percentile) and healthy (≥6.0 or ≥75th percentile) [21].

2.3. Academic Outcomes

The academic outcomes measured were HS GPA, the likelihood of HS completion, and the likelihood of taking college entrance exams. Data on GPA and high school completion were obtained from publicly available records at the Academic Assessment Unit of the Ministry of Education of Chile. Following the Ministry of Education criteria, GPA (on a scale of 1–7) was transformed into standardized scores (ranging from 210–825), and adjusted by type of secondary education (academic, vocational or adult school). Data on college examination rates were derived from publicly available information from the Assessment and Measurement Department of the University of Chile, which administers the tests for college entrance on behalf of the Ministry of Education. Although the exams for college admission are non-mandatory for HS graduates (only for those aiming at enrolling in higher education), more than 85% of Chilean HS graduates take the tests and, thus, have plans for future schooling [24].

2.4. Weight Status at Age 16

A research physician used standardized procedures to measure the adolescent’s height (cm) and weight (kg) in duplicate. Body mass index (BMI = kg/m2) at age 16 was evaluated and z-scores were estimated according to the World Health Organization (WHO) 2007 references [25]. Weight status was defined as follows: underweight (BMI-z < −1 SD), normal weight (BMI-z from −1 SD to 1 SD), overweight (BMI-z from 1 SD to <2 SD) and obesity (BMI-z ≥ 2 SD).

2.5. Physical Activity at Age 16

Physical activity has been found to be associated with academic achievement in studies conducted in Chile [26,27]; therefore, it could be a relevant confounder for the association between diet and academic results. We approached physical activity habits with scheduled, repetitive and planned exercise, accounting for the number of weekly hours devoted to Physical Education (PE), and extracurricular sports. To measure this, we used a questionnaire that was validated in a previous study using accelerometry-based activity monitors in both elementary and high school children [28]. The questionnaire was administered by a researcher to all students at the time they attended the anthropometric examination. Participants were asked: (1) On average, over the past week, how often did you engage in PE? (2) On average, over the past week, how often did you engage in extracurricular sports, either school- or non-school-organized? (3) On those days, on average, how long did you engage in such activities? With this information, we estimated the average hours per week of scheduled physical activity. Participants having ≤90 min of weekly scheduled physical activity, which is the mandatory time for school-based PE, were considered to be physically inactive.

2.6. Other Covariates Collected in Previous Waves

Parental educational attainment is an important measure of human capital level among populations and, also, is an important predictor of children’s educational outcomes [29]. In infancy, participant’s mother and father were asked to report the highest schooling level they have been enrolled in, as well as the highest grade they completed at that level. In our analysis, five standard hierarchic levels were defined according to the 2011 International Standard Classification of Education: (1) no education completed; (2) first level (primary school or 1st–8th); (3) secondary level (first phase or 9th–10th); (4) secondary level (second phase or 11th–12th); and (5) post-secondary non-tertiary educations or short-cycle tertiary education [30]. Then, we merged these categories into two: incomplete secondary education (1 + 2 + 3), and complete secondary education or higher (4 + 5). In health research, parental education has been often used as proxy for socioeconomic background [31]. Also, because the literature describes correlations between children’s educational outcomes and family structure [32], we include a variable denoting whether the participant was raised in a fatherless family. This information was reported by the participant’s parents or guardian. Finally, to control potential design biases, we used a categorical variable denoting whether the participant had received iron supplementation or no added iron at 6–12 months.

2.7. Statistical Analysis

Data were processed using Stata SE for Windows 12.0 (Lakeway Drive College Station, TX, USA). All categorical data were expressed as absolute and relative frequencies, while continuous data were expressed as means and standard deviations. Statistical analysis included χ2 for categorical variables, and analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni correction for comparison of means. We tested for effect measure modification (interaction) by weight status and physical activity, in the association between quality of snacking and academic outcomes using two-way ANOVA. The interaction of quality of snacking with weight status and physical activity was non-significant at p < 0.05 and, therefore, we did not stratify the analysis. Unadjusted logistic models were used to explore cross-sectional patterns of variation in academic behavior across snack categories (unhealthy and unhealthy-to-fair vs. healthy). Next, the models were adjusted for sex, weight status, physical activity, familial background and a variable to control potential design biases. Odds ratios are presented in the tables with 95% CI to evaluate the strength and precision of the associations. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to determine whether high school GPA differed by nutritional quality of snacking, accounting for the same potential confounders. Because GPA scores do not have an intrinsic meaning, the effect size for difference was estimated using Cohen’s d coefficients. A p < 0.05 denoted statistical significance.

3. Results

As shown in Table 1, our sample was composed of 16.8-year-old (0.3 SD) adolescents (47% males). Eighty-four percent completed HS (n = 571) and were allowed to take the exams for college admission. Of them, 68% (n = 388) took the college entrance exam. High school GPA ranged from 269–795 points, and mean value was 481.1 (92.3 SD) points. Mean value of BMI-z was 0.65 (1.2 SD). Of the participants, 25% and 14% were overweight and obese, respectively. In the sample, 60% were physically inactive.
The share of students completing the secondary education significantly increased with better nutritional quality of at-school (χ2 = 6.73, p < 0.05) and in-home (χ2 = 7.19, p < 0.05) snacking (Figure 1). Likewise, the proportion of students taking the exams for higher education was significantly higher among participants having healthy in-home (χ2 = 12.40, p < 0.01) and at-school (χ2 = 11.66, p < 0.01) snacking (Figure 2).
Table 2 shows the estimated cross-sectional association between graduating HS and the nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking. After adjusting for sex, weight status, physical activity, parental education, family structure and iron supplementation in infancy, unhealthy snacking significantly reduced the odds of completing the secondary education. For instance, students having unhealthy in-home snacks were 53% (odds ratio (OR): 0.47, 95% CI: 0.25–0.88) less likely to complete HS than students having healthy in-home snacks. Odds were lower but non-significant among students eating foods of unhealthy-to-fair nutritional quality at home compared to those eating healthy snacks at home. When school snacking was the exposure, we also found a positive significant association of nutritional quality of snacks with the likelihood of getting the HS diploma (aOR: 0.49, 95% CI: 0.27–0.89). In all these models, sex and physical activity were also related to the chances of HS graduation.
Similarly, among students who completed HS, the odds of taking the college entrance exam were significantly lower for those having unhealthy in-home snacks compared to those having healthy in-home snacks (Table 3). After controlling other influences, students who reported consumption of unhealthy in-home snacks were 47% less likely (aOR: 0.53; 95% CI: 0.31–0.88) to take the college entrance exam, compared to students eating healthy snack items. In addition, students eating in-home snacks of unhealthy-to-fair nutritional quality had lower odds of taking the college entrance exam, compared to those with healthier habits, though the association was non-significant. When school snacking was the exposure, the odds of taking the examination for college were also lower in students eating unhealthy snacks (aOR: 0.57; 95% CI: 0.35–0.90) compared to those eating healthy at-school snacks. In all these models, the odds of taking the college entrance exam were significantly associated with sex, maternal education and family structure.
The nutritional quality of snacking was also significantly related with students’ final GPA as shown in Figure 3. After accounting for the effect of sex, weight status, physical activity, parental education, family structure and iron supplementation in infancy (Table 4), the group snacking on unhealthy foods at home had a final GPA of 490.0 points, on average, whereas participants having healthy snacks at home had a final GPA of 530.1 points (GPA mean difference = −40.1 points; 95% CI: −59.2; −16.9, d = 0.43). When comparing those having unhealthy-to-fair snacks vs. those having healthy snacks at home the GPA mean difference was −27.9 points (95% CI: −43.5; −8.2, d = 0.30). It is worth noting that Cohen’s d coefficients around 0.20 are considered of interest in educational research when they are based on measures of academic achievement [33]. Lastly, the same pattern was observed when the main exposure was the nutritional quality of at-school snacking.

4. Discussion

4.1. Main Findings

This study explored whether the nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking among high school students in Santiago, Chile, was cross-sectionally associated with secondary school academic achievement and the intention to enroll in higher education. Although numerous studies have approached the effects of short-term exposure to a WD on academic outcomes [19], very few have focused on foods consumed during snack times. We found that unhealthy snacking was correlated with lower high school GPA and rate of graduation, as well as a reduced likelihood of taking college admission exams. When controls for sex and other potentially confounding variables (e.g., weight status, physical activity, familial background, etc.) were entered into the models, unhealthy snacking continued to be associated with worse academic results.
Our findings are consistent with previous research that found evidence of a relationship between a healthy diet and academic achievement. The results of a population-based study of 4th and 8th grade Chilean school children—students from subsidized, partially subsidized, and private schools—showed a positive cross-sectional association between performance in language and mathematics as measured by Chile’s standardized System for the Assessment of Educational Quality test and the nutritional quality of school snacking, regardless of sex, SES, and other educational influences [17]. Similarly, in a subset (n = 395) of the current sample, Correa et al. [18] observed that, among students taking college entrance exams, unhealthy dietary habits of 16-year-old were associated with lower performance on college examination tests when compared to the performance of students with healthy dietary habits.
Cross-sectional studies conducted in adolescents from other countries also found that participants having healthy dietary habits performed better at school compared to those having unhealthy dietary habits. For instance, the native and foreign language attainment among 14- and 15-year-old Icelandic students, as well as their mathematics achievement, were negatively influenced by poor dietary habits [13,15]. Norwegian 9th and 10th graders with a high intake of sugar-sweetened soft drinks, candies, chocolate, chips, pizza, hot dogs, and hamburgers were up to 6 times more likely to manifest learning difficulties in mathematics. Conversely, a diet of fresh fruits at least once daily reduced the chances of difficulties in these areas [14]. Also in 15- to 17-year-old Norwegian adolescents, high academic achievement was associated with a high intake of fruits and berries, and a low intake of sugar-sweetened beverages [34]. Unfavorable academic performance, as measured by a standardized test, was positively associated with unhealthy dietary patterns in 6- to 13-year-old Taiwanese students. The likelihood of underperforming on the test was 1.63 times higher for students with greater consumption of low-quality foods (e.g., sweets and fried foods) than it was for students with low intake of such items. Fu et al. also showed that students with poor academic performance were less likely to regularly eat foods that are rich in protein, vitamins and minerals [35].
Diet is also an important influence on other determinants of academic success. Among adolescent students from Iceland, having an optimal diet was cross-sectionally associated with decreased odds of behavioral problems in the classroom [36]. Likewise, in 15- and 16-year-old male students from Oslo (Norway), intake of >4 glasses/day of sugar-sweetened soft drinks more than doubled the probability of having behavioral problems at school, compared to students drinking <1 glass of sugary drinks per day [37]. Among female students in Oslo with excessive intake of sugar-sweetened soft drinks, the chances of conduct problems at school were 4.1 times higher compared to the reference group. In males, soft drink consumption was also related to hyperactivity and higher levels of mental distress, both of which are associated with academic difficulties [38].
It is likely that the effect on academic results of excessive consumption of foods high in saturated fats and simple sugars is mediated by the effect of these macronutrients on brain health and cognitive function. In developmental stages such as adolescence, the brain is particularly vulnerable to the effects of excessive intake of saturated fats and simple carbohydrates [5,6]. Diet-induced impairment in learning and hippocampus-dependent memory processes have been widely documented [7,8,39,40]. In addition to reducing production of neurotrophins such as BDNF, other WD-induced effects have been reported on this brain structure, including overexpression of proinflammatory cytokines, mitochondrial damage due to oxidative stress, and altered blood–brain barrier permeability [7,8,10,39]. Also, insulin resistance and hyperleptinemia have been linked to impaired hippocampal synaptic plasticity and poor cognitive functioning [41,42,43]. Furthermore, evidence suggests that juvenile exposure to a WD may be more harmful than such exposure in adulthood. A 3-week juvenile WD regimen induced similar weight gain and metabolic alterations as did a 12-week adult WD regimen. Juvenile exposure, however, also affected memory consolidation and flexible memory expression while promoting exaggerated pro-inflammatory cytokine expression in the hippocampus after an immune challenge, and it diminished hippocampal neurogenesis [8,39].
While the cross-sectional design of our study prevents definitive conclusions about causality and the direction of the associations depicted here, it is worth noting that research conducted in both animals and humans described short-term effects of Western-type dietary habits on hippocampal-dependent learning and memory. In animals, it is well established that a WD causes rapid impairments of hippocampal-based tasks, with diet-related cognitive effects observed after only 72 h [44,45]. Studies in humans are limited but they confirm that a WD impacts hippocampal memory tasks following a relatively short exposure. Healthy 20-year-old college students from Australia consuming a HFS breakfast (30% saturated fats plus 18% refined sugars), over four consecutive days, showed significantly poorer memory recall compared to control students consuming a healthier breakfast of similar palatability and food types, but significantly lower in saturated fats and refined sugars (5% saturated fats plus 10% sugars). Since these changes in memory performance were linked to shifts in blood glucose across breakfast, authors suggest that this could be one potential mechanism by which a WD affects hippocampal function [46]. In a similar manner, in sedentary men aged 25–45 years, Edwards et al. found decreased power of attention and increased simple reaction time after seven days of consuming a diet comprising 74% kcal. from fat [47]. It is less clear for how long the cognitive effects of a WD will remain and, thus, further investigations should address that question. Although experimental studies in humans show that improvements in memory can occur following reductions in energy intake and fat [48], or shift to a diet low in saturated fats and refined sugars [49], observational longitudinal studies conducted in Anglo-Saxon countries suggest that unhealthy dietary practices in developmental periods have a lasting association with cognitive and educational outcomes that seem to persist over time, regardless of later changes in diet [16,50,51,52].
Our results also showed that a significant share (73%) of participants in the sample ate snacks of intermediate or poor nutritional value. This is consistent with population surveys conducted nationally and internationally. In Chile, adolescents (aged 14 years to 18 years) ranked first in the consumption of refined sugar (121 g/day) and second in the consumption of saturated fats (12.7 mL-g/day) compared to other age groups. In this age group, the consumption of sugar-sweetened soft drinks was 254 mL/day, according to the latest National Food Consumption Survey [53]. This survey also reported that 97% of children and adolescents aged 6 years to 18 years need to improve the quality of their diets. The World Health Organization’s Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) survey found that in Scotland, 35% of adolescents eat sweets or chocolate every day, and 18% eat chips every day. In addition, 20% of Scottish female adolescents and 27% of their male counterparts consume sugary soft drinks daily [54]. Among US high schoolers, 22% of males and 17% of females report consumption of sugar-sweetened soft drinks ≥2 times per day [55].

4.2. Implications for Practice

Our results are of interest for a number of reasons. Translating research knowledge to practice and policy is much needed in the field of health promotion [56,57]. The idea of testing the connection between diet and cognition using functional cognitive measures such as GPA, graduation rates, and rates of taking college entrance exams was aimed at bridging the gap between research and policymaking. Although evidence on the consequences of unhealthy diets on learning and cognition is growing, the failure to implement effective interventions persists. A more informed approach to this connection can influence healthcare practitioners, educators and parents.
In addition, lower academic results have been associated with several health-risk behaviors in youths. In US adolescent populations, over the past three decades, cross-sectional and longitudinal studies demonstrate links connecting poor academic performance with sedentary lifestyle, alcohol/tobacco abuse, sexually risky behaviors, and violence [58]. All of these risk behaviors have been regarded as important contributors to poor health status in adulthood and multiple social problems. Since the influence of academic performance on future health is known [59], the relationship of diet and academic results may be an important public health tool. It is also important to identify the nutrients and dietary patterns that most influence cognitive health and academic performance.
The fact that adolescents struggle to make healthy dietary choices is not new information. Youthful anomalous health decision-making has been attributed to an aversion to forced choices; the inclination to rely on taste, brands, and convenience as primary drivers of food decisions; and the tendency to discount the value of delayed rewards or penalties [60]. Also, sufficient nutrition knowledge does not necessarily correspond to responsible dietary behavior [61]. Thus, associating healthy dietary choices with school performance can perhaps enhance the value of healthy eating and boost motivation. After all, academic achievement, academic behavior, and academic performance are closely linked to expectations of better postsecondary opportunities and subsequent job status [62,63].
Our results that show an association between a healthy diet and improved cognitive and educational outcomes should be a matter of interest to support nutrition interventions designed for adolescents. To date, the majority of interventions that emphasize the relationship between diet type and cognition and academics have been designed for infants and young children [6,19,52], who are less independent in their food choices. For health promotion purposes, unhealthy dietary habits during adolescence are usually said to be related to early onset of cardiometabolic disorders, including high blood pressure, type-2 diabetes and coronary heart disease, while arguments based on the potential cognitive impact of diet are still lacking. We have seen that adolescents are also exposed to the detrimental cognitive effects of a diet high in saturated fats and refined sugars. Moreover, adolescence is a transitional period with subcortical regions associated with reward-seeking and emotion developing earlier than prefrontal control regions [64]. Greater emotional reactivity and sensitivity may in part explain unhealthy dietary habits among teenagers. Sociocultural changes, the need to fit in, food availability and the quest for independent decision-making also contribute to unhealthy food choices that are common during adolescence [65], making this period one of tremendous importance in terms of cognitive development.
A further implication of these findings is that they can potentially play a major role in health promotion by educational agencies and schools. Dietary habits that comport with food guidelines might help pave the way for students on the path to higher education. Chilean high school students perform far below the Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) average in mathematics, reading and science, with less than 2% of 15-year-old scoring in the group of top performers [63]. Evidence shows that students who fail to reach baseline levels of performance in these areas have difficulties with academic readiness, persistence and higher education completion [66]. Nonetheless, 80% of Chilean parents expect their children to obtain a college degree [63].

4.3. Limitations and Strengths

This research provides results that support a connection between nutritious dietary intake and higher academic achievement. Given that most studies have been conducted in the developed world, one strength of this study is that it provides evidence that may be useful for countries undergoing nutritional and epidemiological transitions. Second, the use of a translational research approach to explore the diet–learning–cognition connection and provide applicable results is a positive contribution. Further, to our best knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the association of nutrition and academic achievement on HS students’ postsecondary education intentions.
Despite these strengths, several limitations persist that should be considered when interpreting these results. Our sample is not representative of the Chilean adolescent population, as it consisted of adolescents from low and middle SES families. However, data from these socioeconomic groups may be especially important: population-based surveys conducted in Chile show that the prevalence of unhealthy dietary habits, physical inactivity, and excess weight is higher in adolescents from low and middle SES families compared to adolescents from high SES families [53,67]. This means that students from low and middle SES families are more exposed to risk factors for difficulties related to progressing from high school to higher education. Encouraging healthy dietary habits and, in particular, intake of healthy snacks, might smooth the pathway to college. Second, although we accounted for the effect of important confounders (including parental education and family structure), we were not able to consider other key influences, such as family support-related variables, general motivational factors (e.g., achievement motivation), and students’ interests in specific subject areas, which may also impact their academic functioning. A third limitation is the cross-sectional nature of the study. Since data on snack quality for each participant was recorded only once, it would be difficult to infer the temporal association between this exposure and the academic outcomes. Thus, only association, and not causation, can be inferred from our study. While our results may be useful to inform new hypotheses, a more complex investigation, such as a longitudinal study or crossover intervention trial, should be conducted to test the temporality of these associations, i.e., that the exposure to Western-type food items precede academic difficulties. Finally, future studies should replicate and extend this analysis in other young populations.

5. Conclusions

Poor nutritional quality snacking at school and at home was associated with poor secondary school academic achievement and lower intention to enroll in higher education. Both types of snacking showed similar associations with these educational outcomes. These results may have important implications for the promotion of healthy lifestyles by educational agencies and schools. Also, associating healthy snacking with educational outcomes can perhaps enhance the value of having responsible health behaviors and boost motivation for a healthy way of life.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge the ongoing commitment of participants and their families. We also thank all the people who contributed to the development of this project, especially, Professors Betsy Lozoff and Marcela Castillo. This study was funded by the National Heart, Blood, and Lung Institute, National Institutes of Health (USA) under grant R01HL088530-2980925, and the National Council for Scientific Research and Technology (CONICYT) (Chile) under grants PAI 79140003 and FONDECYT 1160240. They had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, and in the decision to publish the results.

Author Contributions

P.C.-B., R.B. and S.G. conceived and designed the study. R.B., P.C.-B. and Y.R. performed the field study. P.C.-B. and R.B. analyzed the data and wrote the paper. Y.R., E.B. and S.G. performed critical analysis of manuscript draft. E.B. and S.G. edited language. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Fraser, B. Latin American countries crack down on junk food. Lancet 2013, 382, 385–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Braithwaite, I.; Stewart, A.; Hancox, R.; Beasley, R.; Murphy, R.; Mitchell, E.A. Fast-food consumption and body mass index in children and adolescents: An international cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 2014, 4, e005813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Ministerio de Salud. Global School-Based Health Survey (Chile). Available online: http://www.who.int/chp/gshs/2013_Chile_GSHS_fact_sheet.pdf (accessed on 10 October 2016).
  4. Currie, C.; Zanotti, C.; Morgan, A.; Barnekow, V. Social Determinants of Health and Well-Being among Young People. Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children (HBSC) Study: International Report from the 2009/2010 Survey. WHO Regional Office for Europe: Copenhagen. Available online: http://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0003/163857/Social-determinants-of-health-and-well-being-among-young-people.pdf (accessed on 2 November 2016).
  5. Benton, D. The influence of children’s diet on their cognition and behavior. Eur. J. Nutr. 2008, 47, 25–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Nyaradi, A.; Li, J.; Hickling, S.; Foster, J.; Oddy, W. The role of nutrition in children’s neurocognitive development, from pregnancy through infancy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Boitard, C.; Cavaroc, A.; Sauvant, J.; Aubert, A.; Castanon, N.; Layé, S.; Ferreira, G. Impairment of hippocampal-dependent memory induced by juvenile high-fat diet intake is associated with enhanced hippocampal inflammation in rats. Brain Behav. Immun. 2014, 40, 9–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Valladolid-Acebes, I.; Fole, A.; Martín, M.; Morales, L.; Cano, M.; Ruiz-Gayo, M.; Del Olmo, N. Spatial memory impairment and changes in hippocampal morphology are triggered by high-fat diets in adolescent mice. Is there a role of leptin? Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2013, 106, 18–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Lynch, M. Long-term potentiation and memory. Physiol. Rev. 2004, 84, 87–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Kanoski, S.; Davidson, T. Western diet consumption and cognitive impairment: Links to hippocampal dysfunction and obesity. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 103, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Beilharz, J.; Maniam, J.; Morris, M. Diet-induced cognitive deficits: The role of fat and sugar, potential mechanisms and nutritional interventions. Nutrients 2015, 7, 6719–6738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Blankenship, T.; O’Neill, M.; Ross, A.; Bell, M. Working memory and recollection contribute to academic achievement. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2015, 43, 164–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Sigfúsdóttir, I.; Krisjánsson, A.; Allegrante, J. Health behaviours and academic achievement in Icelandic school children. Health Educ. Res. 2007, 22, 70–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Kristjánsson, A.; Sigfúsdóttir, I.; Allegrante, J. Health behavior and academic achievement among adolescents: The relative contribution of dietary habits, physical activity, body mass index, and self-esteem. Health Educ. Behav. 2010, 37, 51–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Øverby, N.; Lüdemann, E.; Høigaard, R. Self-reported learning difficulties and dietary intake in Norwegian adolescents. Scand. J. Public Health 2013, 41, 754–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Feinstein, L.; Sabates, R.; Sorhaindo, A.; Emmett, P. Dietary patterns related to attainment in school: The importance of early eating patterns. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 2008, 62, 734–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Correa-Burrows, P.; Burrows, R.; Orellana, Y.; Ivanovic, D. The relationship between unhealthy snacking at school and academic outcomes: A population study in Chilean schoolchildren. Public Health Nutr. 2015, 18, 2022–2030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Correa-Burrows, P.; Burrows, R.; Blanco, E.; Reyes, M.; Gahagan, S. Nutritional quality of diet and academic performance in Chilean students. Bull. World Health Org. 2016, 94, 185–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Burrows, T.; Goldman, S.; Pursey, K.; Lim, R. Is there an association between dietary intake and academic achievement? A systematic review. J. Hum. Nutr. Diet. 2016, 30, 117–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Lozoff, B.; Castillo, M.; Clark, K.; Smith, J.; Sturza, J. Iron supplementation in infancy contributes to more adaptive behavior at 10 years of age. J. Nutr. 2014, 144, 838–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Burrows, R.; Díaz, E.; Schiaraffia, V.; Gattas, V.; Montoya, A.; Lera, L. Dietary intake and physical activity in school age children. Rev. Med. Chile 2008, 136, 53–63. [Google Scholar]
  22. Gattas, V.; Burrows, R.; Burgueño, M. Validity Assessment of A Food Frequency Questionnaire in Chilean School-Age Children. In Proceedings of the XVI Congress of the Latin-American Society of Pediatric Research and the XXII Pan-American Meeting of Pediatrics, Santiago, Chile, 25–30 April 2007. [Google Scholar]
  23. Ministerio de Salud. Tablas Chilenas de Composición Química de los Alimentos; Ministerio de Salud: Santiago, Chile, 2010.
  24. Departamento de Evaluación, Medición y Registro Educacional (DEMRE). Prueba de Selección Universitaria. Informe Técnico. Volumen IV. Proceso de Admisión 2016. Unidad de Desarrollo y Análisis. Universidad de Chile. Available online: http://psu.demre.cl/estadisticas/documentos/informes/2016-vol-4-informe-tecnico-admision-2016.pdf (accessed on 19 September 2016).
  25. De Onis, M.; Onyango, A.; Borghi, E.; Siyam, A.; Nishida, C.; Siekmann, J. Development of a WHO growth reference for school-aged children and adolescents. Bull. World Health Org. 2007, 85, 660–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Correa-Burrows, P.; Burrows, R.; Orellana, Y.; Ivanovic, D. Achievement in mathematics and language is linked to regular physical activity: A population study in Chilean youth. J. Sports Sci. 2014, 32, 1631–1638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Burrows, R.; Correa-Burrows, P.; Orellana, Y.; Almagiá, A.; Lizana, P.; Ivanovic, D. Scheduled physical activity is associated with better academic performance in Chilean school-age children. J. Phys. Act. Health 2014, 11, 1600–1606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Godard, C.; Rodríguez, M.; Díaz, N.; Lera, L.; Salazar, G.; Burrows, R. Value of a clinical test for assessing physical activity in children. Rev. Med. Chile 2008, 136, 1155–1162. [Google Scholar]
  29. Dobow, E.; Boxer, P.; Huesmann, L. Long-term effects of parents’ education on children’s educational and occupational success: Mediation by family interactions, child aggression, and teenage aspirations. Merrill Palmer Q. 2009, 55, 224–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO). International Standard Classification of Education. ISCED 2011. Available online: http://www.uis.unesco.org/Education/Documents/isced-2011-en.pdf (accessed on 19 September 2016).
  31. Braveman, P.; Cubbin, C.; Egerter, S.; Chideya, S.; Marchi, K.S.; Metzler, M.; Posner, S. Socioeconomic status in health research: One size does not fit all. JAMA 2005, 294, 2879–2888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Ginther, D.; Pollak, R. Family structure and children’s educational outcomes: Blended families, stylized facts, and descriptive regressions. Demography 2004, 41, 671–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Hedges, L.; Hedberg, E. Intraclass correlation values for planning group-randomized trials in education. Educ. Eval. Policy Anal. 2007, 29, 60–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Stea, T.; Tortsveit, M. Association of lifestyle habits and academic achievement in Norwegian adolescents: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 2014, 14, 829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Fu, M.; Cheng, L.; Tu, S.; Pan, W. Association between unhealthful eating patterns and unfavorable overall school performance in children. J. Am. Diet Assoc. 2007, 107, 1935–1943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Øverby, N.; Høigaard, R. Diet and behavioral problems at school in Norwegian adolescents. Food Nutr. Res. 2012, 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Lien, L.; Lien, N.; Heyerdahl, S.; Thoresen, M.; Bjertness, E. Consumption of soft drinks and hyperactivity, mental distress, and conduct problems among adolescents in Oslo, Norway. Am. J. Public Health 2006, 96, 1815–1820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Polderman, T.; Boomsma, D.; Bartels, M.; Verhulst, F.; Huizink, A. A systematic review of prospective studies on attention problems and academic achievement. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2010, 122, 271–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Boitard, C.; Etchamendy, N.; Sauvant, J.; Ferreira, G. Juvenile but not adult exposure to high fat diet impairs relational memory and hippocampal neurogenesis in mice. Hippocampus 2012, 22, 2095–2100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Mellendijk, L.; Wiesmann, M.; Kiliaan, A. Impact of nutrition on cerebral circulation and cognition in the metabolic syndrome. Nutrients 2015, 7, 9416–9439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Stranahan, A.; Norman, E.; Lee, K.; Mattson, M. Diet-induced insulin resistance impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity and cognition in middle-aged rats. Hippocampus 2008, 18, 1085–1088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Irving, A.; Harvey, J. Leptin regulation of hippocampal synaptic function in health and disease. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2013, 369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Lee, S.; Zabolotny, J.; Huang, H.; Lee, H.; Kim, Y. Insulin in the nervous system and the mind: Functions in metabolism, memory, and mood. Mol. Metab. 2016, 5, 589–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Murray, A.; Knight, N.; Cochlin, L.; McAleese, S.; Deacon, R.; Rawlins, J.; Clarke, K. Deterioration of physical performance and cognitive function in rats with short-term high-fat feeding. FASEB J. 2009, 23, 4353–4360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Beilharz, J.E.; Maniam, J.; Morris, M.J. Short exposure to a diet rich in both fat and sugar or sugar alone impairs place, but not object recognition memory in rats. Brain Behav. Immun. 2014, 37, 134–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Edwards, L.; Murray, A.; Holloway, C.; Clarke, K. Short-term consumption of a high-fat diet impairs whole-body efficiency and cognitive function in sedentary men. FASEB J. 2011, 25, 1088–1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Attuquayefio, T.; Stevenson, R.; Oaten, M.; Francis, H. A four-day Western-style dietary intervention causes reductions in hippocampal-dependent learning and memory and interoceptive sensitivity. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0172645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Attuquayefio, T.; Stevenson, R. A systematic review of longer-term dietary interventions on human cognitive function: Emerging patterns and future directions. Appetite 2015, 95, 554–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Nilsson, A.; Tovar, J.; Johansson, M.; Radeborg, K.; Björck, I. A diet based on multiple functional concepts improves cognitive performance in healthy subjects. Nutr. Metab. 2013, 10, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Smithers, L.; Golley, R.; Mittinty, M.; Lynch, J. Do dietary trajectories between infancy and toddlerhood influence IQ in childhood and adolescence? Results from a prospective birth cohort study. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e58904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Theodore, R.; Thompson, J.; Waldie, K.; Mitchell, E. Dietary patterns and intelligence in early and middle childhood. Intelligence 2009, 37, 506–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Nyaradi, A.; Li, J.; Hickling, S.; Whitehouse, A.; Foster, J.; Oddy, W. Diet in the early years of life influences cognitive outcomes at 10 years: A prospective cohort study. Acta Paediatr. 2013, 102, 1165–1173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Ministerio de Salud. Encuesta Nacional de Consumo Alimentario. Informe Final de Resultados. Subsecretaría de Salud Pública; Ministerio de Salud: Santiago de Chile. Available online: web.minsal.cl/sites/default/files/ENCA-INFORME_FINAL.pdf (accessed on 27 January 2017).
  54. Currie, C.; Van der Sluijs, W.; Whitehead, R.; Currie, D.; Rhodes, G.; Neville, F.; Inchley, J. HBSC 2014 Survey in Scotland National Report. Child and Adolescent Health Research Unit (CAHRU). University of St Andrews: Fife. Available online: www.hbsc.org/news/index.aspx?ni=3272 (accessed on 27 January 2017).
  55. Kann, L.; Kinchen, S.; Shankil, S.; Zaza, S. Youth risk behavior surveillance. United Sates 2013. MMWR CDC Surveill. Summ. 2014, 63, 29–35. [Google Scholar]
  56. Spoth, R.; Rohrbach, L.; Greenberg, M.; Hawkins, J. Addressing core challenges for the next generation of type 2 translation research and systems: The translation science to population impact (TSci impact) framework. Prev. Sci. 2013, 14, 319–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Biglan, A. The ultimate goal of prevention and the larger context for translation. Prev. Sci. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Bradley, B.; Greene, A. Do health and education agencies in the United States share responsibility for academic achievement and health? A review of 25 years of evidence about the relationship of adolescents’ academic achievement and health behaviors. J. Adolesc. Health 2013, 52, 523–532. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  59. Baker, D.; Leon, J.; Smith-Greenaway, E.; Collins, J.; Movit, M. The education effect on population health: A reassessment. Popul. Dev. Rev. 2011, 37, 307–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Just, D.; Mancino, L.; Wansink, B. Could Behavioral Economics Help Improve Diet Quality for Nutrition Assistance Program Participants? US Department of Agriculture: Washington, DC, USA.
  61. Kersting, M.; Sichert-Hellert, W.; Vereecken, C.; Sette, S. Food and nutrient intake, nutritional knowledge and diet-related attitudes in European adolescents. Int. J. Obes. 2008, 32, 35–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. Health and Academic Achievement. Available online: http://www.cdc.gov/healthyschools/health_and_academics/pdf/health-academic-achievement.pdf (accessed on 2 November 2016).
  63. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development, Program for International Students Assessment. Programme for International Students Assessment 2012 Results in Focus. What 15-Year-Old Know and What They Can Do with What They Know. Available online: http://www.oecd.org/pisa/keyfindings/pisa-2012-results.htm (accessed on 7 December 2016).
  64. Casey, B.; Jones, R.; Hare, T. The adolescent brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1124, 111–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Pedersen, S.; Grønhøj, A.; Thøgersen, J. Following family or friends. Social norms in adolescent healthy eating. Appetite 2015, 86, 54–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Hakkarainen, A.; Holopainen, L.; Savolainen, H. Mathematical and reading difficulties as predictors of school achievement and transition to secondary education. Scand. J. Educ. Res. 2013, 57, 488–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Ministerio de Educación. Informe de resultados Estudio Nacional Educación Física 2013. Agencia de Calidad de la Educación. Available online: http://archivos.agenciaeducacion.cl/biblioteca_digital_historica/resultados/2013/result8b_edfisica_2013.pdf (accessed on 2 November 2016).
Figure 1. Proportion of students getting their high school diploma (outcome) by nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking (exposure) (n = 678). Error bars are 95% CI (upper limit). CI: confidence interval.
Figure 1. Proportion of students getting their high school diploma (outcome) by nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking (exposure) (n = 678). Error bars are 95% CI (upper limit). CI: confidence interval.
Nutrients 09 00433 g001
Figure 2. Proportion of participants taking the exams for college admission (outcome) by nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking (exposure) (n = 571). Only those students graduating from high school (n = 571) are allowed to take the exams for college admission. Error bars are 95% CI (upper limit).
Figure 2. Proportion of participants taking the exams for college admission (outcome) by nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking (exposure) (n = 571). Only those students graduating from high school (n = 571) are allowed to take the exams for college admission. Error bars are 95% CI (upper limit).
Nutrients 09 00433 g002
Figure 3. Mean high school grade point average (GPA) by nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking (n = 571). GPA expressed as standardized score, according to the Chilean Ministry of Education. a, significantly different from the group having healthy snacks at home or at school. p value estimated with Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni adjustment.
Figure 3. Mean high school grade point average (GPA) by nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking (n = 571). GPA expressed as standardized score, according to the Chilean Ministry of Education. a, significantly different from the group having healthy snacks at home or at school. p value estimated with Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni adjustment.
Nutrients 09 00433 g003
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the sample: adolescent students from Santiago, Chile (n = 678).
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the sample: adolescent students from Santiago, Chile (n = 678).
VariablesMean or nSD or Percentage
Chronological age
Age (years)16.80.3
In-home snacking
Healthy18026.55
Unhealthy-to-fair33749.71
Unhealthy16123.74
At-school snacking
Healthy18326.99
Unhealthy-to-fair30244.54
Unhealthy19328.47
Academic outcomes
Graduated high school57184.09
Took college admission exams * (n = 571)38767.76
High school GPA (score) (n = 571)481.192.3
Sex
Male35752.58
Anthropometrics
BMI (z-score)0.651.2
Weight status
Normal41761.42
Overweight16724.59
Obesity9514.99
Physical activity
Weekly scheduled PA ≤ 90 min40359.35
Parental education
Maternal education: incomplete secondary24035.40
Paternal education: incomplete secondary19228.32
Family structure
Fatherless family27440.4
Iron supplementation in infancy
No added Fe (6–12 months)28642.18
* Only those students graduating from high school (n = 571) are allowed to take the exams for college admission. BMI: Body-Mass Index. Normal weight: BMI-z from −1 SD to +1 SD. Overweight: BMI-z from >1 SD to 2 SD. Obesity: BMI-z ≥ 2 SD. GPA: grade point average; SD: standard deviation; PA: physical activity.
Table 2. Estimated cross-sectional association between achieving the high school diploma (outcomes) and nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking (exposure) in students from Santiago, Chile, after adjusting other influences (n = 678).
Table 2. Estimated cross-sectional association between achieving the high school diploma (outcomes) and nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking (exposure) in students from Santiago, Chile, after adjusting other influences (n = 678).
In-Home SnackingAt-School Snacking
OR95% CIaOR95% CIOR95% CIaOR95% CI
Unhealthy0.44 **0.25–0.820.47 *0.25–0.880.47 *0.26–0.830.49 *0.27–0.89
Unhealthy-to-fair0.670.39–1.160.700.39–1.240.650.37–1.130.670.37–1.20
Male (…)-0.42 ***0.27–0.67(…)-0.43 ***0.27–0.68
Overweight(…)-0.880.52–1.46(…)-0.890.53–1.48
Obesity(…)-0.810.44–1.49(…)-0.810.44–1.49
Physically inactive(…)-0.37 ***0.22–0.61(…)-0.37 ***0.22–0.63
Maternal education(…)-0.660.42–1.02(…)-0.660.42–1.02
Paternal education(…)-0.910.55–1.47(…)-0.910.56–1.48
Fatherless family(…)-0.770.51–1.20(…)-0.770.50–1.19
No added Fe(…)-0.890.57–1.37(…)-0.890.58–1.38
OR: Odds ratio. aOR: adjusted OR. (…) Non-observed variables. Overweight: BMI-z from >1 SD to <2 SD. Obesity: BMI-z ≥ 2 SD. Physically inactive: ≤90 min/week of scheduled exercise. Maternal and paternal education: incomplete high school. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Estimated cross-sectional association between taking the exams for higher education (outcome) and nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking (exposure) in students from Santiago, Chile, after adjusting other influences (n = 571).
Table 3. Estimated cross-sectional association between taking the exams for higher education (outcome) and nutritional quality of in-home and at-school snacking (exposure) in students from Santiago, Chile, after adjusting other influences (n = 571).
In-Home SnackingAt-School Snacking
OR95% CIaOR95% CIOR95% CIaOR95% CI
Unhealthy0.46 **0.29–0.710.53 *0.31–0.880.49 ***0.32–0.740.57 *0.35–0.90
Unhealthy-to-fair0.68 *0.47–0.980.750.48–1.150.710.49–1.040.810.51–1.27
Male(…)-0.66 *0.45–0.96(…)-0.66 *0.45–0.97
Overweight(…)-0.990.64–1.52(…)-0.990.65–1.55
Obesity(…)-0.970.56–1.66(…)-0.970.57–1.67
Physically inactive(…)-0.850.57–1.25(…)-0.840.57–1.24
Maternal education (…)-0.63 *0.42–0.92(…)-0.63 *0.42–0.92
Paternal education(…)-0.750.49–1.13(…)-0.760.50–1.15
Fatherless family(…)-0.68 *0.48–0.99(…)-0.68 *0.47–0.98
No added Fe(…)-0.840.59–1.21(…)-0.840.58–1.21
OR: Odds ratio. aOR: adjusted OR. (…) Non-observed variables. Overweight: BMI-z from >1 SD to <2 SD. Obesity: BMI-z ≥ 2 SD. Physically inactive: ≤90 min/week of scheduled exercise. Maternal and paternal education: incomplete high school. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Cross-sectional association between academic attainment in high school (outcome) and nutritional quality of in-home snacking and at-school (exposure) in students from Santiago, Chile, after adjusting other influences (n = 571).
Table 4. Cross-sectional association between academic attainment in high school (outcome) and nutritional quality of in-home snacking and at-school (exposure) in students from Santiago, Chile, after adjusting other influences (n = 571).
In-Home SnackingAt-School Snacking
Mean GPAMeanMeanMeanSD
Unhealthy (1)490.0473.2473.290.2
Unhealthy-to-fair (2)502.2486.8486.889.3
Healthy (3)530.1512.4512.493.6
Comparison of mean GPA §Mean diff.95% CId ǂMean diff.95% CId ǂ
(1) vs. (2)−12.2−32.7; 4.60.09−13.6−33.2; 2.40.15
(1) vs. (3)−40.1 ***−59.2; −16.90.41−39.2 ***−57.0; −17.10.44
(2) vs. (3)−27.9 ***−43.5; −8.20.30−25.6 *−40.6; −4.90.31
GPA: Grade point average (expressed in score according to the Ministry of Education). § ANCOVA: * p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001. Models were adjusted for sex, weight status, familial background and iron supplementation in infancy. ǂ Cohen’s d coefficients account for the effect of different sample sizes. ESs around 0.20 are of policy interest when they are based on measures of academic achievement [33]. ANCOVA: analysis of covariance.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Correa-Burrows, P.; Rodríguez, Y.; Blanco, E.; Gahagan, S.; Burrows, R. Snacking Quality Is Associated with Secondary School Academic Achievement and the Intention to Enroll in Higher Education: A Cross-Sectional Study in Adolescents from Santiago, Chile. Nutrients 2017, 9, 433. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9050433

AMA Style

Correa-Burrows P, Rodríguez Y, Blanco E, Gahagan S, Burrows R. Snacking Quality Is Associated with Secondary School Academic Achievement and the Intention to Enroll in Higher Education: A Cross-Sectional Study in Adolescents from Santiago, Chile. Nutrients. 2017; 9(5):433. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9050433

Chicago/Turabian Style

Correa-Burrows, Paulina, Yanina Rodríguez, Estela Blanco, Sheila Gahagan, and Raquel Burrows. 2017. "Snacking Quality Is Associated with Secondary School Academic Achievement and the Intention to Enroll in Higher Education: A Cross-Sectional Study in Adolescents from Santiago, Chile" Nutrients 9, no. 5: 433. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9050433

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop